NCM 22nd Annual Meeting HILTON MOLINO STUCKY | VENICE, ITALY NCM Posters Authors, Titles, Affiliations & Abstracts Sessions 1 & 2 Satellite Meeting April 22 – 23, 2012 22nd Annual Meeting April 23 – 28, 2012 Society for the Neural Control of Movement | www.ncm-society.org 8:00 8:15 8:30 8:45 9:00 9:15 9:30 9:45 10:00 10:15 10:30 10:45 11:00 11:15 11:30 11:45 12:00 12:15 12:30 12:45 13:00 13:15 13:30 13:45 14:00 14:15 14:30 14:45 15:00 15:15 15:30 15:45 16:00 16:15 16:30 16:45 17:00 17:15 17:30 17:45 18:00 18:15 18:30 18:45 19:00 19:15 19:30 19:45 20:00 20:15 20:30 20:45 21:00 21:15 21:30 21:45 22:00 22:15 22:30 (19:00 - 21:00) Professor Jose Maria Delgado Garcia in honor of Satellite Dinner (17:00 - 19:00) Satellite Registration Arrivals, Free Time, Excursions 22-Apr Registration /Information Desk Open (19:00 - 21:00) Opening Dinner (17:00 - 19:00) Conference Registration (8:30 - 17:30) Satellite Meeting Satellite Registration 23-Apr Monday Free Time and/or Excursions (15:00 - 17:15) Session 3 Panel AZIM (13:00 - 15:00) Poster Session 1a and Lunch (10:45 - 13:00) Session 2 Panel SABES (10:15 - 10:45) Break (8:00 - 10:15) Session 1 Panel SCHIEBER 24-Apr (16:00 - 17:20) Session 7 Individual Presentations (14:30 - 16:00) Session 6 Panel GRIBBLE (12:45 - 14:30) Poster Session 1b and Lunch (10:45 - 12:45) Session 5 Individual Presentations (10:15 - 10:45) Break (8:00 - 10:15) Session 4 Panel FLASH 25-Apr Free Time and/or Excursions Exhibits on Display Wednesday Exhibits on Display Registration /Information Desk Open (19:45 - 21:15) *NCM Special Excursion* Private Evening of St. Mark’s Church FreeTime and/or Excursions (12:05 - 13:00) Business Meeting (10:45 - 12:05) Session 9 Individual Presentations (10:15 - 10:45) Break (8:00 - 10:15) Session 8 Panel POZZO 26-Apr Thursday Exhibits on Display Free Time and/or Excursions (15:00 - 17:00) Session 12 Individual Presentations (12:45 - 15:00) Poster Session 2a and Lunch (10:45 - 12:45) Session 11 Individual Presentations (10:15 - 10:45) Break (8:00 - 10:15) Session 10 Panel SUMINSKI 27-Apr Friday 29-Apr 28-Apr Free Time and/or Excursions (14:00 - 15:00) Session 15 BIZZI (12:15 - 14:00) Poster Session 2b and Lunch (10:45 - 12:15) Session 14 Panel BURDET (10:15 - 10:45) Break (8:00 - 10:15) Session 13 Panel KRAKAUER Sunday Saturday Exhibits on Display Tuesday Exhibits on Display Sunday Departures, Free Time, Excursions Time 2012 At-A-Glance Satellite and Annual Conference Schedule Hilton Molino Stucky, Venice, Italy Posters on Display (Session 2) Registration /Information Desk Open Posters on Display (Session 2) Registration /Information Desk Open Posters on Display (Session 1) Registration /Information Desk Open Posters on Display (Session 1) Registration /Information Desk Open Poster Sessions Full Abstracts SESSION 1 A - Adaptation & Plasticity in Motor Control 1-A-1 Paradoxical arm stiffness: Humans actively reduce endpoint arm stiffness in force tasks Bram Onneweer1, Erwin de Vlugt1, Alfred C Schouten1, Winfred Mugge1, Carel G.M. Meskers2, Frans C. T. van der Helm1 1 TU Delft, 2Leiden University Medical Center For humans to efficiently interact with their surroundings, the central nervous system (CNS) manipulates the stiffness of the neuromuscular system. Joint stiffness consists of an intrinsic and reflexive component. Intrinsic stiffness is the stiffness of a contracted muscle that can be increased with level of muscle activation. Reflexive stiffness originates from reflexive muscle activations as a result of the imposed perturbations that are sensed and fed back to the CNS by the muscle afferents like muscle spindles and Golgi tendon organs. A recent study on a single joint found that humans, when asked to give way to external forces, actively reduce joint stiffness to values below those found in the relaxed state. Here we assess if humans are capable of tuning the stiffness over multiple joints and at endpoint space during 2D (planar) hand tasks. Subjects (n=9, all male, mean age 34,7) held the handle of a planar manipulandum, in the horizontal plane in front of their shoulder with the elbow in 90 degrees, which applied unpredictable continuous isotropic forces to their hand. The subjects performed three tasks with their dominant arm, 1) force task (FT) where 'hand reaction forces need to be minimized'; 2) position task (PT), where 'hand displacement need to be minimized' and 3) relax task (RT) where subjects were instructed to 'do nothing' and which was used as a reference condition. Endpoint stiffness was calculated from the recorded movements and interaction forces at the handle. Joint stiffness of the shoulder and elbow, as well as the stiffness contribution of the bi-articular muscles, were derived from the endpoint stiffness using the Jacobian of the kinematic relations between joint angles and endpoint coordinates. As expected, both joint and endpoint stiffness was magnitudes larger in PT than in RT and FT. Joint and endpoint stiffness was smaller in FT than RT, although not as pronounced as in the single joint case. With the change in stiffness, the shape and orientation of the stiffness ellipse changed, indicative of a change in the contribution of the muscle groups. Due to the different stiffness directions of the muscle groups, the shape of the ellipse widens if more, and narrows if less muscle groups contribute to the endpoint stiffness. The orientation of ellipse is directed to the shoulder for PT and rotates clockwise for RT and more for FT. In addition to orientation, the shape of the ellipse narrows for RT and more for FT. The reduction of stiffness was more pronounced for the single joint shoulder and elbow muscles compared with the bi-articular shoulder-elbow muscles. Muscle activation levels (monitored with EMG) were higher for FT than RT, indicating that humans actively reduce joint stiffness. In conclusion, during a force task humans actively reduce endpoint arm stiffness to levels below the relaxed state, suggesting the usage of continuous inhibitory feedback mechanisms. 1-A-2 Internal model reference frames and generalizability Max Berniker1, Konrad Kording1 1 Northwestern University How the central nervous system represents and stores the information necessary to control our bodies is one of the central questions in movement science. This information is usually assumed to reside in an internal model, a representation of how movements are generated. However, fundamental questions such as which variables they encode, and how they generalize to new circumstances, for example, are still unclear. Depending on the kind of motor behavior subjects adapt to (e.g. force field, inertial perturbation, visuomotor rotation) and the type of generalization examined (e.g. interlimb or intralimb) different groups find different results. In one of the earliest studies of this kind (Shadmehr, '94) it was found that subjects were able to successfully adapt to a force field, and appeared to use intrinsic (i.e. joint-based) variables to generalize that knowledge to a new area of their workspace. However, subsequent studies have found evidence for both intrinsic and extrinsic (i.e. Cartesian-based) variables in internal models. Therefore, we reproduced this early study in an effort to carefully examine how subjects adapt and generalize new motor behaviors. As in the original study, we had subjects adapt to either a force field based on the velocity of their hand (extrinsic) or the velocity of their limb's joints (intrinsic). During adaptation, subjects made 1,000 reaches in the force field to randomly generated targets, forming a pseudo-random walk. Subjects were then examined for their performance when generalizing in a new area of the workspace. In this generalization phase, subjects were randomly exposed to either the intrinsic, extrinsic or null fields. Thus if the subjects made straight reaches in the intrinsic/extrinsic field, it would imply the internal model acquired during adaptation used intrinsic/extrinsic coordinates to adapt to the force field. Surprisingly our results were distinct from what was originally reported. We found subjects did not adapt to the level observed in the previous study and continued to display "hooked" reaches even after 1,000 trials (indeed, a second group of subjects that adapted for 1,500 trials continued to display hooked reaches). Perhaps more importantly, when the subjects were examined for their ability to generalize in a new area of the workspace, their performance was equally poor in both the intrinsic and extrinsic fields. Our findings suggest that the ability to adapt is more pronounced when the range of movements is restricted. Similarly, we find that subject's ability to generalize is very modest, suggesting an internal model may be formed locally and not capable of global generalization. On the whole, the many contrasting findings on adaptation and generalization suggest our basic understanding of the variables internal models represent, and how they extrapolate to new circumstances, needs further clarification and examination before a clear understanding of internal models can be found. 1-A-3 Properties of a grasping tool affect grasping behavior Raoul Bongers1, Leonoor J Mouton1, Frank Zaal1 1 University of Groningen The use of objects as tools is a hallmark in evolution of which we only start to understand the basic underlying processes. The current study examines changes in the grasping pattern of healthy participants when picking up objects with pairs of pliers that differed in length and that differed in the relation between digit movement and movement of the beak of the pair of pliers. The goal of this study was to reveal how the kinematics of the grasp profile change as a function of properties of the manipulated 1 Poster Sessions Full Abstracts relation between beak and digit movement and to link these changes to neural mechanisms of prehension and tool use. In the first experiment participants picked up a cylindrical object with a pincer-like pair of pliers, with the beak at 3 cm, 8 cm or 16 cm from the fingers. Our findings showed that contrary to natural finger grasping, the grasping pattern with the pair of pliers showed a clear plateau phase. Moreover, the duration of this plateau phase was independent of the length of the pair of pliers. Finally, the maximum opening of the beak of the pliers was larger for longer tools. A second experiment was designed to investigate if the plateau phase would disappear when participants use the pair of pliers repetitively for huge number of trials. The analysis of pilot data suggested that this is not the case. In the third experiment, different participants used a pincer-like pair of pliers, a pair of pliers in which the digits were crossed with the beak, and a reverse pair of pliers, all of a length of 20 cm or 40 cm. Results showed that also with this set of pair of pliers the grasp profile showed a plateau phase. Importantly, the plateau phase was shortest for pincer pliers and longest for reverse pliers. Also, the duration of the plateau phase was longer for longer tools. Furthermore, hand opening was larger with pincer pair of pliers, which follows from the functioning of that particular pair of pliers. Importantly, participants showed that they were able to use each pair of pliers immediately in an appropriate way, independent of the complexity of the required transformations. Moreover, participants adapted the hand opening to the transformations between digit movement and movement of the beak of the pair of pliers. However, pliers grasping was characterized by a plateau phase in the grasping profile, something that is not often reported for fingers grasping. Interestingly, the plateau phase increased as a function of complexity of transformations between digits and beak. The discussion will focus on the possible origins of this change in the plateau phase as function of properties of tools. It will be discussed how interactions between neural sites active during tool use and during prehension predict the emergence of a plateau phase in using a pair of pliers. In particular, the focus will be on parietal networks coding for grasping of tools vs grasping of non-tools, and those coding for objects within reach by hand and tool, and those networks in dorsal premotor cortex that are hypothesized to be involved in coordinating the reach and the grasp. 1-A-4 Oral and pharyngeal sensory processing have differential control over hyo-laryngeal kinematics Ianessa Humbert1, Akshay Lokhande1, Heather Christopherson1, Rebecca German1, Alice Stone1 1 Johns Hopkins University Before a bolus is pushed into the pharynx, oral sensory processing is critical for planning movements of the subsequent pharyngeal swallow, including hyoid bone and laryngeal (hyolaryngeal) kinematics. However, oral and pharyngeal sensory processing for hyo-laryngeal kinematics is not fully understood. In 11 healthy adults, we examined changes in kinematics with sensory adaptation, sensitivity shifting, with oropharyngeal swallows versus pharyngeal swallows (no oral processing), and with various bolus volumes and tastes. Only pharyngeal swallows showed sensory adaptation (gradual changes in kinematics with repeated exposure to the same bolus) and sensitivity shifting (changing sensory threshold for a small bolus when it immediately follows several very large boluses). Conversely, only oropharyngeal swallows distinguished volume differences, while 2 pharyngeal swallows did not. No taste effects were observed for either swallow type. The hyo-laryngeal kinematics were very similar between oropharyngeal swallows and pharyngeal swallows with a comparable bolus. These findings indicate that once oral sensory processing has set a motor program for a specific kind of bolus (i.e. 5ml water), hyo-laryngeal movements are already highly standardized and optimized, showing no shifting or adaptation regardless of repeated exposure (sensory adaptation) or previous sensory experiences (sensitivity shifting). Also, the oral cavity is highly specialized for differentiating certain properties of a bolus (volume) that might require a specific motor plan to ensure swallowing safety, while the pharyngeal cavity does not make the same distinctions. Pharyngeal sensory processing might not be able to adjust motor plans created by the oral cavity once the swallow has already been triggered. 1-A-5 Interference between geometric cues and sensorimotor memories for anticipatory control of manipulation Qiushi Fu1, Marco Santello1 1 Arizona State University Numerous studies using force fields and visuomotor rotations have shown that sensorimotor memory of one learned action can exert an anterograde interference on the subsequent learning of an opposite action. However, few studies have investigated whether an anterograde effect occurs also when visual cues about object properties are congruent with the action to be performed. For object manipulation, visual geometric cues are normally used to anticipate the properties of the object and the dynamics of the upcoming manipulation. Our recent study showed that sensorimotor memory built through manipulation with Ushaped object, requiring a torque in a given direction, interferes with the ability of using congruent geometric cues when learning to manipulate the same object requiring a torque in the opposite direction. This result led us to ask whether the interference is object-dependent or actiondependent. In the former case, an anterograde interference might be found only within manipulations on one object, whereas the latter case would occur every time that sensorimotor memories conflict with geometric cues and would therefore point to fundamental limitations in motor planning. To address this question, we asked subjects (n = 16) to reach and lift one of the two L-shaped objects while minimizing the roll by grasping the vertical handle with three digits. The two objects have the same visual and mechanical properties except the vertical handle being either on the right (R) or left (L) side. The shape of the object and the task require that, when grasping the L or R object, subjects generate a counterclockwise or clockwise compensatory torque (320 Nmm), respectively. Each subject performed four blocks of eight trials. Within each block, subjects grasped the same object and were instructed to switch to the other object after the last trial of each block. We evaluated subjects' anticipatory control of manipulation by measuring compensatory torque produced at object lift onset. The geometric cues were effective in eliciting an anticipatory compensatory torque in the very first trial (174.8±14.7 Nmm) and quickly improved to a level close to 320 Nmm in subsequent trials. However, in the first trial of the second block after switching to the other object, the compensatory torque was poorly scaled (83.9±16.7 Nmm) indicating anterograde interference. Subsequently, subjects were able to re-adapt to the second object to the same performance level attained at the end of the first Poster Sessions Full Abstracts block. The third and fourth block also showed similar interference and re-adaptation, but the magnitude of interference decreased as the total number of trials increased (165.5±15.1 Nmm and 205.9±12.1 Nmm of compensatory torques for the beginning of third and fourth blocks, respectively). These results confirm earlier observations that subjects are able to use object geometric cues for anticipatory control of manipulation. Most importantly, however, the present findings significantly extend previous work by revealing that sensorimotor memory of previous manipulations strongly interfere with visually-based estimation of object properties for planning manipulations even when manipulating different objects. 1-A-6 Patterns of hand muscle activation in pianists: From amateur to expert Sara Winges1, Shinichi Furuya1, Martha Flanders1 1 University of Minnesota The effect of training can be observed in the quality of the performance of a task or movement but it may also be discerned at the cortical and neuromuscular levels. For piano playing, the effect of training on the performance of skilled finger movements can be demonstrated in the quality of play and the economy of movements and improved accuracy. Highly trained pianists are also more capable of restricting movement and force production at non-striking digits when compared to amateur pianists. At the cortical level, the effect of training is associated with enhanced motor cortical excitability and plasticity and can be observed specifically as changes in patterns of cortical activation for specific finger movements. Presumably with training these cortical changes would allow the pianist to perform highly practiced finger movements more precisely with less effort. However, at the neuromuscular level, the muscle activation patterns that accompany changes in performance with respect to training are not known. (FPB), as well as four intrinsic finger muscles: first dorsal interosseus (FDI), middle and ring lumbricals (LUMm, LUMr), and abductor digiti minimi (ADM). The extent of phasic coactivation across these hand muscles was assessed for each musical piece and compared across subjects and training levels. 1-A-7 Neural correlates of sensory plasticity following motor learning Sazzad Nasir1, Mohammad Darainy2, David J Ostry2 1 Northwestern University, 2McGill University Though it is reasonable to assume that the neuroplasticity associated with motor learning might affect sensory as well as motor systems, there is limited neurophysiological evidence supporting this idea. We show in the present study that motor learning indeed alters sensory areas of the brain. We adopted a force-field adaptation paradigm in which, as subjects reached out to a visual target, a robot pushed the hand laterally in proportion to its velocity in the direction of the target. We used somatosensory evoked potentials (SEPs), in response to brief force-pulses elicited by a robotic device, as a probe to assess sensory plasticity. The brain's response to these perturbations was measured before and after learning. Our experiments show that motor learning produces short latency changes to somatosensory areas of the brain and in particular, to second somatosensory cortex, an area of the brain presumably involved in sensory aspects of tactile learning. The changes we observe are substantially linked to motor learning. They were not obtained when subjects passively traverse the same movement trajectories and experience the same spatial distribution of movements, but do not experience learning. Moreover, the magnitude of the response is correlated with the amount of learning; the more subjects learn the greater the change in the sensory evoked potential. These results are consistent with the idea that the effects of motor learning extend into somatosensory areas of the brain. The purpose of this study was to characterize the temporal multimuscle activation patterns of pianists with different levels of training during performance. The goal of the experiment was to determine how training changes patterns of muscle activation during skilled finger movement sequences and thus demonstrate the plasticity within the patterns of neuromuscular control. Therefore, we tested the specific hypothesis that asynchronous phasic EMG bursts to limit movement at the non-striking digits would characterize higher levels of training, while tonic coactivation of hand muscles would characterize lower levels of training. 1-A-8 Sensory preference in speech motor learning revealed by simultaneous alteration of auditory and somatosensory feedback Ten subjects (4 Male; 35±10 yrs) were included in the study. The training level for each subject was determined by the number of years playing (range: 7-48 yrs). Other training related information was also recorded such as age at start of training, days per week and hours per day spent practicing. Each subject performed 14 different musical scores that required the use of the right hand. Excerpts were chosen to be challenging but also easy enough that subjects with all levels of training included in the study could play them. Subjects were allowed to familiarize themselves with the piano prior to the start of data collection. Prior to data recording for each new score, subjects were allowed to practice the musical score in order to play consistently and accurately. Each of the 14 scores was played for 10 successful trials at a normal tempo provided by a metronome. Trials with miss-strikes were repeated. Rest periods were given between each score to prevent fatigue. Surface EMG was recorded from three portions of extrinsic finger muscles: a central portion of extensor digitorum (ED) and two portions of flexor digitorum superficialis, one closer to the middle finger (FDS) and the other closer to the ring finger (FDS2). EMG was also recorded from two intrinsic thumb muscles: abductor pollicis brevis (APB) and flexor pollicis brevis The idea that the nervous system learns and maintains accurate speech by carefully monitoring auditory feedback is widely held. But this view neglects the fact that auditory feedback is highly correlated with somatosensory feedback during speech production. Somatosensory feedback from speech movements could be a primary means by which cortical speech areas monitor the accuracy of produced speech. We tested this idea by placing the somatosensory and auditory systems in competition during speech production. To do this, we combined two speech motor learning paradigms to simultaneously alter somatosensory and auditory feedback in real-time as subjects spoke. A robotic device was used to perturb the motion path of the jaw, altering somatosensory feedback during speech; an acoustical effects processor was used to change the frequency of vowel sounds, altering the sound of the voice so that subjects heard something different from what they produced. The amount of compensation for each perturbation was used as a measure of sensory reliance. In a large sample of subjects, we found that everyone corrected for at least one of the two perturbations. By applying the perturbation alone and then in combinations we Sazzad Nasir1, Daniel R Lametti2, David J Ostry2 1 Northwestern University, 2McGill University 3 Poster Sessions Full Abstracts found that subjects have a trait-like preference for either somatosensory or auditory feedback during speech. These results have two surprising and important implications for our understanding of how the brain produces accurate speech. The first is that auditory feedback does not dominate speech production. The second is that, in contrast to studies of sensorimotor adaptation in limb movements, where all subjects are observed to integrate sensory feedback in the same way, in speech production the weighting of sensory feedback appears to differ on an individual basis. 1-A-9 Task-specific effect of Transcranial Direct Current Stimulation on motor learning Cinthia Saucedo1, Xue Zhang1 1 K.U. Leuven Anodal transcranial Direct Current Stimulation (tDCS) applied to the human primary motor cortex (M1) proves to have beneficial effects on motor skill learning in both: healthy controls [1-2] and patients [3-4]. However, it remains unclear whether tDCS improves motor learning in a general manner or whether there is a task-specific effect. In this study, we tested the effect of tDCS in two different motor tasks: (1) explicit sequence learning and (2) visually guided force control task. Our hypothesis was that anodal tDCS would lead to greater motor learning in both tasks, with these improvements being task-dependent. Thirty-two healthy subjects participated in this double-blind, sham-controlled crossover designed study. All subjects were randomly assigned to an anodal-tDCS group or sham-group. We applied tDCS over the primary motor cortex (M1) while subjects performed the motor task. Two different sessions (session interval>1 month) were performed, with the task-order randomized across participants. Motor training of each task consisted of 20 min training for 3 continuous days. Retention tests were performed on the final training day and one week after the training. Learning scores were calculated and compared using a mixed model ANOVA analysis. The results showed that for both tDCS groups there was an overall improvement of scores across training (**p<0.001). Anodal tDCS showed more improvement compared to sham, in both motor tasks, but not to a significant level. In the explicit sequence task, a significant interaction between the TIME of stimulation (pre, training, post, RT) and TYPE of stimulation (anodal/sham) was found (*p=0.01), with the greatest improvement by anodal tDCS being at the 20 min retention test (*p=0.01). On the other hand, the visually guided control motor task showed the greatest improvement in the 1 week retention test (*p=0.03). This findings suggest that anodal tDCS does lead to an increase in motor learning most likely by improving consolidation. Further, the exact expression of this effect seems to be dependent on the motor task itself. 1-A-10 Enhanced locomotor adaptation after-effect in the 'broken escalator' phenomenon using anodal tDCS Diego Kaski1, Shamim Quadir1, Nada Yousif1, Adolfo M Bonstein1 1 Imperial College London The everyday experience of stepping onto a stationary escalator causes a stumble, despite our full awareness that the escalator is broken. In the laboratory, this 'broken escalator' phenomenon is reproduced when subjects step onto an obviously stationary platform (AFTER trials) that was previously experienced as moving (MOVING trials), and attests to a process of motor adaptation. Given the critical role of M1 in upper limb motor adaptation, and the potential for transcranial direct current stimulation (tDCS) to increase cortical excitability, we hypothesised that anodal tDCS over leg M1 and premotor cortices would increase the size and duration of the locomotor after-effect. Thirty healthy volunteers received either sham or real 4 tDCS (anodal bihemispheric tDCS; 2mA for 15 minutes at rest) to induce excitatory effects over the primary motor and premotor cortex, prior to walking onto the moving platform. The real tDCS group - compared to sham - displayed larger trunk sway, and increased gait velocity in the 1st AFTER trial and a persistence of the trunk sway after-effect into the 2nd AFTER trial. We also used transcranial magnetic stimulation to probe changes in cortical leg excitability using different electrode montages and eye blink conditioning, before and after tDCS, as well as simulating the current flow of tDCS on the human brain using a computational model of these different tDCS montages. Our data show that anodal tDCS induces excitability changes in lower limb motor cortex, with resultant enhancement of locomotor adaptation after-effects. These findings might encourage the use of tDCS over leg motor and premotor regions to improve locomotor control in patients with neurological gait disorders. 1-A-11 Enhancing voluntary control of neural oscillatory activity driving a brain-machine interface (BMI) Surjo Soekadar1, Matthias Witkowski1, Niels P Birbaumer1, Leonardo G Cohen2 1 University of Tübingen, 2NINDS / National Institutes of Health Introduction: Movement of a hand prosthesis after hand paralysis following stroke can be established using brainmachine interfaces (BMI) that translate electric or metabolic brain activity into movements of robotic or prosthetic devices. Buch et al. (2008 and 2012) have shown that severely affected chronic stroke patients are able to use murhythms, a form of oscillatory brain activity recordable over motor areas with a frequency of 7-13Hz, to open and close a mechanical hand orthosis. However, learning to control a BMI based on modulation of oscillatory brain activity requires training and is characterized by high variance of trial-to-trial performance. Previous work demonstrated that transcranial application of direct currents (tDCS) can improve motor performance and motor learning (Hummel et al. 2006; Reis et al. 2009). Thus, we tested the hypothesis that application of weak electric currents preceding BMI training will facilitate learning to control neural oscillatory activity driving a brain-machine interface, and long-term retention of this skill. Methods: Thirty healthy participants (n=30) were randomly assigned to one of three groups. They all trained over five consecutive daily sessions imagining hand-opening motions under EMG monitoring to modulate their mu-rhythm. Immediately before training, depending on group assignment, either anodal, cathodal or sham tDCS was applied. During training, successful mu-rhythm modulation was reflected in proportional online feedback delivered through an orthotic device that passively opened the subject's hand, mimicking the imagined training task. Changes in successful mu-rhythm modulation were analyzed across the training days and 30 days later to assess long-term retention. Results: Training in the anodal tDCS group improved murhythm control compared to the sham and the cathodal tDCS groups. Cathodal tDCS cancelled the beneficial effects of training alone evidenced in the sham group. One month later, this skill remained significantly superior in the anodal relative to both the sham and cathodal tDCS groups (p<.05 and p<.01, respectively). Poster Sessions Full Abstracts Conclusion: Our results show that application of painless noninvasive weak anodal tDCS over a relevant primary cortical region can improve learning voluntary control of neural oscillatory activity. 1-A-12 How action shapes space and body representations Michela Bassolino1, Alessandra Finisguerra1, Andrea Serino2, Thierry Pozzo1 1 Istituto Italiano di Tecnologia, 2CsrNC, Centro studi e ricerche in Neuroscienze Cognitive In order to interact with the external world, our brain relies on multisensory representations of the body in space, related both to the position and dimension of the body parts (the body schema, BS) and to the location of external objects in the space surrounding the body (the peripersonal space, PPS). Both the PPS and the BS are dynamically shaped by actions (e.g. Làdavas & Serino, 2008): interacting with far objects by means of a tool extends the boundaries of the PPS and affects the BS. In order to better characterize the relation between action, PPS and BS, here we investigate if the absence of motion may alter these representations. We compare PPS and BS before and after 10 hours of right arm immobilization in healthy subjects. Modifications of PPS are tested by means of an audio-tactile integration task: we consider the boundaries of the PPS as the critical distance at which an auditory stimulus, approaching or receding to the body affects the processing of a tactile stimulus on the arm (Serino et al., 2007; Bassolino et al., 2010). If action is necessary to shape space representation, the boundaries of the PPS should shift closer to the body after immobilization, suggesting a contraction of the PPS around the arm. Moreover, modifications of the BS are tested by means of a tactile distance perception task: we consider the perceived distance between two tactile stimuli on the forearm as an implicit measure of the perceived arm length (Canzoneri et al, under revision). Since after tool use subjects perceived their forearm as elongated, after immobilization we expect a shrinkage of the arm representation in the BS. Further, the same measurements are performed on the left, unrestricted, arm in order to evaluate if the compensatory overuse of the free limb during non-use (Avanzino et al. 2011), can modify space and body representation, probably in the opposite direction as compared to the effect of immobilization. Our results are discussed in terms of plasticity and mechanisms underlying PPS and BS representations. 1-A-13 Adaptation of surround inhibition in the human motor system Panagiotis Kassavetis1, Tabish A Saifee1, Anna Sadnicka1, Isabel Pareés1, Maja Kojovic1, John C Rothwell1, Mark J Edwards1 1 University College London, Institute of Neurology Motor surround inhibition (SI) is proposed as a mechanism to enhance motor performance by actively inhibiting the excitability of the corticospinal pathways which control surround muscles that potentially interfere with desired movements. Motor performance can be modified through a feed-forward motor adaptation process when a mismatch of the motor command and the sensory feedback occurs. Motor adaptation has been extensively studied with behavioural paradigms but electrophysiological evidence is lacking especially from the scope of surround inhibition. We probed adaptation of SI by introducing a false sensory feedback from a surround muscle during a training session when participants were requested to repetitively perform a brief index finger flexion movement while stimulation of the muscle spindles with vibration was applied to a surround muscle either at the onset of movement or with a delay of 100ms. We assessed motor SI before and after the training session with transcranial magnetic stimulation (TMS). Statistical analysis of the results showed that SI measured after the training session is increased compared to baseline when vibration was timed at the onset of the movement but not when vibration was delayed. The effect persisted for a while after withdrawal of vibration and slowly returned to baseline. The present study demonstrates that SI can be modified by experience. The timing of the sensory stimulation was found to be critical for the modification of SI, suggesting that only sensory signals closely related to the movement onset can induce adaptive changes, presumably through a feed-forward process. 1-A-14 Transitions between finger keypresses during acquisition of novel piano sequences are related to patterns of low frequency local field potential activity in the human globus pallidus internus Maria Herrojo Ruiz1, Christof Brücke1, Gerd-Helge Schneider1, Andrea A Kühn1 1 Charité - University of Medicine The regulation of temporal and spatial order in sequence production engages cortical and subcortical structures to a different degree depending on the stage of skill acquisition. The current study explores further the role of the basal ganglia in organizing and enacting novel action sequences during learning. To this aim, we investigated the temporal patterns of local field potential (LFP) activity in the human internal globus pallidus (GPi) in nine patients undergoing bilateral DBS for idiopathic cervical or segmental dystonia (hands not affected) during the performance of novel piano sequences. The GPi is the main output station of the BG and sends inhibitory projections to the thalamus. Specifically, we tested whether patterns of pallidal oscillatory LFP activity prior to correct or erroneous (in pitch) keypresses in a finger sequence differ. Such a phenomenon would be an indication of early gating mechanisms of cortical representations of the sequence, leading to facilitation of the execution of correct sequence items and avoidance of upcoming errors. Patients had to learn (~20min) five finger sequences for the right hand, consisting of 4-10 notes at a rate of 3 Hz, on a digital piano. Following the initial learning session, patients performed 10 trials of 25 seconds of each sequence type, while the performance was recorded as MIDI (music instruments digital interface) files. Simultaneously, LFPs were recorded bipolarly in the motor area of the GPi at sampling rate of 1kHz, with a bandpass filter of 0.5-400Hz. Analysis of the performance data revealed an average interonset-interval (IOI) of 400ms (standard deviation [SD] 60m) in correct pitch notes. Interestingly, prior to and following wrong pitch keypresses the IOI was significantly larger reflecting pre- and post-error slowing mechanisms (pre: 560 [70] ms, post: 700 [200] ms). Moreover, the MIDI velocity in pitch errors was significantly reduced as compared with the values of correct pitch keypresses. In sum, performance analysis pointed to an early detection of errors during motor skill acquisition which manifested as performance adjustments in pitch errors. Correspondingly, analysis of the LFP 4-100Hz oscillatory activity time-locked to correct and wrong pitch keypresses demonstrated two salient effects: first, a significant decrease in theta and alpha (4-13Hz) oscillatory activity 300-100ms prior to correct keypresses in the contralateral GPi, which was not present prior to errors. Second, in pitch errors, the reduced MIDI velocity converged with significantly less enhanced gamma (40- 5 Poster Sessions Full Abstracts 90Hz) oscillations at key onset, as compared to correct keypresses. In sum, decrease in pallidal low frequency LFP activity before correct keypresses might facilitate the selective execution of upcoming correct elements in a sequence during skill acquisition. This putative gating mechanism possibly depends on the degree of coherence in a prior cortical and striatal representation of the sequence serial order. 1-A-15 Limited visuomotor adaptation to variable amplitude gains within a movement trajectory trajectory. Instead, the results suggest that a single gain is employed over the entire movement in conjunction with a reactive control strategy. Decomposing the overall movement into smaller, fixed-gain movements might allow the motor system to efficiently compensate for tracking errors due to gain-mismatches using feedback control. 1-A-16 Physiological evidence of reduced neuroplasticity in human adolescents who were born preterm Julia Pitcher1, Alysha M Riley1, Michael C Ridding1 Deborah Barany1, Shivakumar Viswanathan1, Scott T Grafton1 1 1 Background: Neuroplasticity is the ability of the brain to alter neuronal synaptic strength in response to activity and experiences. It is widely accepted to be the mechanism underlying learning and memory formation, but also plays a key role in brain growth and development. Preterm children have alterations in cortical development, functional connectivity and neural activation patterns that suggests their capacity for neuroplastic reorganization may also be reduced, critically contributing to their common difficulties with learning and memory. Hypothesis: Preterm birth is associated with a reduced response to a non-invasive neuroplasticity induction intervention designed to induce a short-term LTD-like (i.e. inhibitory) change in motor cortex (M1) excitability. University of California, Santa Barbara Gain adaption is the motor system's ability to appropriately scale movement amplitude to match the physical demands of the environment with practice. Does this ability extend to learning different visuomotor gains within the same environment? The motor system can learn to use different amplitude gains along different movement directions (Pearson, Krakauer, & Mazzoni, 2010). However, it remains unclear whether the motor system can adapt to amplitude gains that vary along different segments within the movement trajectory itself. We evaluated this possibility in two experiments using a novel virtual-reality paradigm where participants tracked the visual movement of a metronome. The metronome was a virtual upright bar pivoted at the lower end, while the upper end oscillated along the perimeter of a notional semicircle with constant angular velocity (75 degrees/s). Participants tracked the metronome with a virtual right hand grasping a bar displayed on a computer screen. The virtual hand's orientation was updated continuously based on the current orientation of the participant's out-of-sight right hand. The gain (the ratio of virtual to actual movement amplitude) was equal to 1.5 within a central sector of width 60°; and 6 everywhere else. On each trial, participants tracked the metronome over 20 oscillations, followed by feedback indicating the Root-MeanSquare-Error (RMSE) on that trial and the percent improvement in RMSE over the previous trial. Participants were exposed to 40 adaptation trials interleaved with probe trials that occurred every 10 trials. In Experiment 1, the probes had a constant gain (equal to 6) over the entire movement trajectory. In Experiment 2, the probes only showed the metronome without any visual feedback about the participant's current position. In both experiments, participants' RMSE decreased over the 40 trials, showing that the task was indeed learnable. Due to the variable gains over the movement trajectory, an additional measure of adaptation was the extent to which the virtual hand "overshot" the metronome's position at the terminal points of the oscillation. As predicted, the overshoot error at these points was substantial on early trials but decreased with practice. Were two visuomotor maps used to minimize tracking error? The behavior on the probe trials suggested otherwise. In Experiment 1, the RMSE on the constant-gain probe trials remained high and largely unaffected by the decreasing error on the variable gain trials. Importantly, we found no evidence of anticipatory velocity changes at orientations corresponding to the previously experienced transition boundaries between gain fields. In Experiment 2, movement amplitudes on the no-feedback probe trials were substantially and symmetrically larger than on the learning trials, consistent with movements scaled using a single, low-valued gain over the entire movement trajectory. As in Experiment 1, we found no evidence of anticipatory effects at the gain-field transition boundaries. Together the results are inconsistent with the use of different spatially defined visuomotor mappings along the movement 6 University of Adelaide Methods: 25 children (15 females) aged 12-15 years (13.67 ± 0.48 years) participated; Term born (37-41 wks GA) N=6, Late preterm (33-36 wks GA) N=9 and Early preterm (24-32 wks GA) N=9. Continuous theta burst stimulation (cTBS) was applied to the M1 to induce LTD-like neuroplasticity. To assess changes in M1 excitability (an indicator of neuroplasticity), transcranial magnetic brain stimulation was used to evoke motor evoked potentials (MEPs) from a hand muscle before and up to 60 min following cTBS. Results: Term-born children showed robust MEP amplitude suppression immediately following cTBS that was greater and more persistent than that previously consistently recorded in adults. In comparison, MEP suppression in both preterm groups was significantly less than term born children and returned to baseline within 40 min of cTBS ceasing. GA correlated negatively with the mean MEP suppression following cTBS, i.e. the least suppression was evoked in the most preterm children. Conclusions: These data provide the first physiological evidence of reduced neuroplasticity in preterm children. While different types of neuroplasticity induction (i.e. LTPlike, behavioural) are yet to be assessed, these results demonstrate that even modest levels of prematurity are associated with significant impairments that persist at least into early adolescence. The underlying mechanisms are not yet clear, but may include synapse specific dysfunction and/or altered cortisol secretion patterns which are known to influence neuroplasticity. 1-A-17 The presence of multiple potential visual targets affects the retrieval of motor memory for a reaching movement Masaya Hirashima1, Daichi Nozaki1, Gaku D Yamawaki1 1 The University of Tokyo Humans can adapt movements to a novel dynamic environment by acquiring an internal model of the dynamics. It is suggested that the internal model is constructed by a population of the primitives encoding the desired joint Poster Sessions Full Abstracts kinematics. In this scheme, the desired joint kinematics determines the combination of the primitives to be retrieved for executing an upcoming movement, suggesting that as long as a physically identical movement is intended, identical primitives are always retrieved. However, a recent study demonstrated that distinct motor memories can be flexibly retrieved for executing a physically identical movement by planning distinct motions in a visual space (Hirashima and Nozaki, Curr Biol 2012). This finding suggests that the primitives to be retrieved are determined not only by the desired joint kinematics but also by the motor planning process. Although there is ample neurophysiological evidence showing that physically identical movement could be generated through distinct preparatory neural activities, it remains largely unknown how such preparatory activity affects the control of upcoming movement. Here, we investigate how motor planning process affects the retrieval of motor memory for an identical movement, by using force field adaptation paradigm with delayed reaching task. At the beginning of a delay period, one or two potential target locations were provided as prior target information available for motor planning (i.e., single-target and double-target condition), and then at the end of the delay period, the final target was randomly selected from the potential targets and illuminated as the GO signal. In experiment 1, after participants adapted to a curl force field with a particular reaching movement (toward 90º target) in single-target condition, they were occasionally required to perform double-target task (with 90º and 120º targets). By using errorcramp trials, we assessed the transfer of learning from single- to double-target condition in the identical 90º movement. We found that the degree of adaptation was significantly lower in doubletarget condition than single-target condition (p < 0.05). Considering the neurophysiological finding that relatively lower population activity was observed during the delay period when multiple potential targets were provided (Bastian et al. Eur J Neurosci 2003), the present results might be explained by assuming that preparatory activity level determines the degree of the retrieval of motor memory. However, it should be noted that this assumption predicts that a hyper generalization occurs from double- to single-target condition. To test this prediction, we conducted experiment 2 in which participants first adapted to the force field with two reaching movements (90º and 120º) in doubletarget condition, and then they were required to perform them in single-target condition. Results exhibited no evidence of hypergeneralization; the degree of adaptation decreased in singletarget condition for 90º target (p < 0.05) and there was no significant difference for 120º target (p > 0.05). Taken together, the results suggest that motor planning process affects the retrieval of motor memory in a way that activation "pattern" of the network rather than the level of population activity determines the primitives to be retrieved. 1-A-18 Plasticity in amputees: Reorganization in somatosensory and motor cortices varies with adaptive strategies for limb use Tamar Makin1, Jan Scholz2, Nicola Filippini1, David Henderson Slater3, Irene Tracey1, Heidi Johansen-Berg1 1 Oxford University, 2Hospital for Sick Children, 3Nuffield Orthopaedic Centre Our brain's ability to reorganize itself by forming new neural connections throughout life is a key mechanism that enables adjustments to novel situations, as well as compensation for nervous system injury. Following hand-amputation, two types of reorganization could occur. First, reorganization associated with sensory deprivation resulting from the loss of the hand, which is commonly viewed as maladaptive. Second, reorganization due to altered patterns of use of the remaining limbs, which could play an adaptive role; to overcome their impairment, individuals will adopt new strategies for using their intact hand or their residual arm (on the amputated side). The majority of research on amputees to date has focused on maladaptive plasticity, whereas we aimed to test whether plasticity plays an adaptive role following amputation. We tested for functional variations associated with different patterns of limb use in individuals with a unilateral congenital or traumatic upper-limb deficit. Using an adapted version of the motor activity log (MAL) inventory, we identified different limb use strategies in these two groups: whereas congenitals were likely to use their residual arm during daily tasks, the late amputees predominantly relied on their intact hand. Accordingly, a fMRI based somatomotor-mapping test showed a dissociation in remapping patterns in these two populations: congenitals showed contralateral overrepresentation of the residual arm, compared with both late amputees and controls. In the primary somatomotor cortex, this over-representation overlapped with activations relating to movements of the phantom (absent) hand in the late amputees, as well as movements of the non-dominant hand in controls. By contrast, late amputees showed ipsilateral over-representation of the intact hand, compared with the other groups, in the primary somatomotor cortex associated with the phantom hand. Moreover, increases in ipsilateral representation of the intact hand correlated negatively with MAL scores across both amputees populations: individuals that were less able to utilize their residual arm in daily tasks were more likely to show increased over-representation of the intact hand in the phantom cortex. These results suggest that functional plasticity is not restricted to a critical period, but is rather contingent upon the limb use strategy adopted by individuals. 1-A-19 Cortical networks involved in speech recovery after glossectomy: Preliminary results of an fMRI study Audrey Acher1, Marc Sato1, Laurent Lamalle2, Coriandre Vilain1, Alexandre Krainik2, Pascal Perrier1 1 Gipsa-lab - UMR CNRS 5216, 2CHU de Grenoble Tongue surgery, also called glossectomy, is often necessary in the clinical treatment of tongue cancer. It is often associated with radiotherapy. In some cases these treatments induce a noticeable reduction of tongue mobility, and capacities of the patient to shape his vocal tract. Intelligibility of speech is strongly altered. Hence, speech production after tongue surgery often requires the patient to go through a long speech recovery process. We investigate cortical networks involved in the recovery process. It is assumed that a major step is the learning of new relations between motor commands, oro-sensory feedback and spectral characteristics of speech. It has been proposed that these relations could be stored in the form of internal representations of the motor system, which could be implemented in the cerebellum. We expect the speech recovery process to be associated with significant changes in cerebellar activations: they should be stronger during the first part of the process, as the results of the learning, before returning to a normal level in the second part. Once these new internal representations acquired, patients should elaborate new compensation strategies, in order to reach their usual acoustic speech goals. Intact articulators, i.e. jaw and lips, should be used more systematically and possibly with a greater accuracy than before surgery, in order to overcome the limits of tongue mobility. This could induce activity changes in ventral premotor and primary motor cortices, associated with orofacial motor control (Grabski et 7 Poster Sessions Full Abstracts al., in press). For some speech units (phonemes or syllables), the usual goals of speech production are likely to be not reachable any more. For these units, speech recovery would then require the definition of new goals, both in oro-sensory and auditory domains. From this view, auditory speech goals have been suggested to be located in the left temporo-parietal junction (Hickok & Poppel, 2004) while brain regions sensitive to somatosensory goals are hypothesized to be located in the somatosensory cortex and the antero-dorsal part of the inferior lobule and adjacent intraparietal sulcus (Golfinopoulos et al., 2011). We expect all these regions to be strongly activated during the first part of the learning process. Finally, learning new coordination strategies between speech articulators should be reflected in increased activity in the left anterior insula. To assess these hypotheses, patients were recorded longitudinally in 1 presurgery and 3 post-surgery sessions, within 9 months. Using sparse sampling acquisition in order to minimize movementrelated artifacts and scanner noise, patients were asked to produce speech and non speech orofacial motor tasks in each fMRI session. Recordings are currently in progress and preliminary results will be presented at the conference. Hickok, G. & Poeppel, D. (2004). Dorsal and ventral streams: A framework for understanding aspects of the functional anatomy of language. Cognition, 92, 67-99. Grabski, K., Lamalle, L., Vilain, C., Schwartz, J.-L, Vallée, N. Troprès, I., Baciu, M. Le Bas, J.-F & Sato, M. (In Press). Functional MRI assessment of orofacial articulators: neural correlates of lip, jaw, larynx and tongue movements. Human Brain Mapping. Golfinopoulos, E., Tourville, J.A., Bohland, J.W., Ghosh, S.S., Nieto-Castanon, A. & Guenther, F.H. (2011). fMRI investigation of unexpected somatosensory feedback perturbation during speech. NeuroImage, 55(3):132438. 1-A-20 Comparing neural activity for repetitive finger and foot movements in cerebellar patients and healthy controls reveals lesion induced changes in specific regions of cortex Paul Pope1, Roxana Burciu2, Maria Dagioglou1, Nina Theysohn2, R Chris Miall1, Dagmar Timmann2 1 University of Birmingham, 2University of Duisburg-Essen Objective: The extent to which cerebellar-cortical connectivity is modified during post-stroke cortical reorganization is little understood, yet potentially useful for understanding lesioninduced network plasticity. Here we used functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging (fMRI) to investigate differential patterns of neural activity in cerebellar patients and healthy controls using tasks that have previous been shown to activate the cerebellum and motor regions of cortex. Method: Blood Oxygenation Level Dependent (BOLD) signals were recorded using a fast fMRI sequence in cerebellar patients at least one year following their stroke (N = 14), and age/education/gender matched controls (N = 14), whilst they reproduced either repetitive sequence of finger-thumb opposition, or foot-tapping movements with the left or right limbs in four separate runs of 372 s, cued by a video inside an MR scanner (1.5 T). Covariates of motor behaviour (time and force) were also recorded simultaneously. Patterns of neural activity for each condition were compared between groups with General Linear Model (GLM) analyses after flipping data sets so that lesion sites were on the same side, and the locus of neural activity was complementary between groups. Results: Inspection of motor behaviour revealed accurate task compliance in the scanner. Functional data associated with this performance revealed a network of task-specific brain regions, including: the cerebellum, motor and pre-motor cortices, supplementary motor area, together with frontal and parietal 8 regions of cortex in all four conditions. However, activity in post central gyrus was different in patients than controls for sequential hand and foot movements, which may be explained in terms of post-stroke reorganization processes that accompany the partial recovery of cerebellar function in patients relative to controls. Further analyses will correlate task-specific brain activity in local regions of cortex with covariates of motor behaviour such as movement time and response force, together with scores for the assessment of motor and cognitive functions. Conclusion: Repetitive and sequential movements of the fingers and feet are accompanied by task-specific MR activations, which are affected differently in cerebellar patients and healthy controls, presumably due to cortical reorganization after stoke. Exploring the relationship between brain activation and covariates of motor behaviour in local regions of interest will further expand our understanding of specific changes in the brain after cerebellar stoke. Supported by the Wellcome Trust and a Marie Curie Initial Training Network C7 (Cerebellar-Cortical Control: Cells, Circuits, Computation and Clinic). 1-A-21 Learning finger coordination patterns by altering dimensionality Robert Scheidt1, Rajiv Ranganathan2, Jon A Wieser1, Kristine M Mosier3, Ferdinando A Mussa-Ivaldi2 1 Marquette University, 2Rehabilitation Institute of Chicago, Indiana University School of Medicine 3 Motor skill acquisition can sometimes be facilitated by breaking a task down into simpler components (part-whole practice). However, in cases where multiple degrees of freedom are involved, learning task components may result in the suboptimal use redundant degrees of freedom when performing the whole task. Here, we investigate whether learning of a redundant 2-D task can be facilitated by manipulating (i.e. either reducing or augmenting) the dimensionality of training. 51 subjects wore a data glove to perform a virtual reaching task using their fingers. 19-signals from the glove were linearly mapped onto the 2-D position of a cursor on a display screen. The screen was a 5x5 grid of 25 target squares. Subjects were instructed to move the cursor into the specified target as quickly and accurately as possible. They practiced this task for three days. The mapping of finger motions onto cursor motion was formed from the first two principal components obtained during a finger-spelling task (hereafter referred to as the "original" map). Subjects were divided into five groups that differed in the map practiced on Day 1: Two groups practiced a 2-D task on Day 1 - (a) the 2D group used the original map, (b) the 2D -Null group practiced with two dimensions that were in the null space of the original map (PC3 and PC4). Two groups practiced with only one-dimension of the task - (c) the 1D group used a map with the first dimension of the original map (i.e., PC1) whereas (d) the 1D-Null group practiced the task with PC3 (in the null space of the original map). Finally, (e) the 3D group practiced the task using three dimensions - two from the original map that controlled the location of the cursor and a third (PC3) that controlled the size of the cursor. After practicing the different maps on Day 1, all groups performed the 2D task using the original map on Days 2 and 3. The extent of learning and transfer was assessed using the performance on Day 2 when all groups performed the same task. On Day 2, there was a systematic trend in task performance depending on the number of Day 1 task components that were shared with the "original" Day 2 map. The best performance was achieved by the 2D and the 3D groups, where two components were Poster Sessions Full Abstracts shared. This was followed by the 1D group which had one component in common - but the performance of this group on Day 2 was not different from the naive performance of the 2D group on Day 1. Finally, the 1D-Null group and the 2D-Null groups that had no shared components had the worst levels of performance on Day 2. In particular, the 2D-Null group showed sustained interference even after 2 days of practice. Data analysis revealed that subjects in the 2D-Null group were exploring the null space even after the map had changed, indicating that the map learned by the participants on Day 1 was interfering with the acquisition of the map on Days 2 and 3. These results demonstrate that when subjects perform a redundant kinematic task, they develop a representation of the degrees of freedom that are relevant to task performance. This representation has strong interference with maps of that are based on orthogonal subspaces. Accordingly, altering dimensionality during training must be conducted with care in tasks with multiple degrees of freedom. In particular, uncontrolled recruitment of null-space motions during practice may lead to the use of suboptimal coordination patterns in the fulldimensional task. 1-A-22 Ontogeny of vestibulo-ocular reflex following genetic or environmental alteration of gravity-perception Mathieu Beraneck1, Mickael Bojados2, Anne Le Séach1, Marc Jamon2, Pierre-Paul Vidal1 1 CNRS - University Paris Descartes, 2Aix-Marseille Univ, UMR 1106 The vestibular organs consist of complementary sensors: the semicircular canals detect rotations while the otoliths detect linear accelerations, including the constant pull of gravity. Several fundamental questions remain on how the vestibular system would develop and/or adapt to prolonged changes in gravity such as during long-term space journey. The aim of present work was to evaluate the role of otolith information during ontogeny of the vestibular system. Inner ear defect (ied) mutant mice suffer from otoconial agenesis and thus develop ontogenetically in the absence of the vestibular perception of gravity. In ied mice maculo-ocular reflexes were absent. While canals-related reflexes were present, the ied deficit also led to the abnormal gain, timing, and spatial tuning of the horizontal angular vestibulo-ocular reflex. The perturbation of the encoding of gravity by the otolith organs could therefore have impaired the development of canal-related pathways. To test this hypothesis and identify putative otolithrelated critical periods, C57Bl/6 mice were subjected to 2G hypergravity by chronic centrifugation during different periods of development and compared to adult centrifuged mice. One month after the end of the centrifugation, horizontal angular vestibuloocular reflex of all centrifuged animals was normal, while maculoocular reflexes was transitorily impaired in mice which completely developed in hypergravity. The maculo-ocular reflexes of adult centrifuged mice were also impaired; however long-term effects were found to persist in a subset of adult mice, suggesting chronic alteration in the processing of vestibular signals. In summary, genetic suppression of gravity-related signals indicated that otolith-related signals might be necessary to ensure proper functioning of canal-related vestibular reflexes. On the other hand, exposure to hypergravity was not sufficient to alter durably motor behaviour, probably because the otolithic perception of gravity was altered but not suppressed. 1-A-23 The impact of limb agenesis on brain structure Erika Rodrigues1, Fernanda Tovar-Moll2, Ivanei Bramati2, Claudia Vargas3, Jorge Moll2, Angela Sirigu4 1 Augusto Motta University Center (UNISUAM), 2D Or Institute for Research and Education (IDOR), 3Federal University of Rio de Janeiro, 4Center for Cognitive Neuroscience Motor areas in the brain are continuously under the effect of sensory-motor learning. Here we asked whether undeveloped body parts following injuries occurring in utero impact on the way the corresponding movement representation develops during ontogenesis. We addressed this question by examining gray and white matter changes in individuals (N=5) with left or right upper limb agenesis and compared them with traumatic upper limb amputees (N=9) and healthy controls (N=10). Conventional MRI (FLAIR, T1- and T2-weighted images) showed no anatomical differences for the three groups. Cortical thickness and diffusion tensor imaging (DTI) analyses, however, revealed clear cortical and white matter changes in agenesic amputees compared to controls and the traumatic group. Whole brain voxelwise tract-based spatial statistics analysis (TBSS; FSL 4.0, FMRIB software) revealed extensive fractional anisotropy (FA) reduction in the cortico-spinal tract, the superior longitudinal fasciculus, the corpus callosum and the cingulate bundle in the hemisphere contralateral to the missing limb in agenesics subjects, but not in traumatic amputees who did not differed from the healthy group. Whole brain analysis further showed reduced grey matter thickness in the agenesics' hand and arm area of the primary motor cortex (M1) contralateral to the absent limb (Freesurfer image analysis suite, version 4.4). These structural differences were not found in the hemisphere ipsilateral to the healthy limb. Thus, having never experienced movements of the limb has an impact on the structural organization of the sensorimotor cortical areas and related white-matter pathways. Such massive reorganization may explain why these patients contrary to traumatic amputees never develop phantom movements, which are supposed to be caused by the activation of a persistent representation of the lost limb in M1. Remarkably, all changes were exclusively contralateral to the absent limb. Our findings suggest that motor experience is crucial for the development of the hand motor representation in the primary motor cortex. We can speculate that patients with limb agenesis have never activated motor commands towards muscles. This may have a consequence on the way cortico-spinal pathways are shaped during ontogenesis. 1-A-24 Recent action determines the encoding of motor memory Ian Howard1, David W Franklin1, James N Ingram1, Daniel M Wolpert1 1 University of Cambridge Real world tasks often require movements that follow on from a previous action. Here we investigate the contextual effect of immediate prior motion using an interference task. Subjects performed trials in which they made movements in a randomly selected clockwise or counter-clockwise velocity-dependent curl force-field. Movements during this adaptation phase were preceded by a contextual phase that determined which of the two fields would be experienced. As expected, when a static visual cue was used in the contextual phase to indicate the direction of the field, strong interference was observed. However, when the contextual phase involved subjects making a movement that was continuous with the adaptation phase movement, a substantial reduction in interference was seen. As the time between these two movements increased, so did the interference, reaching a level similar to the interference seen for static visual cues for delays greater than 600 ms. The contextual effect for such recent motion generalized to purely visual motion, active movement without vision and passive movement. In addition, isometric force generation 9 Poster Sessions Full Abstracts without movement also provided the same contextual effect. Our results show that active, passive or visual prior motion, as well as static force production, can each engage different representations in motor memory. This suggests that recent action in a variety of modalities has a special role in determining the context for motor learning, as compared to static cues. However, as the delay between recent action and the movement is increased, the contextual effect progressively decays, suggesting that the encoding of motor memory depends strongly on the recency of the action. 1-A-25 Adaptation to force perturbations alters feedforward and feedback signals in Purkinje cell simple spike firing Angela Hewitt1, Laurentiu S Popa1, Timothy J Ebner1 1 University of Minnesota Motor control theorists have postulated that the nervous system employs a forward internal model to predict the consequences of a motor command. A forward internal model has many useful properties, including bypassing sensory feedback delays, coordinating movements between effectors, and providing training signals. Although the neurophysiological substrate for a forward internal model remains unknown, psychophysical, imaging, and patient case studies suggest it is implemented in the cerebellum. If true, Purkinje cell (PC) simple spike firing should behave as a forward internal model and "learn" new input-output properties of the motor apparatus during adaptation to predictable force perturbations. To test this, extracellular firing from single PCs (lobules IV-VI of the intermediate/lateral zones) was recorded while two rhesus monkeys used a robotic manipulandum to move a cursor from a start target to a cue target, resulting in very fast (< 750 ms) reaching movements along a 10 cm path. Animals completed four blocks of movements. First, baseline trials obtained the cell's preferred direction and the firing discharge in an error clamp with no force perturbations. The error clamp consisted of robotgenerated virtual walls that required the animal to move along a highly defined path towards the target, minimizing small tangential kinematic errors that might mask learning effects. The second block adapted the animal to the force perturbation. Perturbation parameters, including magnitude, start position, and duration, were randomized between recording days. The third block continued using the perturbation, but also included catch trials (randomized at 10-15%), where the perturbation was unexpectedly absent. A fourth block repeated baseline conditions. Kinematic analyses illustrate that both animals make large changes in arm velocity and force to rapidly correct for the perturbation within several trials, and then gradually continue adjusting parameters to optimize their movements. Similarly, 6070% of task-modulated PCs show steady, progressive firing changes with adaptation. Catch trials also demonstrate strong adaptation, as reaching movements often over- or undershoot the target as expected. Firing activity during a catch trial closely matches that of adaptation trials in the period before the perturbation should occur, but large differences arise thereafter. Preliminary modeling fit the firing data to multiple linear regression models of hand kinematics (position and velocity) and kinetics (force parallel or tangential to movement). An additional model parameter τ estimated the time lead or lag between PC firing and hand kinematics or kinetics. During baseline conditions, maximum model R2adj values peak at both feedforward and feedback τ timepoints for many PCs. Initial exposure to the perturbation disrupts these depictions, and the best model fit often reflects a feedback representation of kinematics or force. With adaptation, best model fits frequently return to including both feedforward and feedback signals that may be shifted in time 10 compared to baseline data. Overall, adaptation appears to alter the relationships between feedforward and feedback signals present in PC firing. This is consistent with a forward internal model learning new input-output properties of the motor apparatus. Supported in part by NIH grants RO1 NS18338, F30 NS071686, T32 GM008244 1-A-26 Motor learning increases the distinctiveness of cortical sequence representations: Evidence from multi-voxel pattern analysis Tobias Wiestler1, Jörn Diedrichsen1 1 University College London The study of motor learning using functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) has revealed a complex picture of activity changes in the human brain. Some regions increase activity, often interpreted as evidence for increased neural recruitment through the learning process. Activity decreases with learning are also found in many regions. These may either indicate that an area has become less important for the skilled behavior, or, alternatively, that the same regions now encodes the learned behavior more efficiently. The complex overlap of these possible different mechanisms makes the interpretation of overall activity changes during learning problematic. Here we test the hypothesis that motor skills are associated with the development of neuronal units within motor cortical network that are specialized for the trained behavior. We therefore predict that local activity patterns for different behaviors should become more distinct from each other when a person acquires higher levels of performance. We show here that these changes are detectable using functional magnetic resonance imaging and multi-voxel pattern analysis, and that they can occur in the absence of overall activity changes. We trained participants over 4 days to produce 4 fast sequences that are comprised of a series of 5 finger presses in different orders. The sequences were matched for difficulty and force, and each sequence included each finger once. Training decreased movement times from 2s to 1.1s over the four days. The learning generalized partially to untrained sequences, as well as to the other hand. Above and beyond this general learning we found a sequence-specific improvement of 280ms for the trained sequences only. To study neuronal representational changes participants produced trained and untrained finger sequences while undergoing fMRI. Due to the matching of sequences, the overall activity level elicited by each sequence was nearly identical. Using local multi-voxel pattern analysis, however, we could detect regions in which the fine-grained activation patterns differed significantly between different sequences. We could show that the right motor cortex, and bihemispheric premotor, supplementary motor and parietal areas encode sequential aspects of left hand actions. We also show that a single behavioral confound, such as force or movement speed alone cannot explain the classification results. We then predicted that the natural skill level of participants to produce fast finger sequence movements should correlate with our ability to decode these sequences from the activity patterns in cortical motor areas. Indeed, faster participants showed clearer differences between the activity patterns for different sequences, even if they did not perform faster during the scan. Interestingly no such correlations could be found between performance and the overall BOLD signal. Comparing the classification accuracies for trained sequences with those for untrained sequences, we also show that motor training leads to specific increases in decoding accuracy in the Poster Sessions Full Abstracts supplementary motor area (SMA). This result suggests that SMA contains highly specialized neuronal units, which represent specific chunks of the trained sequences and therefore underlies the sequence specific-learning effects. No changes in overall activity were found in SMA. Our results therefore provide a novel method for assessing the development of skilled representations in the human brain. 1-A-27 The effect of acquisition of an internal forward model on an exploration task Maria Dagioglou1, Joaquin Bugella1, Tom Walton2, Tom Stafford2, Peter Redgrave2, Chris Miall1 1 University of Birmingham, 2University of Sheffield Fast and accurate execution of movements is achieved to a large extent because the brain is able to predict their outcome. The cerebellum is believed to contain internal forward models that simulate our movements and calculate the predicted outcome. In parallel, the acquisition and selection of actions is believed to depend on the basal ganglia, which also subserve reinforcement learning. We hypothesize that participants can perform better in a novel exploration task (e.g. a task where detection of a target is via a "tool" whose dynamics are uncertain), if they can first learn the relationship between movements and outcomes. If supported, this hypothesis suggests cerebellar learning of forward models can be exploited by reinforcement-based learning within the basal ganglia. Participants used a joystick under two different kinds of trials: exploration trials and tracking trials. During exploration, participants were asked to find two hidden targets and hit them alternately, as many times as possible. The workspace flashed 150 ms after a target was hit, imposing a delayed relationship between current action state and reward. During these trials participants had no visual feedback of their position in the workspace. In the tracking trials the subjects were asked to follow a continuous, pseudorandomly moving target. Their position in the workspace was displayed as a cursor representing either the current position of the joystick (Group A, n=10) or its position delayed by 150 ms (Group B, n=10). Block 1 consisted of sixteen 30-s exploration trials with new random target positions in each trial. In each of Blocks 2-8, participants completed ten 30-s tracking trials, followed by two exploration trials. During the final Block 9 participants again performed 16 exploration trials, with 150 ms delayed reward. An Index of Performance (IoP) was calculated for each of the exploration trials, equal to the number of hits per trial over the total distance travelled in each trial (normalized by the separation of the targets). The results showed a significant interaction of Block x Delay (F(1,18)=5, p=0.038).We observed an increase of the mean IoP in Group B (delay in both exploration and tracking trials) and a decrease in Group A (delay only in the exploration trials). The preliminary results support our hypothesis that experience in delayed tracking (Group B) would improve performance in the delayed exploration task. Hence adapting the state estimate of movement outcome, putatively in the cerebellum, facilitates reinforcement learning, putatively in the basal ganglia. Funded by the Wellcome Trust and EU-fP7-ITN "C7" 1-A-28 Transfer of ballistic motor skills between bilateral and unilateral contexts in young and older adults: Neural adaptations and behavioural implications Mark Hinder1, Timothy J Carroll2, Jeffery J Summers1 1 University of Tasmania, 2University of Queensland Bilateral movement rehabilitation therapies are gaining popularity and aim not only to improve the recovery of bimanual actions, but also aim to improve unilateral motor functions. Despite this, the neural mechanisms mediating the transfer of bilateral training gains into unimanual task contexts are not fully understood. Converging evidence from behavioural, neurophysiological and imaging studies suggests that bimanual movements are not simply the superposition of unimanual tasks undertaken with both (upper) limbs. However, bilateral practice may release inhibition within both hemispheres, conceivably facilitating subsequent unilateral actions. The current study was designed to investigate the neural mechanisms associated with a bilateral ballistic motor task, and to assess the extent to which improvements in performance transferred to a unimanual task. We have previously used this motor task to investigate the transfer of practice related gains between the hands. The task is appealing as training related improvements are mediated substantially within primary motor cortex, and thus can be assessed by way of transcranial magnetic stimulation (TMS). Moreover, recent work has shown that the mechanisms mediating performance gains in ballistic motor task are akin to those mechanisms mediating strength gains during resistance training. Information from such experiments is therefore applicable to rehabilitation contexts where, following stroke or limb immobilisation, muscle atrophy and loss of strength are ubiquitous. Young (n=9; mean age 19.4 years) and older (n=9; 66.3 years) adults were trained in a bilateral motor task in which they were required to simultaneously abduct their left and right index fingers as quickly as possible with visual feedback of task performance. Before, during and after bilateral training we assessed performance in a bimanual and unilateral task context (in the absence of feedback) and measured corticospinal excitability and intracortical inhibition using TMS. We observed a high degree of transfer between the bimanual and unimanual contexts for both groups, i.e., bimanual training resulted in significant performance gains in both bimanual and unimanual tasks. However, regression analyses revealed that only for the older adults did the bimanual training gain predict subsequent unimanual performance for either hand. Task-related increases in corticospinal excitability and releases of inhibition (which we have previously consistently shown in both hemispheres following unilateral ballistic motor tasks) were minimal and only observable for older adults. Overall, the results indicate a greater overlap of the neural mechanisms mediating bilateral and unilateral ballistic motor learning in older adults. 1-A-29 Risk-sensitivity in motor learning Michael Trent1, Alaa A Ahmed1 1 University of Colorado Not all movement errors are created equal. For example, compare a 5 cm error in foot placement when approaching the edge of a curb, to the same error approaching the edge of a cliff. One would likely avoid the cliff edge more than the edge of the curb. Thus, movement planning would be risksensitive and depend on the subjective value of an error, rather than its actual value. Interestingly, models of movement adaptation have traditionally assumed that adaptation is proportional to movement error1. In recent years the notion of proportionality been challenged2,3. However, the role of risk-sensitivity, which emerges from a distortion between the subjective and actual value of an error, has not been investigated in movement adaptation. Here we quantified adaptation in a unique cliff-like virtual environment that was stable, but only within certain limits, to influence the subjective value associated with a given movement error. Seated subjects (N=8) made reaching movements to a target directly ahead of them while grasping the handle of a robotic arm. Feedback was presented on a monitor at eye-level. The protocol consisted of: 50 no-force 11 Poster Sessions Full Abstracts trials, 200 in a viscous curl-field (stable condition), 450 in an identical curl-field with a cliff-like region of instability (unstable condition), and 50 no- force. Curl-field gains varied from trial to trial and were biased to the left. During the unstable condition, curl-field dynamics were maintained but rightward errors greater than 2.5cm were penalized, with a line indicating edge of the boundary. In the first 50 trials of the unstable condition a strong rightward force was applied if the cursor crossed the cliff. After these 50 trials, this force was de-activated leaving only an audiovisual warning should the subject cross the boundary. Importantly, because most movement errors were less than 2.5 cm, subjects were merely alerted to the presence of the cliff. To adapt subjects must produce a force towards the instability. We hypothesized that errors closer to the edge would be penalized more heavily than errors of the same magnitude that occurred when the instability was not present. This would lead to reduced adaptation to the stronger gains. Adaptation was calculated in both conditions using previously developed methods2. Movement error was similar across conditions (P > 0.1027). Adaptation to the strongest perturbation was greater in the stable compared to the unstable condition (P = 0.0050), but similar for the remaining gains. Model results corroborate these findings. Sensitivity, fit using a state-space model2, was larger in the stable compared to the unstable condition only for the largest gains (P < 0.01), and similar for all others (P > 0.123). This asymmetry suggests that subjects were responding to the fact that over-compensation was penalized only for rightward errors. These results imply that movement adaptation is risk-sensitive. It is not solely dependent on movement error, but can be modulated by the subjective value associated with the error. 1. Scheidt et al. (2001) Learning to move amid uncertainty. Journal of Neurophysiology. 2. Fine and Thoroughman (2007) Journal of Neurophysiology. 3. Wei and Kording (2009) Journal of Neurophysiology. 1-A-30 Our brain sees and learns during fast eye movements Muriel Panouilleres1, Valérie Gaveau1, Christian Urquizar1, Denis Pelisson1 1 CRNL, INSERM U1028 Motor adaptation relies on our ability to detect errors when we perform actions and to correct them with subsequent movements. To study adaptation of saccadic eye movements in human, a trick is used to create an artificial saccade inaccuracy, thanks to the double-step target paradigm (McLaughlin, 1967). This paradigm consists in stepping the target a first time to trigger the saccade and a second time to a new location during the eye movement. Because of saccadic suppression, subjects are usually unaware of the second intra-saccadic target step. It is thus thought that the post-saccadic error between eyes and target positions is the signal necessary to recalibrate the saccadic system (postsaccadic hypothesis, e.g.: Bahcall et Kowler, 2000). In a previous study, we showed that very short post-saccadic visual information (stepped target visible only for saccade duration 15 ms) was sufficient for inducing optimal saccadic adaptation (Panouillères et al, 2011). Moreover, Gaveau et al. (2003) showed that some retinal information is processed during large eye movement. Based on these two previous studies, we then made the assumption that our brain could process and learn from an error available only during the saccade, but not after its completion (intra-saccadic hypothesis). To test this hypothesis, we used a modified double-step target paradigm in which the second stepped target was switched off at the end of saccades. In two separate experiments, the intra-saccadic target step was directed toward the fixation point to induce a decrease of saccade amplitude or away from the fixation point to produce an increase of saccade amplitude. Twenty subjects took part to this study, and were randomly assigned to one of the two experiments (10 12 subjects each). Each experiment comprised 3 recording sessions, performed with at least 5-7 days in-between. In the intra-saccadic adaptation session, we tested our intrasaccadic hypothesis by stepping the target at the beginning of the saccade and then switching it off at the end of it. In the post-saccadic adaptation session, the stepped target was presented for the same duration as in the intra-saccadic adaptation session, but immediately after the end of the saccade. According to the classical post-saccadic hypothesis of error processing, this session should provide the largest amount of saccadic adaptation. In the control session, the target did not jump, but remained at its location for the duration of the saccade and was then turned off. For the first experiment, we found that the same amount of saccadic adaptation was achieved for the intra-saccadic and the post-saccadic adaptation sessions. Even more interestingly, this adaptation was highly similar to the one we found in our previous study, where the error signal was presented for the duration of the saccade plus 15ms in the post-saccade period. This suggests that our brain can process error signals during an eye movement and that it can learn from this error. Preliminary data in the second experiment suggest that this conclusion can be extended to large errors requiring an increase of saccade amplitude. B - Integrative Control of Movement 1-B-89 A study of the influence of biomechanics on decisions between reaching movements Ignasi Cos1, Paul Cisek1 1 Université de Montreal There is still considerable debate about the influence of the arm's biomechanical properties on the planning of voluntary movements. While several models describe reach planning as primarily kinematic, some studies have suggested that implicit knowledge about biomechanics may also exert influence on the planning and preparation of reaching movements. Here we present the results of three related experiments showing first that biomechanics and factors related to movement stability are predicted during motor planning, because they influence choices between voluntary movements, and second, that these influence the choice at different latencies. In the first experiment, human subjects made free choices between two potential reaching movements, each defined by the origin, a via-point and a target, varying in path distance and biomechanical factors related to movement energy and stability. Our results show that subjects preferred movements whose final trajectory was better aligned with the direction of lowest biomechanical cost, even when the launching properties were very similar. This reveals that the nervous system can predict biomechanical properties of potential actions prior to movement onset and that these predictions, in addition to purely abstract criteria, exert an influence on the decisionmaking process. Next, we performed a second set of experiments to test whether cost factors related to arm stability were also predicted and whether they exerted a bias on reach selection. To this end, we quantified the modulation of the baseline pattern of choices of each subject, as a function of two additional factors related to the ease of control of the end-point in the vicinity of the target: the ease of aiming and the requirement of stopping. The results show that the strongest preference for the low biomechanical cost choice appears in the absence of these Poster Sessions Full Abstracts constraints. As the control constraints are gradually imposed, this preference is gradually reduced. This shows that factors related to movement stability modulate the influence of biomechanical cost on the subjects' choices already during movement preparation, prior to movement onset. Finally, our last set of experiments examined the latencies at which different factors influence motor decision-making prior to movement onset. Hypothetically, if the encoding of factors such as distance or direction is attributed to the fronto-parietal loop and the computation of biomechanics to the cerebellum, their latencies to reach the motor plan should be significantly different. To test this, we constrained the time subjects could observe the targets and via-points prior to movement onset to a range between 0.2 and 1s. Unexpectedly, the subjects' choices showed that the influence of biomechanical factors precedes the influence of path distance, therefore suggesting that the calculation of biomechanical factors may take advantage of priors including knowledge of biomechanics along different arrival directions. In summary, these three experiments provide insights into decision-making between reaching movements, demonstrating that: 1) biomechanics and stability are two influential factors, even prior to movement onset; and 2) motor decision-making is a gradual process, first incorporating intrinsic and stability properties and secondly additional kinematic factors such as path distance. Presumably, this process yields choices that are most comfortable and easiest to control. Support: NSERC, CRCNS. 1-B-90 Measurable improvements in dexterous manipulation throughout adolescence reveal previously undetected functional effects of neuromaturation Sudarshan Dayanidhi1, Åsa Hedberg2, Francisco J ValeroCuevas1, Hans Forssberg2 1 University of Southern California, 2Karolinska Institutet Neural control of dexterous manipulation is attributed to specific neuroanatomical structures whose connectivity and function are known to have a prolonged period of development into late adolescence[1-3]. In contrast, functional improvements in dexterous manipulation--as measureable by current developmental and clinical milestones--show few changes past the age of eight[4] because most measures of hand function saturate. We now show that an extension of our prior work[5, 6] bridges this apparent discrepancy and establishes a novel and clear link between known neuroanatomical development and dexterous manipulation well in to late adolescence. Importantly, musculoskeletal growth and strength are poorly correlated with these functional improvements in dexterity. These results begin to clarify the behavioral benefits of such neural maturation, enable the systematic study of specific neuroanatomical structures, their connectivity, and plasticity. For example, neuroimaging studies to disambiguate the differential roles and contributions of maturation of the corticospinal tract vs. the emergence of fronto-parietal and cortico-striatal-cerebellar networks. Clinically, this extends the ages for which therapeutic interventions can be considered fruitful, and provides a clinically-practical means to chart functional development of dexterous manipulation in typically developing children, and children with neurological conditions. References 1. Paus, T., et al., Structural maturation of neural pathways in children and adolescents: in vivo study. Science, 1999. 283(5409): p. 1908-11. 2. Lebel, C., et al., Microstructural maturation of the human brain from childhood to adulthood. Neuroimage, 2008. 40(3): p. 1044-55. 3. Muller, K., V. Homberg, and H.G. Lenard, Magnetic stimulation of motor cortex and nerve roots in children. Maturation of cortico-motoneuronal projections. Electroencephalogr Clin Neurophysiol, 1991. 81(1): p. 63-70. 4. Forssberg, H., et al., Development of human precision grip. I: Basic coordination of force. Exp Brain Res, 1991. 85(2): p. 451-7. 5. Vollmer, B., et al., Evidence of validity in a new method for measurement of dexterity in children and adolescents. Developmental Medicine & Child Neurology, 2010. 52(10): p. 948-954. 6. Valero-Cuevas, F.J., et al., The strengthdexterity test as a measure of dynamic pinch performance. J Biomech, 2003. 36(2): p. 265-70. 1-B-91 Identification of human limb impedance in 5 DoF Patrick van der Smagt1, Dominic Lakatos1, Daniel Rüschen1, Jörn Vogel1 1 DLR / RM Dynamic interaction with the environment means handling impacts and unknown contact forces. Therefore compliant systems are active topics of research in the field of robotics. Surpassing traditional rigid robots, the control loops of modern robotic systems are extended with additional impedance parameter, viz. stiffness and damping. Even though the implementation of impedance control in robotics is resolved to a large part, one important issue still needs to be resolved: how are the impedance parameters set to optimally perform a predefined task? Traditionally, robotic tasks are only defined in target end-effector positions or, in some cases, end-effector trajectories; but the impedance around these positions or trajectories remains a matter of common sense, at best. For instance, when performing a peg-in-hole task, high stiffness in the perpendicular and low stiffness in the lateral directions, so as to allow for imprecise positioning while solving the task, appears to be useful. But how do we find general rules-ofthumb for setting these extra parameters? Beside heuristic methods tuning the impedance parameters, mimicking the behaviour of the human arm is an auspicious field of research, and leads to what we call biologicallyinspired robotics. By measuring and subsequently analysing human arm impedance parameters, we can attempt to extract general rules and project these to the robotic domain. The only direct method to measure stiffness in a functioning feedback system is to apply external force perturbations to the limb and to measure the resulting displacements; such measurements have only been satisfactorily realised in planar (2D) movements. To date, no fully satisfactory method exists to investigate the time-varying impedance during movements. Early efforts were subject to error because they assume that subjects perform the same movement on repeated trials and they ignore the non-linear properties of the musculo-skeletal system. The human arm's capability to alter its impedance has motivated multiple developments of robotic manipulators and control methods. It provides advantages during manipulation such as robustness against external disturbances and task adaptability. However, how the impedance of the arm is set depends on the manipulation situation; a general procedure is lacking. We provide a method to identify human arm impedance in more than 2 degrees of freedom. We do this by initially identifying the kinematic and inertial parameters of the arm through movement. Subsequently we identify stiffness parameters of the human arm in 5 degrees of freedom (shoulder, elbow, and lower arm rotation), while taking the numerical stability of the data into account. Confidence criteria to determine the accuracy of the estimated parameters are given. The data are related to a representation of the stiffness by EMG which, in 13 Poster Sessions Full Abstracts combination with the kinematics, gives us a 3D Cartesian identification of the impedance parameters of the human arm. 1-B-92 Simulated hemianopia drives eyes to distraction Liana Brown1, Carina La Mantia1 1 Trent University We used gaze contingent displays to explore how people adapt their visual exploration and reaching behaviour to simulated homonymous hemianopia (SH). Participants were asked to search for and point to a target displayed with distractors, and gaze location at reach onset was measured. Previously, we found that participants tended to look short of blind-field targets and reach blind. This study revealed that participants no longer pointed blind when good-field distractors were removed or when a hand was placed in the blind-field target to anchor gaze there. Partial visual loss leaves people vulnerable to distractors, but this can be reduced somewhat by placing one's hand in the blind field. 1-B-93 Different brain pathways for strategic control versus sensorimotor recalibration: Evidence from a dual task reach paradigm Joshua Granek1, Lauren E Sergio1 1 York University We have previously demonstrated that patients with optic ataxia (OA), having damage to their superior parietal lobules, rely on explicit strategic control when reaching under a 90° cursor feedback rotation condition (i.e. non-standard, "decoupled" reaching). We observed that OA patients displayed improved performance when moving towards ordinal (on-axis) as opposed to oblique (off-axis) visual targets. We assume that for these rotated cursor feedback situations a) oblique targets require an implicit realignment between proprioception and vision, referred to as sensorimotor recalibration, while b) ordinal targets rely on the use of explicit rule integration, referred to as strategic control. Our OA patient data suggests that the network used for sensorimotor recalibration is to some extent independent of that used for strategic control. This observation motivates the general question: Do neurologically intact adults have difficulty with non-standard reaching when strategic control, but not sensorimotor recalibration, has been interfered with? If so, this would support the notion of independent brain pathways for the processing of these different types of visuomotor mappings. Here, we trained healthy adults on visually-guided reaching with both veridical and rotated (90° clockwise rotation) cursor feedback, while simultaneously performing an attentionally- demanding task (backwards counting by different amounts). We tested the hypothesis that different pathways are used for these two classes of movement control by increasing the neural load associated with explicit rule integration, but not implicit visual-proprioceptive alignment. We predicted that performance in the dual task would decline when making a non-standard reach towards ordinal targets (where explicit rules are more useful) relative to oblique targets (relying on sensorimotor recalibration). Overall, simultaneous performance of the two tasks had detrimental effects on hand movement timing (increased movement timing), accuracy (increased constant error) and precision (increased variable error). Importantly, the time to prepare the reaches having rotated visual feedback (reaction time) increased when performing the dual task towards only the ordinal targets, relative to controls. Similarly, subjects' trajectories for reaches having rotated feedback were longer (increased path length) and were initiated towards the wrong direction (increased angle at peak velocity) during the dual task towards the ordinal targets, while they remained unaffected relative to the controls towards the oblique targets. These observations support our 14 assumption that certain types of non-standard reaches rely on explicit strategic control, while others rely on implicit sensorimotor recalibration. Our results suggest that independent neural pathways may underlie the control of these different types of reach, since one class of movement was interfered with while the other was not. 1-B-94 Separating standard and non-standard reaches: Topographical differences within PMd Patricia Sayegh1, Kara M Hawkins1, Lauren E Sergio1 1 York University It is well established that reaching movements rely on a network of brain regions including the dorsal premotor (PMd) and superior parietal lobule (SPL), which are regions located within the dorsomedial parieto-frontal network. However, what is less understood is how the regions within this network are modulated during a reaching movement when there is a dissociation between the action of the eye and the hand, termed a non-standard movement. There is a large body of evidence to suggest that the action of the eye and hand are tightly linked (1-3), that decoupled eye-hand coordination is not innate(4), and that the accuracy and movement profile of a non-standard reaches are altered when compared to standard reaches (5). Additionally, patients with neurological disorders demonstrate deteriorated reaching performance on non-standard tasks while leaving reaching performance on standard tasks unaffected (6). This suggests that during non-standard reaching movements, a specific set of processes must occur in order to break the tight linkage between the eye and the hand. As a result, non-standard reaching movements likely depend on neural circuitry that is different albeit interconnected with the circuitry important for controlling natural (standard) reaching movements (7). We recorded the local field potential (LFP) within PMdr and PMdc during the planning phase of two types of visually-guided reach movements. During the standard condition, a visually guided reach was performed in which the visual stimulus guiding the movement was the target of the reach itself. During the non-standard condition, the visual stimulus provided information about the direction of a required movement, but was not the target of the motor output. We observed distinct task related differences as well as topographical differences between PMdr and PMdc. Our results support functional differences between PMdr and PMdc during visually-guided reaching. PMdr activity appears more involved in integrating the rule-based aspects of a visually-guided reach, while PMdc is more involved in movement related activity. More broadly, our results highlight the necessity of accounting for the non-standard nature of a motor task when interpreting movement control research data.1. Henriques, et al. J. Neurosci. (1998). 2.Gorbet, L. E. Sergio. Brain Res. (2009). 3. Neggers et al. J. Neurophysiol. (2000). 4. Bo, et al. Hum. Mov. Sci. (2006). 5. Messier, et al. Exp. Brain Res. (1997). 6. Tippett, L. E. Sergio. Brain Res. (2006). 7. Clavagnier, et al. Neuroscientist (2007). 1-B-95 Mapping the spatio-temporal structure of motor cortical LFP and spiking activity during reach and grasp movements Alexa Riehle1, Thomas Brochier1 1 CNRS Grasping an object involves shaping the hand and fingers in relation to the object's physical properties. Following object contact, it also requires a fine adjustment of grasp forces for secure manipulation. Earlier studies suggest that the control Poster Sessions Full Abstracts of hand shaping and grasp force involve partially segregated motor cortical networks both during preparation and execution. However, it is still unclear how information originating from these networks is integrated over motor cortical areas. We addressed this issue by analyzing massively parallel activity of LFPs and single neurons recorded in primary motor (MI) and dorsal premotor (PMd) cortices of macaque monkeys performing a delayed reach to grasp task. They are trained to grasp and pull an object using either a Side Grip or a Precision Grip. The object is either heavy or light. The trial starts by the monkey pressing a switch. A first cue provides the instruction about the grip (object load). After a delay of 1s, a second cue provides additional information about the object load (grip) and serves as GO signal. The monkey has then to release the switch, grip the object, pull it and hold it in a narrow position window for 500ms to receive a food reward. Neuronal activity was recorded in two monkeys by using a 100 electrode "Utah" array, chronically implanted at the MI/PMd border. In motor cortical areas, LFPs exhibit a large multicomponent movement-related potential (MRP) around movement onset. However, little is known about its spatio-temporal distribution across motor cortex in relation to task requirements. Our data show that MRP amplitude and shape depend both on grip type and electrode position. The strength and the generation of each of the 5 individual MRP components depend on the location on the cortical surface. The first two components, P1 occurring just before and N1 around movement onset, were related to reaching and generated in the proximal representation, towards the precentral dimple, whereas the later components, P2, N2 and P3, were closely related to grasping and generated in the distal representation, the most posterior part of the array towards the central sulcus. Single neuron activity related to the four trial types was analyzed during movement preparation and execution. During both periods, a much larger proportion of neurons was selective to the grip than to the object load. During movement preparation, the highest amount of selective grip- and forcerelated activity was found in PMd, and the smallest amount in the proximal representation of MI. Grip-related activity during movement execution was widely distributed over the array, but highest at the location of distal representation, thus overlapping with that of the late MRP components of the LFP. A close matching was observed between the distribution of distal somatosensory receptive fields on hand and fingers and the distribution of grip-related activity of both LFP and single neurons. These data show that in motor cortex, a precise spatio-temporal pattern of activation is involved for the control and hand position and force during the preparation and execution of reach to grasp movements. These data provide some new insight about the functional organization of motor cortex for the control of hand grip and contact forces during object grasp and manipulation. Funding: Collaborative Research Agreement Riken-CNRS, CNRS (PEPS, Neuro_IC2010) 1-B-96 Reach to grasp movements: A combined EEG and kinematic study Teresa De Sanctis1 1 Università degli studi di Padova Introduction: Reach-to-grasp movements have been widely investigated in both humans and monkeys with a variety of tasks and techniques (Filimon, 2010; Grafton, 2010). These studies aim to integrate information from various domains to reveal the neural circuits that underlie reach-to-grasp movement. The fast development of techniques might allow researchers to investigate how the various grasping related areas in the human brain act in concert (Castiello & Begliomini, 2008). To address this issue here we co-register electrical and kinematic signals with the intent to ascertain how and whether they show some form of correlation The combination of different techniques offers the unique opportunity to understand more fully the processes underlying reach-to-grasp movements. Methods: Participants. Fourteen right-handed participants (9 female; age 19-27 years old) participated in the study. Apparatus. The electroencephalographic activity was recorded by means of a 30-channel set up placed on the scalp according to the international 10-20 system. Movement kinematics was recorded by means of a 3D motion analysis system (SMART-D, BTS Bio Engineering). Stimuli. Two spherical wooden objects (large object, 7 cm diameter; small object, 3 cm diameter), placed at 30 cm along the participants' mid sagittal plane. Procedures. Participants were instructed to reach toward and grasp the object. The action started at the time the stimulus became visually available. Vision was controlled by means of liquid crystal goggles worn by participants. There were two experimental conditions: reach-to-grasp towards the small object, and reach-to-grasp toward the large object. Forty trials per condition were administered in a semi-random order. Data analysis. An analysis of variance with type of stimulus (small, large) as within-subject factor was conducted for several dependent measures. For the EEG measures, we considered amplitude and latency of eventrelated potentials (ERP) for each channel. For the kinematics we considered key reach-to-grasp kinematic landmarks. They were time to (i) peak velocity; (ii) peak acceleration; and (iii) peak deceleration for the reaching component. And, the time of maximum grip aperture (the time at which the maximum distance for the markers placed upon the thumb and index finger occurred) for the grasping component. Furthermore, ERP components were correlated with kinematic landmarks. Results: Object size affected both EEG and kinematic signals. The ERP component related to movement planning was evident as a negative deflection frontally distributed, and was differentially modulated in amplitude and latency. At parietal sites, a sustained positive ERP variation was found, likely related to visuo-spatial processing. Both these components were evoked by the object appearance. As expected, there were kinematic differences depending on stimulus size. Concerning the approach phase, time to peak velocity occurred earlier and the time to peak deceleration occurred later for the small than for the large stimulus. For the grasping component, the time of maximum grip aperture occurred earlier for the small than for the large stimulus. ERP components and temporal kinematic events correlated depending on object size. Conclusions: These findings provide the first evidence of how kinematic landmarks characterizing reach-to-grasp movements are synchronously related to electrical neural activity recorded within the fronto-parietal network. 1-B-97 On the relationship between spiking activity and low-frequency local field potentials in primate motor cortex Thomas Hall1, Kianoush Nazarpour1, Andrew Jackson1 1 University of Newcastle-upon-Tyne, UK There is increasing interest in the low-frequency (0.5 to 5 Hz or 'delta band') components of the local field potential (LFP) in motor cortex. Recent studies have demonstrated that movement-relevant information can be decoded from these frequencies, with application in brain-machine interfaces for restoring motor function. However it is not yet clear how these LFP components relate to the spiking activity of neurons in the motor networks. 15 Poster Sessions Full Abstracts We have recorded simultaneous spikes and LFPs using chronic intracortical electrode arrays in the primary motor (M1) and ventral premotor (PMv) areas of awake macaque monkeys performing wrist torque-tracking and brain-machine interface tasks. Spike-triggered averages (STAs) of LFPs exhibited lowfrequency (1 to 2 Hz) features that have not previously been well described. These features have a remarkably consistent morphology in two subjects across a variety of task configurations, consisting usually of a biphasic potential with initial negativity at latencies between -200 and 200ms, followed a positivity between 100 and 500ms. Unlike higher frequencies in the beta band, these low-frequency features do not correspond to precise phase-locking of individual spikes to a global rhythm. By cross-correlating event-related potentials with peri-event time histograms, we further showed that the spike-field relationship is not explained by covariation with motor- or sensory-evoked potentials but instead reflects slow, stochastic fluctuations in spike rate and LFP. distance as fast as possible without missing and sustain the cursor in the target for 0.5 s. The relationship between movement time and target difficulty was calculated. Fitts Law predicts that movement time will be linear in the log of the target difficulty. Preliminary results show that subjects are able to adapt to the task quickly, and their movement times after practice are well described by Fitts' Law. In ongoing work we will compare the calculated index of performance for myocontrol with the index of performance for a similar task based on position or force control of the index finger. 1-B-99 When to move and how to move: Information about the movement type and the timing of movement are concurrently processed in the human motor system Nobuhiro Hagura1, Yosuke Goto2, Michikazu Matsumura2 1 We studied the localisation of spike-related low-frequency LFP components by comparing STAs for spikes and LFPs recorded on the same electrode, pairs of electrodes within the same cortical area and across different areas. In general, the amplitude of lowfrequency features tended to reduce with increasing electrode separation, suggesting they reflect local sources. Moreover the shape and latency of features in STAs triggered by the same cell differed significantly for LFPs across and, to some extent, within areas suggesting multiple, distributed sources may correlate with the recorded neuron. Using a double-spike triggered averaging technique with pairs of simultaneously recorded neurons, we found that each cell contributes an independent component to the LFP at a third site. Extending this to multiple units, we attempted to predict the low-frequency LFP from a convolution of each spike train with a unique kernel. We found that the LFP was welldescribed by a linear sum of individual contributions, with the goodness of fit improving with increasing numbers of units. The LFP is often conceived as a global phenomenon that drives synchronous firing of broad neuronal populations. By contrast, we hypothesise that the low-frequency LFP is the summation of contributions from a large number of relatively independent sources, each reflecting the correlated firing of a local or distributed neuronal ensemble. Because multiple electrodes will sample a slightly different combination of sources, this may explain why simultaneous LFP recordings can be decoded to yield a wealth of movement-related information at these frequencies. 1-B-98 Nonlinear EMG estimation as a control signal in a one-muscle myocontrol task Adam Feinman1, Terence D Sanger1 1 University of Southern California Nonlinear filtering of surface EMG (sEMG), using a recursive Bayesian method sidesteps some of the issues in using sEMG as a control signal, such as the trade-off between smoothing and signal delays. The use of nonlinear filters raises the possibility of using myocontrol for precision tasks for which sEMG has traditionally been considered too noisy. In order to quantify the control abilities of sEMG in terms of both precision and speed of responses, we test using a Fitts Law paradigm and calculate the index of performance for myocontrol. We have implemented this methodology in a simple one-muscle myocontrol task to examine a person's ability to isolate control of one muscle and to determine the bandwidth of the control signal. Subjects were asked to control the FDI muscle of their dominant hand with visual feedback of filtered sEMG. Vertical position of a bar cursor was proportional to the level of contraction of the FDI. Subjects were instructed to move the cursor into targets of varying size and 16 University College London, 2Kyoto University On the tennis court, when waiting for the opponent to serve, you have to prepare for a movement by taking 1) the kind of movement type (forehand or backhand), and 2) the timing of movement initiation (when to swing), into account. Intuitively, the timing of movement initiation cannot be prepared before deciding what kind of movement to make. Here, we show that two information are concurrently processed during motor preparation, both giving benefit for efficient action preparation by each independently modulating the motor system. 14 subjects participated in the study. In a trial, an arrow indicating either left of right was presented on a monitor (GO signal). Subjects made flexion movement of their right wrist for the left arrow and extended for the right in a ballistic manner. In each block, the presentation ratio of left and right arrows was changed (7:1, 3:1, 1:1, 1:3, 1:7), enabling the subjects to estimate which movement (flexion or extension) is more likely to be performed during that block. In each block, for half of the trials, a warning signal was presented 500ms before the GO signal. This allowed the subjects to predict the initiation timing of the movement. To evaluate the cortico-spinal excitability during motor preparation, single pulse transcranial magnetic stimulation (TMS) was applied to the left primary motor cortex 50~500ms before the GO signal, and motor evoked potentials (MEP) were measured from flexor carpi radialis (FCR) and extensor carpi radialis (ECR). Reaction time (RT) was reduced both by information about the movement type and the movement timing, without showing any effect of interaction. This indicated that two information independently acted on the motor system for efficient motor preparation. This behavioural pattern of independent information processing was also observed as modulation of MEP. Overall amplitude increased depending on the probability of the movement type (effect of movement type), but on top of that, reduction of MEP towards the GO signal was observed when the warning signal was presented (effect of movement timing). Critically, both component of MEP modulation directly explained the variance of RT; reduction of individual RTs were able to be explained by the increase in MEPs by the probability of movement, and also by the amount of reduction of MEP due to the warning signal. Our result of increased MEPs depending on the probability of movement resembles with the notion that selection of movement is achieved through competition of neuronal firing that can be biased by the probability of future movements. Also, as in the present study, MEP, including the excitability of spinal motoneurons, is reported to reduce when the warning signal is presented, suggested to function for increasing the sensitivity to the Poster Sessions Full Abstracts descending motor command at the critical timing. We propose that the information about movement types and movement timings are concurrently processed, possibly in different brain regions, acting on the cortico-spinal pathway from the motor cortex at different stages. Even when you are not sure which swing to make, we can still prepare for the timing of the swing. reward, TMS and compatibility on the independent variables reaction time and accuracy were tested in a 2 × 2 × 2 repeated measures analysis of variance (ANOVA). Accuracy rates were analyzed separately for fast and slow responses after a median split to assess the effects of rTMS on premature incorrect responses. 1-B-100 Hand-related sensory-motor activity in secondary somatosensory cortex of the macaque monkey Results: In reward trials, participants significantly increased reaction speed compared to neutral trials (F(1,13) = 16.65, p = 0.001). For trials following an incompatible trial, fast responses were more error-prone than for trials following a compatible trial (F(1,13) = 8.321, p = 0.013). Real rTMS over pre-SMA reduced subjects' tendency to make such premature incorrect responses, but only in the context of reward (F(1,13) = 4.711, p = 0.049). Laura Grandi1, Hiroaki Ishida2, Luca Fornia1, Vittorio Gallese2 1 University of Parma, 2Italian Institute of Technology (IIT), Unit Parma Dexterous use of the hand requires continuous update of somatosensory input to the motor system. Both in humans and in non-human primates, the secondary somatosensory cortex (SII) has been implicated in sensory-motor integration during active touch. However, physiological properties of reaching-grasping movements in this area remain largely unclear. Here we investigated whether, and to what extent, sensory and visual information of target can integrate at the single neuron level in the posterior SII of non-human primate. To explore this issue, two monkeys were trained to perform a reaching-to-grasping task, which required three different hand prehension types (grasping objects from a grove, a cup, a plate) tested both in light and dark conditions. Our results showed that within SII hand representation region, although most neurons responded to passive somatosensory stimulation and during the motor task, a consistent percentage of them was only activated during monkeys' active hand grasping/manipulation. Approximately half of them was active before the onset of object contact. Furthermore, few neurons showed stronger activation during manipulation in the dark in comparison with that performed under visual control. The present findings show the existence of grasping- and manipulation-related neurons in SII. This suggests that neuronal activity of SII hand region likely contributes to hand motor control both in the initial and finale phases of grasping and object manipulation motor acts. Such motor-related activity could perhaps also provide the sensory-motor binding principal enabling the translation of diachronic somatosensory inputs into a coherent image of the explored object. 1-B-101 Improved control over impulsive but inappropriate response tendencies following 1 Hz repetitive TMS of pre-supplementary motor area Damian Herz1, Hartwig R Siebner1, Mark S Christensen1, Norbert Bruggemann1, Brian N Haagensen1, Kristoffer H Madsen1 1 Copenhagen Unversity Hospital Hvidovre Background: In choice-reaction tasks, participants respond slower and less accurately to an arbitrary cue when the position of the cue and the response are spatially incongruent. This response interference caused by spatial incongruency between cue and response is flexibly modulated by the compatibility between consecutive trials: Responses are slower and less accurate if an incongruent trial follows a congruent trial or a congruent trial follows an incongruent trial. Aim. To study the role of the pre-supplementary motor area (pre-SMA) in the flexible control of response inhibition. Methods: On two sessions, 14 young healthy participants received either 1 Hz repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation (rTMS) to induce a lasting suppression of cortical excitability in the pre-SMA or sham stimulation. Following rTMS, participants performed a Simon task in which two arbitrary cues prompted spatially incompatible or compatible responses. To increase the motivation to respond, participants were rewarded in 50% of the trials in case of accurate and fast responses. Main effects of Conclusion: Suppressing cortical excitability in pre-SMA leads to a context-dependent improvement in interference control by enhancing inhibition of premature response tendencies. This finding suggests a critical role of pre-SMA in flexible response inhibition. 1-B-102 Intra-hemispheric parietal-motor paired associative stimulation induces bidirectional plasticity Giacomo Koch1, Domenica Veniero1, Francesco Di Lorenzo1, Carlo Caltagirone1 1 Santa Lucia Foundation According to the Hebbian rule, synapses increase their efficacy if the synapse consistently assists the postsynaptic target neuron to generate action potentials. This type of plasticity is referred to as spike-timing-dependent plasticity (STDP). STDP-like plasticity can be induced in the intact human cortex using paired associative stimulation (PAS). Conventional PAS protocols consistently pairs electrical stimulation of the median nerve with TMS of the contralateral primary motor or sensory cortex (Stefan et al. 2000). Here we sought to investigate whether PAS of intrahemispheric cortico-cortical connections between the posterior parietal cortex (PPC) and the primary motor cortex (M1) may induce plastic changes in the excitability of M1. Ten healthy subjects were enrolled. To activate the ipsilateral PPC-M1 connection within the left hemisphere, a conditioning stimulus (CS) was applied over the PPC at an intensity of 90% of the ipsilateral resting motor threshold (RMT) (Koch et al., 2007, 2008). The CS preceded or followed the M1 test stimulus (TS) by 5-50 ms. In each protocol (PAS-5 and PAS+5) one hundred pairs of stimuli were continuously delivered at a rate of 0.2 Hz for 8 min. Each pair of stimuli consisted of a monophasic stimulus given to the left PPC followed by another monophasic stimulus given to the left M1. In a second set of experiments we also evaluated the changes occurring in intracortical circuits over the target M1 testing both short intracortical inhibition (SICI) at 2 and 3 ms ISI and intracortical facilitation (ICF) at 15 ms ISI using the standard paired pulse protocol (Kujirai et al., 1993). We found that intra-hemispheric PPCM1 PAS was able to induce bidirectional long lasting plastic changes in the excitability of the target M1. When the PPC TMS preceded the M1 TS by 5 ms we found evidence for a long lasting inhibition of the MEP that lasted for up to 20 minutes after the end of the protocol (ANOVA with TIME as main factor (F(5,45)=3.81; p=0.009), implicating a long-term depression (LTD)-like phenomenon. On the other hand, When the PPC TMS followed the M1 TS by 5 ms we found evidence for a long lasting increase of the MEP that lasted for up to 25 minutes after the end of the protocol (ANOVA with TIME as main factor (F(5,45)=2.70; p=0.021), 17 Poster Sessions Full Abstracts implicating a long-term potentiation (LTP)-like phenomenon. These data demonstrate the possibility to explore bidirectional hebbian-like plasticity between cortical areas of the same hemisphere. The current protocol could be important to study plastic mechanisms related to motor learning and to develop specific rehabilitative approaches 1-B-103 UCM analysis of human multi-joint coordination and mechanical arm impedance Shunta Togo1, Takahiro Kagawa1, Yoji Uno1 1 Nagoya University Humans can naturally perform many types of dexterous tasks. It is considered that the humans coordinate their redundant multijoints to achieve the dexterous task. We investigated a human dexterous task "carrying a cup with water without spilling it". This task requires the individual to dampen hand vibration while walking, and in generally, the human can naturally perform it. Moreover, this simply task has an interesting problem of human control e.g. redundant whole body movement, control of mechanical impedance of an arm. We hypothesize that the human reduces the hand jerk (the rate of change of acceleration) and maintains the cup angle constant by multi-joint coordination to achieve the task. We conducted an experiment in which human subjects walked on a treadmill holding a cup with water (WW task) and with stones (WS task). Their kinematics data were measured using a three-dimensional position measurement system. We empirically confirmed that the value of the hand jerk and the variance of the cup angle in the WW task were smaller than those in the WS task. The jerks of some body parts were gradually dampened from a hip to a hand, indicating that the jerk might be dampened by use of redundant multi-joints. To quantify the multi-joint coordinated movements, we used UCM (UnControlled Manifold) analysis. This method divided joint variance into two orthogonal components: UCM component which does not affect the hand jerk and the cup angle, and ORT component which directly affects them. Using these components, we defined the index of hand jerk coordination and that of cup angle coordination. According to these indices in the WW task, we found that the human reduces hand jerk and maintains the cup angle constant by multi-joint coordination. To consider the mechanism of coordinated movements especially the mechanism of reducing hand jerk, we focused on the mechanical impedance of the human arm. We considered the two-link arm with joint stiffness and viscosity, and simulated human walking by moving the origin of the two link arm. In the simulation, we changed the value of joint stiffness and viscosity, and calculated the hand jerk. We found that the smaller value of joint stiffness or the larger value of joint viscosity was assigned, the smaller hand jerk was appeared. To qualitatively confirm these results, we conducted an experiment in which subject's hand was perturbed while carrying a cup. In this experiment, subjects performed the WW task and the WS task, and a held cup was quickly perturbed by the tension in the almost same range. We measured subject's kinematic data and strength of the perturbation. Our results showed that the displacement of the hand in the WW task was larger than that in the WS task. These results suggest that the human recovers the effect of the perturbation by changing the characteristics of mechanical impedance of the arm. This suggestion also indicates that the human reduces hand jerk by controlling their mechanical impedance of the arm. 1-B-104 Two hands, one perception: Bimanual integration of haptic information 1 1 1 Valentina Squeri , Alessandra Sciutti , Monica Gori , Lorenzo Masia1, Giulio Sandini1, Juergen Konczak2 18 1 Universitá degli Studi di Genova - DIST-IIT, 2University of Minnesota Introduction: Humans routinely use their hands to haptically explore objects in the environment. In many cases, both hands are used to gain information about the properties of the object. However we know very little about how the brain combines the haptic information of our two hands to achieve a single percept of an object and about the underlying mechanism of how the nervous system integrates or fuses information from two haptic systems. This study systematically measured the haptic precision of humans exploring a virtual curved object contour with one or both hands to understand if the brain integrates haptic information from the two hemispheres. Evidence on unimanual visuo-haptic integration showed that vision and the haptic information from one hand are integrated optimally. The extension of the Bayesian perception theory would predict that redundant information from both hands should improve haptic estimates. Method: Twenty right-handed adult participated to this study. A bimanual robotic manipulandum passively moved the hands of the blindfolded subjects along virtual curved contours. Passive movements constrained by the robot assured that both hands performed simultaneous and synchronous displacements with the same speed profile. Thus, any possible differences in perceptual sensitivity between unimanual and bimanual conditions cannot be explained by differences in the motor performance of the two hands. In a single trial two different contours were presented: a standard (constant) and a comparison (variable) stimulus. Within each trial, the order of presentation of the two stimuli was random. After probing both contours, subjects indicated which contour was more "curved" (forced choice). Participants explored contours in 4 unimanual and 6 bimanual conditions (left, right, both hands coupled; both hands moving independently). Respective psychophysical discrimination thresholds were computed. Results: Analysis of the unimanual left and right hand exploration showed a tendency for one hand being more sensitive than the other with most of the subjects exhibiting a non-dominant (left) hand bias. Bimanual thresholds were mostly within the range of the corresponding unimanual thresholds and were not predicted by a maximum-likelihood estimation model. Moreover, during bimanual exploration perception tended to be biased towards the motorically dominant, right hand, not towards the haptically more sensitive left hand. Conclusion: Two-handed exploration does not necessarily improve haptic sensitivity. We found no evidence that haptic information from both hands is integrated using a maximumlikelihood estimation mechanism. Rather, results are indicative of a process of "sensory selection", where information from the dominant, right hand is used, although the left non-dominant hand may yield more precise haptic estimates. 1-B-105 Predicting sensory consequences of intended movements in monkey posterior parietal cortex Alexander Gail1, Shenbing Kuang1 1 German Primate Center The ability to predict the sensory outcomes of our own actions is considered essential, since it allows to compensate for system delays during adaptive motor control. While clinical and neuropsychophysical studies Poster Sessions Full Abstracts suggest that the posterior parietal cortex (PPC) of primates could be involved in such forward predictions, convincing direct neurophysiological evidence of predictive signals in PPC is still lacking. During movement execution such evidence is also very difficult to provide, since feed-forward motor commands, sensory feedback about the movement, and putative efferent copy or forward prediction signals change dynamically and co-vary substantially. This almost unavoidably leads to various potential confounds when trying to interpret neural data in highly integrative sensorimotor areas during movement execution. Beyond immediate motor control, the encoding of anticipated sensory effects of an intended movement, prior to execution, has been postulated as a possible mechanism of action selection and planning. Here we directly test this hypothesis for neurons in monkey PPC. We designed a novel delayed and memory-guided reach tasks in which monkeys had to perform either pro or anti reaches in either normal viewing conditions or under reversed vision through inverting prisms. With this task we can double-dissociate the spatial information contained in the sensory instruction cue, the intended physical movement, and the predicted visual feedback about the movement. The pro vs. anti comparisons dissociated the visual memory of the cue from the movement intention ('motor goal'), in both the normal viewing context and the prism viewing context. The prism vs. non-prism comparisons dissociated the physical intention from the visual prediction in the pro and anti contexts. Especially, the combined prism and anti-reach task created situations where the sensory cue and physical reach directions were identical, but the visual predictions were opposite. About one fourth of the neurons in PPC which were movement related and directionally selective in the prism and no-prism condition encoded the direction of the anticipated visual feedback, rather than the intended physical arm movement direction. The remaining neurons encoded the physical direction of planned movement. Our results provide direct evidence for the notion that motor planning not only evokes neuronal representations linked to the planned action per se, but also to its anticipated perceivable sensory consequences. These observed predictive representations establish a role of forward model prediction in the parietal cortex, even before a motor command is issued, as an integral part of the planning and probably also selection process for movements, one of the distinguishing features of the ideomotor principle. This idea dates back to the early 19th century and is prominent until today in research of goal-directed behavior, not least since it tightly relates to the ecological approach of perception, motor awareness, the mirror-neuron system, and modern concepts of neural adaptive motor control. C - Control of Eye & Head Movement 1-C-31 Where cats look during walking Irina Beloozerova1, Trevor J Rivers1, Mikhail G Sirota1, Neet A Shah1, Andrew I Guttentag1, Dmitri A Ogorodnikov2 1 Barrow Neurological Institute, 2Mount Sinai School of Medicine Vision is important for locomotion, particularly when an individual is moving through complex natural environments. How and what visual information is collected during locomotion in complex environments is far from being understood, however. In this study we used freely walking cats. Cats walked in five different conditions: (1) on a flat surface in complete darkness, (2) on flat surface in the light, (3) on a horizontal ladder with crosspiece of different widths (5, 12, 18 cm), (4) on a pathway cluttered with many small stones, and (5) overstepping different series of thin 7 cm high obstacles. We recorded cat's eye movement using a scleral coil and headmounted magnetic field generator, and head and limbs movement using an active-marker 3-dimensional tracking system. We calculated cat's gaze, determined where it intercepts walking surface, and compared gazing behaviors and coordination of gaze with stepping movements across different walking conditions. We found that as the density of obstructions along the pathway increases, cats look closer, fixate more, and also shift gaze more often. In agreement with previous studies in cats, we found that cats make an overwhelming majority of gaze shifts two strides prior to stepping on the site looked at. There is a strong correlation between vertical gaze shifts and stride phase. A typical stride has two gaze shifts. Gaze shifts ending further away from the cat occur near the end of the stance phase of either forelimb (peaking 200 ms before the onset of swing phase), while gaze shifts ending closer to the cat, occur near the beginning of the either forelimb swing (peaking 100-200 ms after the onset of swing). In addition to correlating with stepping movements, vertical gaze shifts also correlate with head pitch rotation. While overstepping barriers, cats tend to look at the top edge, but not at the center or the bottom edge of the barriers. On the ladder, cats' gaze is typically directed at the front or back edge of the crosspiece, regardless of the crosspiece width, and they rarely look either at the center of a crosspiece or at the space between rungs. Thus, we found that for successful stepping in complex environments cats as people are more concerned with obtaining visual information about the boundaries within which they can step, rather than with examining the exact spots of future foot placement with their central vision. Thanks to the use of scleral coil and computerized 3 dimensional head tracking techniques, this study provides the first accurate numeric characterization of cat gazing behavior during locomotion and sets a stage for analysis of neuronal mechanisms for processing of visual information during locomotion. 1-C-32 Robotic models for neural control of eye movements Dinesh Pai1, Martin Lesmana1, Per-Erik Forssén2 1 University of British Columbia, 2Linköping University Theoretical models of neural control are essential for developing insights into physiological experiments but it is difficult to test whether these models are actually effective for control, and not just for describing the data. Here we describe an approach which uses robotic models of the oculomotor control system to investigate theories of neural control of movement. We focus on the oculomotor system as it is a relatively simple yet complete sensorimotor system, with a wealth of neurophysiological data. In particular, we look at two types of eye movements: saccadic gaze shifts and gaze stabilization (with VOR and OKR). Saccades are too fast to control using the usual engineering approach of relying on online sensory feedback. This stems from the delays inherent in the nervous system; a saccade of less than 10 degrees can be completed in less than 50 milliseconds, while low level visual processing alone takes 40 milliseconds. To achieve fast movements in the face of large delays, we developed a biologically inspired controller that learns a non-linear dynamic model of the eye and predicts the required pulse-step activation signal. We tested 19 Poster Sessions Full Abstracts the controller on a robotic eye with complicated non-linearities (similar to those due to orbital tissues) and demonstrate saccade performance comparable to that observed in the human eye. Interestingly, the controller also reproduces the characteristic the main sequence relationship. Gaze stabilization enables clear vision during locomotion by moving the eyes to compensate for head movements. This is achieved through combining head movement information measured by the fast vestibular system and the slower visual information from the eye. We develop a gaze stabilization controller that uses vestibular input as the primary input while visual error is used as a teaching signal and to prevent drift. We implemented the controller on a robotic head mounted on a high speed robotic testing platform. The ''head'' contains the equivalent of an eye (actuated camera) and the vestibular system (inertial measurement unit). We demonstrate the controller's effectiveness in stabilizing the camera to high speed perturbations. 1-C-57 Smooth pursuit eye movement (SPEM) in patients with idiopathic Parkinson's disease (PD): Movement preparation and execution is impaired but not visual motion working memory exhibited initial pursuit (latency 128±19SD ms) followed by corrective saccades (212±56SD ms), and latencies of these components were similar to those of controls (p>0.6). Postsaccadic SPEM enhancement was also seen in most PD (13/16). (4) In both tasks, peak SPEM velocity during pursuit maintenance was lower in PD than controls (p<0.01). Discussion: Since initial SPEM of most PD during task B was basically similar, though reduced, to that of controls, the initial SPEM difficulty during task A was unlikely to reflect impaired SPEM per se; possibly it reflects difficulty in inducing priming effects using cue information (Fukushima et al. 2011) and/or generating extra retinal components for SPEM (Barnes and Collins 2008). The difficulty in the latter may also be involved in impairing pursuit maintenance. Difficulty in terminating fixation may also contribute to the prolonged latency of the first saccades during memory pursuit (Warabi et al. 2011). Conclusion: The working memory of motion-direction and go/no-go selection was normal in PD but they had difficulty in initiating SPEM using the cue information. Norie Ito1, Nozomi Tamaki1, Ayumi Masuno1, Kunihiro Ikeno1, Sachiyo Onishi1, Nobuyoshi Kobayashi1, Hidetoshi Takei1, Peter M Olley1, Susumu Chiba1, Kiyoharu Inoue1, Grahamu Barnes2, Kikuro Fukushima1, Tateo Warabi1 1-C-58 Oculomotor adaptation of sound localization depends upon the temporal relationship between targets and eye movements 1 Gary Paige1, Emily C Clark1, William O'Neill1 Sapporo Yamanoue Hospital, 2University of Manchester 1 Purpose: To examine whether the impaired SPEM in PD includes impaired visual motion working memory and/or movement preparation. Background: Clear vision of slowly moving objects depends on SPEM during which target images are maintained on the fovea by predictive compensation of the inherent delays in responding to target motion. Various factors influence prediction such as cues and working memory of stimulus trajectory (Barnes 2008 for review). Prediction could occur not only in motor commands to prepare for and maintain ongoing movements but also in the sensory and/or perception pathways. A memory-based smooth pursuit task separates neuronal activity related to visual motiondirection memory from movement preparation (e.g. Shichinohe et al. 2009). SPEM is impaired in most patients with PD (Leigh and Zee 2006 for review), though the nature of the impairment is poorly understood. Impaired working memory during cognitive tasks has also been reported in PD (e.g. Lee et al. 2010). Methods: We applied this task (task A) to 16 PD patients (Hoehn and Yahr stage 2.6±0.6SD) and 7 age-matched controls. In task A, cue 1 indicated visual motion-direction (rightward or leftward) while cue 2 instructed the subject to prepare to pursue (go) or not to pursue (no-go). Based on their motion direction memory and go/no-go instruction, subjects had to select the correct spot from 2 oppositely moving spots at 10°/s and to pursue it or not pursue by selecting a 3rd stationary spot. For comparison, simple ramp pursuit using a single spot with the identical spot velocity (task B) was examined. Infrared oculography was used to record eye movements. Results: (1) In task A, error rates of all PD patients tested during go and no-go trials were low (<5%) and were similar to those of controls (p>0.3). (2) During go trials, controls exhibited SPEM with a mean latency of 158±40SD ms followed by corrective saccades which were further followed by enhanced SPEM. Most PD (13/16) tracked the correct spot predominantly with saccades; SPEM was absent before the saccades, and 9/16 PD lacked postsaccadic SPEM enhancement. The mean latency of the first saccades in PD (326±57SD ms) was longer than that of controls (289±32SD ms, p= 0.02). (3) During task B, most PD (11/16) 20 University of Rochester We have described a unique oculomotor adaptation in which prolonged changes in eye position shift the perception of auditory space in the same direction as ocular deflection (mean magnitude ~40%, time-constant ~1 min.). One consequence is that localization of ongoing sounds overshoots target azimuth when the head is fixed but the eyes are free to guide laser-pointing responses, entailing gaze eccentricity for a few seconds. This overshoot is largely eliminated when the eyes are fixed. However, this distinction disappears for transient targets, whether the eyes are fixed or free to guide localization from memory. Perhaps adaptation is limited to the state of eye position at the time targets are presented. Ongoing targets provide a continuous sampling regardless of eye movements, but transient targets register at a single moment with respect to eye movements. To test this concept, normal human subjects (age <30) localized transient targets (5 noise bursts over 1 s) while fixating a visual reference. Trials began with the eyes centered, followed by sustained eccentric fixation (20° left or right). Subjects waited 10 seconds after the ocular deflection before localizing targets, each presented during one of three different times in the trial: A) eyes fixed centrally, immediately before eccentric fixation, B) immediately after re-fixation eccentrically, or C) after 10 seconds of eccentric fixation. These conditions were compared to a baseline control condition with only center fixation and no delay in localizing targets, and another control in which eye position was held eccentrically for all targets. The key variable is the time between the attainment of new eye position and target presentation. Results for condition A were indistinguishable from baseline. In contrast, a small response overshoot arose in condition B, when targets were presented just after eccentric fixation, which increased for condition C, when targets were presented 10 seconds after eccentric fixation, and proved greatest for the persistent eccentric gaze task. We conclude that oculomotor adaptation of auditory spatial perception depends upon current eye position and duration at the time of transient target presentation, but not subsequent changes Poster Sessions Full Abstracts in eye position. The role of oculomotor adaptation of auditory space presumably reflects a need to establish and maintain a sense of 'straight ahead,' which includes eye, head and body orientation as well as sensory counterparts. A cohesive alignment system, together with co-calibration of spatial gain, secures our concept of space constancy for both spatial offset and gain across modalities. 1-C-59 Comparing models for eye-head trajectory interactions during head-free gaze shifts 1 2 Iman Haji Abolhassani , Daniel Guitton , Henrietta L Galiana Radboud Univ Nijmegen, 2University of Hamburg The gaze (eye-in-space) orientation system uses multiple platforms, i.e., eyes, head, and body, to move gaze in space. Body-fixed head-free gaze shifts, which are the main focus of this study, comprise two phases: the fast phase (saccade) and the slow phase (fixation). A complete head-free gaze shift model needs to include both phases and a mechanism for switching between the two. Two major types of models have been proposed for head free-gaze shifts: the independent eye-head control models and the shared gaze feedback control models. In the former, it is presumed that plant controllers operate in parallel and are independent, whereas, in the shared gaze feedback control models, the updated gaze motor error affects both plants. The model tested here is an updated version of the Prsa-Galiana using shared gaze error feedback. In this study the efficiency of this model is compared to other available popular models for head-free gaze shifts, in comparison to experimental data. It will be shown that our model has prominent advantages to replicate numerous experiments, including: ‐ ‐ ‐ Pieter Medendorp1, Frank T Leone1, Ivan I Toni1, Tobias Heed2 1 McGill University, 2Montreal Neurological Institute ‐ 1-C-60 Testing effector-specificity of human posterior parietal cortex for eye, hand and foot movements: A multivariate analysis 1 1 ‐ neurophysiology regarding their pathways and elements. The switch: The transition between the fast and slow phases of gaze shifts must be consistent with behaviour and with data on OPN/Burst cells, e.g., the fact that the eyes can reach their peak deviation before the end of the gaze saccade (end of OPN pause) and produce a 'VOR-like' turn around during a saccade. In contrast, other models consider the moment of maximum deviation of conjugate eyes as the switching moment for OPNs. Head perturbations & brakes: The gaze-shift system is known to show high robustness to head perturbations both during the fast and slow phases. Passive head rotations during the fast phase especially, have drastic impact on eye and head trajectories. However, their impact on the overall gaze trajectory is far less noticeable. The ability of various models to predict this robustness in gaze shift trajectory will be compared. OPN lesions: In some patients, due to the failure of the mechanism responsible for switching between phases (Burst neuron lesions), the gaze shift system gets stuck in the slow phase. While these patients lose their ability to perform fast saccadic movements, they are still capable of moving their gaze, although in a slower fashion. The ability of the models to replicate this phenomenon is also explored. Vestibular lesions: The predictions of the models in acute vestibular lesions will be compared with experimental results. A scheme will also be proposed for our model to replicate compensated gaze shifts for these cases. Network topology/neurophysiology: Finally, the models will be compared on the basis of consistency with Monkey neurophysiology and human neuroimaging have reported segregated processing in the posterior parietal cortex (PPC) for the planning of goal-directed eye and hand movements. These findings have been interpreted to indicate effector-specific processing for movement planning in the PPC. However, when comparing eye, hand, and foot motor planning in humans using fMRI, our group recently found that processing in the PPC did not differ between hand and foot motor planning, whereas limb processing differed from saccade planning (Heed et al. JNS, 2011). While these results may reflect a shared representation for hand and foot movements, it is also possible that there are separate hand and foot representations, which are hidden to standard methods of analysis. To distinguish between these possibilities, we applied multivariate analysis. Participants were instructed to make delayed eye, right hand, or right foot movements to targets, presented at three different locations on either side of the body midline. Delay times were randomized between 2 and 6 s. In 12 separate runs we acquired multi-echo BOLD-sensitive echo-planar MR images (TR=2.0 s, 26 slices, 3.0x3.0x3.0 mm voxel size) using a Siemens 3-Tesla MRI system. Multiple regression analysis was applied to the unsmoothed data per run, focusing on the delay activation, with separate regressors for each effector-side-repetition combination. The t-values characterizing the respective regressors were used as input for a searchlight classification procedure, which tested local effector-specific content using a leave-one-run-out SVM to each possible sphere with a radius of 2 voxels. The analysis revealed the frontoparietal motor network, with effectorspecific tuning in PPC, precuneus, (pre-) motor cortex, supplementary motor cortex, and the frontal eye fields. There was a spatial gradient from caudal PPC regions, mainly concerned with planning eye movements, to more rostral PPC regions, mainly concerned with planning hand and foot movements. Crucially, activity in caudal PPC regions predicted whether subjects were planning to move their eyes or their limbs, but did not discriminate between hand or foot movements. We conclude that PPC contains a shared representation for hand and foot movements, distinct from that involved in planning eye movements. Hence, processing in the PPC is not primarily organized according to the effectors, but rather according to functional criteria, which differ markedly between eyes and limbs. 1-C-61 A nonlinear model of the horizontal Angular Vestibulo-Ocular Reflex (AVOR) - A mechanism for context-dependent responses Mina Ranjbaran Hesarmaskan1, Henrietta L Galiana1 1 McGill University The vestibulo-ocular reflex (VOR) produces involuntary eye movements that are evoked in response to vestibular stimuli. The VOR is responsible for stabilizing retinal images during head perturbations to maintain clear vision. This reflex is composed of slow phases where the eyes move in the opposite direction to the head and fast phases when 21 Poster Sessions Full Abstracts they usually move in the direction of the head. The gain of the VOR is defined as the ratio of peak eye velocity to peak head velocity during harmonic testing or high-frequency pulse perturbations. Testing the VOR after fixation at different depths shows that the gain of the VOR depends on the location of the fixation target (real or imaginary) relative to the observer, since the eyes are not concentric with the head center of rotation [1]. It can be demonstrated geometrically that the magnitude of the ocular deviations required for compensating a translation of the eyes relative to a near target increases as a function of decreasing fixation distance. The common approach in modeling studies that focus on the context-dependence of the VOR is to use an estimate of target position to scale the gain of the reflex in the neural circuit. However, the underlying neural site and mechanism are still unknown. Recent works experimentally [2] and theoretically [3] suggest that nonlinear computations in VOR premotor circuits could generate target-distance related changes in VOR gain. The work presented here expands on this concept, with a simple bilateral model for slow phase horizontal angular VOR in the dark, based on recent knowledge of its premotor anatomy and physiology. This model is able to replicate binocular responses for targets at different depths and eccentricity during short duration head velocity 'bumps'. In this model it is postulated that nonlinear neural computations appear at the level of premotor cells in the vestibular nuclei to generate target-distance-dependent VOR gains. This nonlinear function is placed in eye-head-velocity (EHV) neurons that receive direct sensory information from the ipsilateral semicircular canals as well as efferent copies of the ipsilateral eye position and vergence signals. EHV cells are reported to have variable sensitivity to head perturbations depending on current monocular eye position and vergence angle and they project directly to motoneurons. The model also includes nonlinear vestibular sensors as described in the literature, for a more realistic sensory system in the VOR. As a result, it is possible to explore different mechanisms for the fusion of vestibular and reafference signals on premotor cells. Using simulations, the new model is shown to be consistent with observations in unilaterally-deficient or canalplugged animals [4]. References: 1. Viirre, E., et al., A reexamination of the gain of the vestibuloocular reflex. Journal of Neurophysiology, 1986. 56(2): p. 439-50. 2. Zhou, W., et al., Multiplicative computation in the vestibulo-ocular reflex (VOR). Journal of Neurophysiology, 2007. 97(4): p. 2780-2789. 3. Khojasteh, E. and H.L. Galiana, Implications of gain modulation in brainstem circuits: VOR control system. J Comput Neurosci, 2009. 27(3): p. 437-51. 4. Migliaccio, A.A., L.B. Minor, and J.P. Carey, Vergence-mediated modulation of the human angular vestibulo-ocular reflex is unaffected by canal plugging. Experimental brain research. Experimentelle Hirnforschung. Experimentation cerebrale, 2008. 186(4): p. 581-7 1-C-62 Analysis of visual behavior in a continuous visual-motor coordination task Cristian Arellano1, Pablo Burgos1, Pedro Maldonado1 1 University of Chile Objectives: Currently there is a broad understanding of the role of perceptual vision, but there is less clarity about the role of vision in the guidance of motor actions. The aim of our study was to characterize visual behavior during the performance of visually guided motor tasks and observe how it adapts to conditions of varying difficulty (number of obstacles in a maze). 22 Methods: Using a computer 10 normal vision subject, went through a series of spiral mazes contained variable number of obstacles in its path. Five maze levels of difficulty were analyzed (level 0 to 4) and two experimental conditions were compared visuo-motor (VM) v/s just gaze (JG). In VM condition subjects went through the mazes using a digital pen (G-Pen 4500). In JG condition subjects performed same task only using the gaze to went through the mazes. In both conditions the instructions and goal task were the same. Time duration task for each difficulty level and eye movements (fixations and saccades) of the subjects were recorded using an eyetracker (EyelinkII). Results: Our results showed as more difficult becomes the task more time was used. In VM condition the number of fixations and saccades increased progressively with task difficult, while saccades duration and amplitude decreased. All these variations were significantly different between the lowest and higest difficulty levels (p< 0.05). By contrast time fixations duration was similar for all difficulty levels without significant differences. When comparing visuomotor (VM) v / s just gaze (JG) conditions, we found that in the JG condition the task duration, fixation number and saccades number showed a consistent decrease for the 5 levels of difficulty assessed, although this difference was significant only at level 4 for task duration, and level 2 of difficulty for the number of fixations and saccades (p <0.05). By contrast when we compared the duration and amplitude of saccades we observed a consistent increase in these variables for JG condition for 3 lower levels of difficulty (p <0.05). Again the duration of fixations in the JG condition is similar for all levels of difficulty, and there were no significant differences compared with the VM condition. Conclusions: The visual strategy used in performing the task is similar for different subjects and consistently adapts to different levels of difficulty. The increased time task duration is given by the increase of the number of fixations and saccades. The duration of fixations is similar for both analyzed conditions (VM-JG). This results suggest that the time of capture and processing of the visual information (fixations) is relatively fixed and could be a physiological constraint to increase speed execution of analyzed task. 1-C-63 Dynamic probabilistic control of audiovisual saccades John van Opstal1, Marc Van Wanrooij2, Brian Corneil3, Doug Munoz4 1 Donders Institute, 2Radboud University Nijmegen, University of Western Ontario, 4Queens University 3 Due to its high resolution, vision is commonly thought to dominate spatial perception over other sensory systems, such as audition. Recent studies have suggested that visual capture could result from statistically-optimal (Bayesian) integration of unreliable auditory and precise visual cues that are typically assumed constant. We here report on how cue reliabilities for auditory, visual, and audiovisual targets, hidden within a cluttered audiovisual background, evolve on a millisecond timescale in a saccadic-search task. Interestingly, not only response variability (conversely, precision) depended systematically on sensory modality, saccadic-reaction time, and signal-to-noise ratio, also the response gain (accuracy) was systematically affected. We reveal a universal near-optimal precision-accuracy relationship, based on posterior probability matching, which shows constrained responses when cue reliability is poor (e.g. for fast visual responses, and for low-level sounds). This study shows for the first time that saccadic eye Poster Sessions Full Abstracts movements, and multisensory integration in particular, obey Bayesian inference on a millisecond timescale. 1-C-64 Properties of FEF projection neurons in smooth pursuit Michael Mustari1 1 University of Washington The frontal pursuit area of primates, which is located in the fundus of the arcuate sulcus plays a role in smooth pursuit (SP). Neurons in FEF discharge during smooth pursuit with appropriate timing to play a role in SP initiation. Our studies were conducted in 2 monkeys (Macaca mulatta) trained to perform SP and other visual-oculomotor tasks. Eye movements were recorded with scleral search coil technology and single units recorded with standard extracellular recording methods. For the FEF to generate SP signals must be delivered to appropriate brainstem centers. The FEF is known to project to the rostral nucleus reticularis tegmenti pontis (rNRTP), dorsolateral pontine nucleus (DLPN), superior colliculus (SC) and basal ganglia. However, we still have relatively little specific information about the signals sent from FEF to each of these regions. To address this we first mapped the SP regions of FEF and rNRTP before conducting antidromic activation studies to identify projection neurons. We tested all SP neurons in FEF for activation and characterized their response dynamics during smooth pursuit eye movements. SP testing was with step-ramp target motion so that we could consider neuronal response during open and closed-loop components of SP. We delivered micro-electrical stimulation (single biphasic pulse 200 µS, 50-200 µA) to the rNRTP and recorded FEF neurons that were antidromically activated (collision testing verified) or not activated. We used multiple linear regression modeling to account for the sensitivity of individual FEF neurons to components of SP motion. We also examined the time to peak firing during SP for antidromically activated or nonactivated neurons. We found differences in projection neurons (antidromically activated) and other neurons. The main findings were that FEF projection neurons showed strong eye acceleration sensitivity. When we attempted to reconstruct unit firing rate without eye acceleration parameters our multiple-regression models produced significantly poorer fits then when acceleration was included. Furthermore, individual FEF projection neurons had a similar early times-to-peak firing, which were appropriate for SP initiation. We found that this timing was mirrored in the properties of rNRTP neurons, which receive a major input from FEF cortex. In contrast neurons that were not antidromically activated often had more spread out times-to-peak firing during SP. Our results suggest that FEF plays a critical role in the initiation of SP presumably through its strong connections to the oculomotor vermis. Further studies are required to determine the FEF signals sent to other brainstem targets that might play a role in SP> Supported by: EY013308; RR000166 1-C-65 Adaptation of intrinsic and synaptic properties of medial vestibular neurons during visual-vestibular mismatch Erwin Idoux1, Daniel Eugène1, Antoine Mialot1, Mathieu Beraneck1 1 Université Paris Descartes The vestibulo-ocular reflex (VOR) stabilizes the gaze during unwanted movements of the head. It relies on the conversion of a head velocity signal to an eye velocity signal at the level of the medial vestibular nuclei. However, these brainstem nuclei receive information from not only the vestibular afferents, but also from visual and proprioceptive origins. A mismatch between the information from the different sources will result in the adaptation of the gain of the VOR, so that the conflict is minimized. Here, we generate a visual-vestibular mismatch (VVM) to trigger the adaptive plasticity, with an innovative protocol where the animal is wearing a translucent stripped helmet for 2 weeks. While a fully opaque helmet prevented the formation of any image altogether and a homogenously translucent helmet produced an image without salient features, the image formed when wearing the stripped helmet is fixed on the retina yet highly contrasted. Thus instead of being stimulated, head fixed, at a given frequency for a short period of time, the animal is free to experience its natural repertoire of behaviors, while constantly undergoing the VVM. The 3 protocols decreased the gain of the VOR drastically (60-70%) at all tested frequencies (0.2 to 2 Hz). After 2 days without the fully opaque or fully translucent VVM creating helmet, the gain of the VOR is restored at all frequencies. Interestingly, the same recovery pattern was found for the stripped helmet VVM at high frequencies (12Hz) but not at low frequencies (0.2-0.5Hz), and stays like that for at least a week. We thus looked for the neural correlates of these gain changes and recorded medial vestibular neurons in acute brainstem slices of control and VVMed animals. We found typical type A and B neurons in both sets, as defined quantitatively in Beraneck et al 2003. Both in control and VVMed animals, type A and B were in comparable proportions with almost identical intrinsic properties (spike height, width, concavity, convexity, AHPR and DAHP), except for changes in opposite directions for the frequency of the spontaneous discharge of both type (increase for type A and decrease for type B). We also investigated the plastic potential of the synapse between the vestibular afferent and the medial vestibular neurons, both for long-term potentiation and depression (LTP and LTD respectively). While this synapse could be potentiated or depressed in control animal, the VVM protocol abolished most of its modulation. This could be part of the mechanism by which the gain is decreased for low frequencies. 1-C-66 When the hand drives the eye: Hand-eye coordination when reaching to a manifold Thierry Pozzo1, Ambra Bisio1, Marco Jacono1, Bastien Berret1 1 Istituto Italiano di Tecnologia In previous works, we introduced a manifold reaching task to study human behavior when facing target redundancy. The pointing task was restricted to a very long target bar without salient point. Despite the absence of imposed endpoint, we found that subjects nevertheless make the decision to reach to particular regions of the bar, depending on the starting posture of the arm. The observed behavior was shown to be in agreement with optimal arm movement control. In this study, we now investigate the role of the oculomotor system in resolving task indeterminacy and decision making processes. Precisely, we wanted to verify if a saccade could be endogenously induced by the arm motor plan elaborated to reach the bar. Indeed, classical paradigms did not allow to study such a question since an imposed salient target point triggers a unique saccadic reflex mechanism preceding the arm reaching. Using EOG (electro-oculography) and 3D motion capture, we investigated the behavior of subjects instructed to perform horizontal reaching movements from a sitting posture. Three types of stimulus were randomly presented to the subjects: a single target point (stimulus S1), two simultaneous target points (S2) and a horizontal target bar (S3). Two initial arm 23 Poster Sessions Full Abstracts postures were tested, starting from the left and right sides of the workspace respectively. Before the stimulus appeared, subjects had to look at a central cross displayed on a large vertical screen. Exactly when the cross disappeared (tS) one of the stimuli was presented on the screen and the subject had to reach to the displayed target (whatever its type S1, S2 or S3). Preliminary results indicate the existence of saccadic movements directed toward the endpoint of the reach and starting before the hand movement onset (tH). However, the latencies of stimulus/eye/hand largely differed across the 3 conditions. For stimulus S1, a saccade occurred 200 ms after tS whereas the hand started approximately 500-600 ms after tS. The same eye reaction time (tE) was observed when stimulus S2 was presented to the subjects, except that in many cases one supplementary saccade occurred just before or around tH. This often increased tH, possibly underlining additional neural processes related to decision-making. In contrast, for the bar stimulus (S3) the first saccadic eye movement, which was directed toward the endpoint of the reach, occurred significantly later than in the two previous conditions: tE was greater than tS by more than 300 ms, but the value of tH was not affected. Reaching to a bar was thus not computationally more intensive than reaching to point but rather induced a significant compression of the relative eye-hand latency (tH-tE). Our results show consistent saccadic eye movements toward predicted locations in space when no salient visual stimulus was present. These locations overlapped the final hand positions onto the bar. Moreover, the timing of eye and hand displacements strongly suggests that eye movement reflect endogenous orienting mechanisms that allocate attentional resources using motor cues (e.g. arm motor plan). Since retinal input cannot specify an unequivocal spatial goal, we speculate that the oculomotor system would wait for a desired location selected by different brain structures involved in decision making and constrained by internal rewards. Thus, this study shows the bi-directionality of the visuomotor processes and that vision can be subordinated to the motor system. D - Disorders of Motor Control 1-D-106 EMG-based visual-haptic biofeedback: A promising tool to improve motor control in dystonia Claudia Casellato1, Giovanni Zorzi2, Alessandra Pedrocchi1, Giancarlo Ferrigno1, Nardo Nardocci2 1 Politecnico di Milano, 2Neurological Institute C. Besta Dystonia is a syndrome characterized by excessive and sustained muscle contraction causing twisting and repetitive movements, abnormal postures, or both. New insights suggest that dystonic motor impairments could also involve a deficit of sensory processing actions. In this framework, biofeedback could be useful, since it makes covert physiological processes more overt. The present work proposes an innovative integrated set-up providing EMG-based visual-haptic biofeedback during upper limb movements, to test if augmented sensory feedbacks can induce motor control improvement in patients affected by primary dystonia. The set-up was composed of a haptic device and a EMG device. The graphic interface was built up showing the required task. The real-time control algorithm was developed in Visual C , so as to synchronize the haptic position-force loop with the EMG signals reading; the brachioradialis EMG values were used to proportionally modify visual and haptic features of the interface: the higher was the EMG level, the higher was the virtual table friction and the background color proportionally moved from green to red. We tested, on dystonic and healthy subjects, a sequence of spiral tracking tasks, without and with biofeedback. 24 The subjects were asked to achieve the task naturally and, with BF, keeping the color far from red as much as possible and the surface friction as lowest as possible. Two-away ANOVA test, where the within-subject factor was the experimental condition (No-biofeedback / biofeedback ) and the between-subject factor was the group (dystonic / healthy), on the median Root Mean Square values of EMG, outlined that the biofeedback had a dystonia-related significant impact on the muscular control, inducing a contraction decrease, correlated with the dystonia severity. Thus, these tests pointed out the effectiveness of biofeedback paradigms in gaining a better specific-muscle voluntary control. The here-developed flexible tool show promising potential for clinical applications and sensorimotor rehabilitation. E - Posture & Gait 1-E-67 Effects of low back pain and proprioception disturbance on precision of trunk control Nienke Willigenburg1, Idsart Kingma1, Jaap H van Dieen1 1 Research Institute MOVE Introduction: Proprioception of the trunk appears to be affected in low-back pain (LBP) patients. This might lead to reduced precision in control of trunk posture and findings of increased cocontraction could be explained as a compensatory strategy to deal with this. AIM: To compare precision of trunk postural control between LBP patients and healthy controls and to study the effect of disturbing proprioceptive information from the low back through paraspinal muscle vibration on precision. Methods: Eighteen subjects with a-specific LBP and 13 healthy controls with no (history of) LBP participated. Subjects performed two tasks requiring high precision of control over trunk orientation. In task 1, a slowly moving medium-sized target was tracked over a spiral-shaped trajectory by circular movements of the trunk, while continuous visual feedback of target and actual trunk orientation was provided. In task 2, subjects maintained a self-chosen upright trunk posture within a static small or large target. Visual feedback of trunk orientation was given only when orientation was outside the target. In both conditions subjects were instructed to stay within the target. Both tasks were performed with and without paraspinal muscle vibration. In task 1, the absolute distance to the orientation corresponding with the center of the target was used as an index of performance. In task 2, both the mean distance to target center and the SD of the trunk orientation were calculated. Surface-EMG was used to obtain the ratio of antagonist over agonist EMG amplitudes as an index of cocontraction. Mixed design ANOVAs were used for statistical comparisons. Results: In task1, errors were significantly higher in patients and an interaction of group with condition, showed that vibration degraded performance in healthy subjects only. In task 2, performance was not significantly different between groups and performance was degraded by vibration in both groups. No indications for increased cocontraction in patients were found. Conclusion: Precision of trunk control was reduced in patients and not affected by muscle vibration when constant visual feedback (task 1) was present. In contrast, when Poster Sessions Full Abstracts visual feedback was absent (task 2, large target) or intermittent (small target), precision was not impaired and was affected by vibration. These results suggest that patients reduce weighting of proprioceptive information from the low back when visual feedback is available, which leads to reduced precision. In absence of visual feedback, proprioceptive information from the low back appears to be used and precision is maintained suggesting that the proprioceptive information itself is valid. 1-E-68 Persistence of motor-equivalent fluctuations during quiet standing Julius Verrel1, Didier Pradon1, Nicolas Vuillerme1 1 MPI for Human Development According to current theories of motor control, the central nervous system stabilizes performance by selectively suppressing taskrelevant variability (TRV) while allowing task-equivalent variability (TEV). In line with such a control mechanism, TEV during quiet standing has been observed to be larger than TRV (Hsu et al, 2007). In addition, TEV and TRV variability are expected to differ in their temporal characteristics, with higher persistence of taskequivalent fluctuations. This prediction was tested in the present study. Kinematics of quiet standing (5 minutes) was measured in five healthy young participants. Using the uncontrolled manifold analysis (Scholz & Schöner, 1999), postural variability in sagittal joint angles was decomposed into TEV and TRV with respect to four task variables: center of mass (CoM) and head position, and trunk and head orientation. Persistence of fluctuations within the two variability components was analyzed using a multivariate autocorrelation index, with time lags between 1 and 60 seconds. The results show that (1) TEV exceeds TRV; (2) persistence is higher for TEV than for TRV; (3) auto-correlation decreases with increasing time lag; (4) this decrease is more pronounced for TRV than for TEV. In addition, the relation between auto-correlation and time lag differed between CoM position, head position and trunk orientation on the one hand, and head orientation on the other. These findings confirm the hypothesis that TEV and TRV do not only differ in amplitude but also in their temporal characteristics, underlining the dynamic nature of whole-body postural control. Differences in the temporal variability structure of different task variables likely reflect functional differences between these postural parameters (equilibrium versus visual orienting). 1-E-69 Swimming patterns of the Octopus vulgaris Dimitris Tsakiris1, Asimina Kazakidi1, Michael Kuba2, Alex Botvinnik2, Michael Sfakiotakis1, Tamar Gutnick2, Shlomi Hanassy2, Guy Levy2, John A Ekaterinaris3, Tamar Flash4, Binyamin Hochner2 1 Institute of Computer Science – FORTH, 2The Hebrew University of Jerusalem, 3Institute of Applied and Computational Mathematics – FORTH, 4Weizmann Institute of Science Swimming is an important means of locomotion for the benthic common octopus (Octopus vulgaris) whenever the animal needs to cover distances at greater speed. Although swimming does not appear to be the predilected mode of locomotion for octopuses in captivity--possibly due to the limited available space and the large amount of energy consumption required--, the behavior has been previously observed in nature. In this study, we used six adult animals (Octopus vulgaris, 200-400 grams of weight) kept in the aquarium, in order to identify possible swimming mechanisms. We define swimming as any type of locomotion of the octopus that involves no attachment to the aquarium walls or other objects and substrates. Using this definition, three different patterns of swimming were observed: arm swimming, that involves undulations of the arms in synchrony, with a power (closing) and a recovery (opening) stroke of the arms; jet swimming, in which the octopus uses the siphon, while the arms trail tightly together, behind the mantle; and head-first swimming, where the octopus swims with the head in front and the arms trailing behind. This is the first time that these swimming patterns have been recorded systematically in the laboratory. Three-dimensional reconstructions of individual or multiple arms during these movements show the underlying mechanisms involved for each swimming pattern and may assist in the design and control of robotic arm prototypes. 1-E-70 Reflex control of treadmill walking in subjects with spinal cord injury Virginia Way Tong Chu1, Thomas G Hornby2, Brian D Schmit3 1 Rehabilitation Institute of Chicago, 2University of Illinois at Chicago, 3Marquette University During walking, it is thought that reflex activity plays an important role in the timing and magnitude of muscle activity throughout the gait cycle. Movement of the body over the stance leg imposes hip extension and limb loading during gait. The effect of limb loading and hip proprioception during stepping has been shown in decerebrated and spinal animals and human infants. As an example, Yang and colleagues (2000) introduced perturbations during different phases of the gait cycle during infant stepping and saw that the stance phase was shortened and swing phase advanced when the hip was extended and load was reduced. Since the myelination of the corticospinal tract do not complete until the age of 2 (Yakovlev & Lecours, 1967), it is reasonable to suggest that these changes in the gait cycle in infant stepping are driven primarily by brainstem and spinal cord circuitry. In this study, we examine the contribution of reflex activity during walking in people with spinal cord injury (SCI) to better understand the effect of spastic reflexes on motor tasks such as walking. We recruited 10 people with incomplete SCI and 5 healthy control subjects to study the effects of hip angle perturbations on walking. To study the effects of imposed hip extension, the subjects walked on an instrumented splitbelt treadmill (Bertec). At certain random steps, one of the treadmill belts accelerated briefly to increase hip extension during mid to late stance. We observed that following a hip extension perturbation, the swing phase was advanced and the limb advanced farther in both subject groups. The moment and EMG data revealed an increase in peak hip and ankle moment and EMG activity in the thigh and shank muscles as a result of the hip extension perturbation. In controlling walking, the subjects appeared to regulate limb position at heel strike. Hip angle at heel strike remained the same regardless of the size of the perturbation and there was a strong correlation between the stride length and hip angle at toe off. The timing of the heel strike was also well controlled. Subjects increased the swing speed such that in control subjects, a further stride was taken in the same amount of time, regardless of the size of the perturbation. In the SCI subject group, the swing speed was increased, but the time in the swing phase was still increased slightly, possibly due to limitations in generating the forces needed to further increase swing speed. This increased in swing speed was only observed in the test leg in the control group, but was observed in both legs (test and contralateral) in the SCI subjects, resulting in an increase in time in double stance phase. The limb position and timing at heel strike 25 Poster Sessions Full Abstracts has emerged to be an important control parameter in both subject groups. Our results suggested evidence of both reflex activity and cortical control of stepping in subjects with incomplete SCI. 1-E-71 Motor resonance during postural imbalance observation Thiago Lemos1, Banty Tia2, Ghislain Saunier1, Sebastian Hofle1, Luís A Imbiriba1, Thierry Pozzo3, Claudia D Vargas1 1 Universidade Federal do Rio de Janeiro, 2Université de Bourgogne, 3Université de Bourgogne and Italian Institute of Technology Neurophysiological as well as behavioral studies provided evidences that the cerebral cortex may be involved in the coding of motor aspects of postural control. In fact, postural control is a complex motor behavior, in which both cognitive and affective states are reflected. We hypothesized that motor representations of postural control could be accessed during action observation, trough a motor resonance mechanism - i.e., the motoric translation of visual aspects of action. Motor resonance during action observation paradigms has been studied via measurement of a particular EEG oscillatory pattern, the so called Mu rhythm (8-12 Hz frequency range) desynchronization. Objective: the aim of this study was to investigate Mu rhythm changes during the observation of postural imbalance situations. Methods: four point-light display (PLD) animations were constructed: biological quiet stance (QB); biological imbalance stance (using an unstable board as support, IB); scrambled quiet stance (QS); and scrambled imbalance stance (IS). Ten individuals (males, 20-39 years) were asked to rest comfortably in a sitting position, in a dimly light room, with a 19' LCD screen in front of them. The experimental sessions comprised the random presentation of the four PLD (3 s duration, 64 times each). A 128channels Geodesic system was used for EEG acquisition. Analysis was performed in a region of interest comprising the centro-parietal electrodes (CP3, CPz and CP4). EEGLAB tools were used for data pre-processing, together with MATLAB script for frequency analysis (Fast Fourier Transformation) in the 8-12 band frequency range. Frequency power changes were calculated trough the logarithm transformation of power ratios [quiet stance ratio = QB/QS; imbalance stance ratio = IB/IS; expressed in arbitrary units (a.u.)]. One-sample t test was used to search differences between log ratio and zero, since log ratio lower than zero characterizes desynchronization, and ratios equal to zero means no changes. Wilcoxon matched pairs test was applied for testing differences between quiet stance and imbalance ratios. Both tests were run assuming p < 0.05. Data was presented as mean ± SE. Results: significant difference from zero was found only for imbalance ratio in the CP3 electrode (-0.39 ± 0.1 a.u.; p = 0.014); there was also a difference between imbalance and quiet stance ratio (0.17 ± 0.2 a.u.) for this electrode (p = 0.017). CPz and CP4 did not show significant Mu rhythm desynchronization (p > 0.05). Conclusions: the observation of postural imbalance situations promotes motor resonance as evidenced trough Mu rhythm desynchronization within the left centro-parietal region. This result suggests that motor representations of postural control are accessed during the passive observation of imbalance situations. Underscoring the role of cognitive states on balance control could contribute to a higher understanding of the organization of balance strategies. 1-E-72 Characterization of the nonlinearity in ankle reflex stiffness dynamics Kian Jalaleddini1, Ehsan Sobhani-Tehrani1, Robert E. Kearney1 26 1 McGill Univerisity Joint stiffness defines the dynamic relation between the position of the joint and the torque acting about it. Joint stiffness plays an important role in the control of posture and movement and thus its accurate identification is important to understand the underlying mechanisms of postural and movement control. Joint stiffness has two components: intrinsic stiffness that is due to the mechanical properties of the joint, active muscles and passive tissues; and reflex stiffness which is due to the modulation of muscle activation as a result of stretch reflex feedback through muscle afferents. Decomposition of the recorded ankle joint torque into its intrinsic and reflex subcomponents is a challenging problem since they occur and change together. To address this, our laboratory has developed a system identification method to decompose ankle torque into intrinsic and reflex torques. Using this we have demonstrated that the reflex stiffness at the ankle joint is uni-directionally sensitive to joint velocity. Moreover, dorsiflexing displacements evoke large reflex responses while plantarflexing displacements evoke little or no reflex response. Therefore, reflex stiffness can be modelled as a cascade of nonlinear static function and a linear dynamic system, where the nonlinearity resembles a half wave rectifier and the linear dynamics are low-pass in nature. Subsequent experiments demonstrated that the reflex dynamics, in particular the gain of the reflex pathway, change dramatically as a function of the ankle operating point defined by mean joint position and the level of activation of the ankle muscles. However, the identification methods were not able to distinguish whether there were changes in the nonlinear element parameters associated with changes in the operating point. This is important to know since the reflex dynamics and static nonlinearity likely reflect different physiological mechanisms. This poster will present some recent methodological developments made to improve the characterization of the static nonlinear element of the reflex stiffness pathway. These include: 1. 2. 3. 4. Extensions to the identification algorithm to use a subspace approach to estimate the parameters of the linear and nonlinear elements in closed form. Incorporating a spline representation for the nonlinearity to improve the ability to model sharp nonlinearities concisely. In addition, we have developed objective methods of determining the complexity of the representation. Development of a novel perturbation waveform whose probability distribution is designed to be more informative in the characterization of the shape of the nonlinearity. Simulation studies demonstrate that with these developments our methods accurately estimate both the shape of the nonlinearity and linear dynamics of the reflex stiffness. Moreover, pilot experiments using these methods have shown that the nonlinear-element does change significantly with mean activation level. We will use these new methods to systematically map out how the nonlinear element changes as a function of the operating point defined by activation level and position. Poster Sessions Full Abstracts 1-E-73 Decoupling of upper and lower limb central pattern generators during human crawling may highlight the influence of cortical control on the upper limb Michael MacLellan1, Yuri P Ivanenko1, Valentina La Scaleia1, Francesco Lacquaniti1 1 Fondazione Santa Lucia Generally, the motion of upper and lower limbs during upright walking and adult hand-and-foot crawling is tightly coupled in a one-to-one relationship. However, the investigation of cat locomotion in which the fore and hindlimbs walk on separate treadmills showed discrepancies: a one-to-one relationship between limbs was not maintained when hindlimb treadmill speed was manipulated (Akay et al. 2006). The purpose of the current study is to use a crawling paradigm in healthy adults to examine limb coupling patterns and suggest how upper and lower limb central patter generators (CPGs) are controlled in a coordinated manner. Ten healthy adults performed hands-and-feet crawling on 2 separate treadmills where treadmill speed was manipulated for the upper (1, 2 and 3 km/h) and lower (1, 2 and 3 km/h) limbs totalling in 9 different speeds combinations. As well, 2 extreme speed difference conditions were performed (upper limb treadmill at 0.5 km/h, lower limb at 3 km/h and vice versa) in order to encourage decoupling of the movements between the upper and lower limbs. Full body 3D kinematics were recorded using a Vicon motion capture system. When the treadmill speed for upper and lower limbs was not matched, we observed in most cases a 1:1 relationship between the limbs. To achieve this 1:1 relationship, increases in treadmill speed were accompanied with decreases in stride time. Interestingly, relative stance and swing durations in the lower limbs were adjusted in every treadmill speed combination. In contrast, relative stance and swing durations in the upper limb were only affected by changes of the upper limb treadmill speed. Thus, the upper limb control seems to be more independent than that of the lower limb. In extreme treadmill speed differences, decoupling of upper and lower limbs was more likely to occur when the upper limbs were at lower speeds (8/10 participants) as opposed to the lower limbs (2/10 participants). Similarly, decoupling was always at lower speeds as in cats. However, it occurred in humans in most cases for upper limbs while decoupling in cats occurred in the hindlimb. Akay et al. (2006) suggested that this decoupling in cats may be accounted for by asymmetric neural pathways connecting the cervical and lumbosacral spinal enlargements. The differences seen in humans may suggest that the preference for the arms to decouple may be due to a larger influence of cortical control on the upper limb which allows for an overriding of spinal CPG control. Supported by the Italian Ministry of Health, Italian Space Agency (CRUSOE grant) and EU FP7-ICT program (MINDWALKER and AMARSi grants). 1-E-74 Changes in the spinal motor output for stepping during development from infant to adult Yuri Ivanenko1, Nadia Dominici1, Germana Cappellini1, Ambrogio Di Paolo2, Carlo Giannini3, Richard E Poppele4, Francesco Lacquaniti2 1 Santa Lucia Foundation, 2University of Rome Tor Vergata, Sant’Eugenio Hospital, 4University of Minnesota 3 Human stepping movement patterns emerge in utero and show several milestones during development to independent walking. Newborns that are adequately supported exhibit stepping behavior that resembles walking, and the behavior can persist for weeks. We examined the spinal locomotor output by mapping the distribution of motoneuron activity in the lumbosacral spinal cord during stepping in newborns, toddlers, preschoolers and adults. Using both kinematic and electromyographic (EMG) recordings from 24 bilateral leg muscles we reconstructed the patterns of spinal segmental ventral root output and approximate motoneuron activation in the lumbosacral spinal cord using myotomal charts available for segmental innervation of lower limb muscles in human neonates and adults. Newborn stepping is characterized by an alternating bilateral motor output with only two major components that are active at all lumbosacral levels of the spinal cord. This pattern was similar across different cycle durations of neonate stepping and is consistent with a simpler organization of neuronal networks. In toddlers, the step-related activity migrates to the sacral cord segments, while the lumbar motoneurons are separately activated at touch-down. In the adult, the lumbar and sacral patterns become more dissociated with shorter activation times. The results also showed that the main features of the neonate stepping could not be modelled by assuming crosstalk in 'mature' EMG recordings or could not be reproduced by simply imitating the appropriate biomechanical conditions. When we asked adults to reproduce neonate stepping conditions (body unloading, bent posture, slow speed) we found that the differences between neonatal and mature patterns could not be simply accounted for by biomechanical differences. We conclude from these results that the development of human locomotion from the neonate to the toddler involves a reorganization of the spinal circuitry. The rostrocaudal coactivation of motoneurons seen in the neonate is no longer apparent in the toddler as the lumbar and sacral motoneurons assume separate activation patterns. The separation becomes more prominent with further development with progressively shorter motoneuron activations. The alternating spinal motor output in neonates and its differential segmental maturation may suggest a common central pattern generation circuitry development in humans and other mammals. Supported by the Italian Ministry of Health and EU FP7-ICT program (MINDWALKER and AMARSi grants). 1-E-75 Error-signals driving locomotor adaptation: Effects of peripheral nerve stimulation on ankle control during walking in an elastic force field Julia Choi1, Jesper Lundbye-Jensen1, Laurent J Bouyer2, Jens Bo Nielsen1 1 University of Copenhagen, 2Université Laval Proprioceptive and cutaneous feedback plays an important role in modifying the ongoing motor pattern during locomotion. In addition to feedback mechanisms, the nervous system must also adapt movements based on mismatch between intended and actual outcomes - this reduces errors in the feedforward command for subsequent movements. We hypothesize that cutaneous signals provide a mechanism for monitoring contact forces during walking, and that mismatch between expected and actual force drives adaptation. Here we degraded sensory afferent input using non-invasive repetitive nerve stimulation and examined the effects on 1) cutaneous sensation, 2) ankle proprioception and force production, and 3) ankle adaptation during walking in a force field. We stimulated the superficial peroneal (foot dorsum) and medial plantar nerves (foot sole). Repetitive electrical nerve stimulation was administered with a constant current stimulator that delivered 1 ms pulses at 80 Hz. The intensity was set at a level where subject perceived a 'tingling' sensation that radiated towards the toes. EXPERIMENT 1: Touch thresholds were measured using Von Frey monofilaments on foot dorsum, and plantar side of great toe and heel. Vibration threshold on great toe was determined using 27 Poster Sessions Full Abstracts Vibratron II. Our results suggest that 80 Hz electrical nerve stimulation selectively worsened cutaneous sensation, without affecting vibration sensation. EXPERIMENT 2: We tested the effects of degraded cutaneous sensation on the accuracy and precision of ankle force production. Subjects were seated and instructed to maintain an isometric dorsiflexion force equivalent to 10% maximal voluntary contraction with the right foot attached to an instrumented pedal. In addition, an active position-matching task was used to test whether proprioception at the ankle joint (above stimulation site) could be affected. We showed that subjects were unable to precisely maintain a constant dorsiflexion force with peroneal nerve stimulation without visual feedback. This was not a general effect of repetitive stimulation because subjects could maintain dorsiflexion force precisely with plantar nerve stimulation. Proprioception at the ankle joint was also unaffected. These results suggest that cutaneous feedback is required for precise ankle force control. EXPERIMENT 3: Finally, we tested the effects of degraded cutaneous feedback on walking adaptation. Subjects were exposed to a position-dependent force field produced by an elastic band attached between the back of the shoe and the calf band of an ankle-foot orthosis, which resisted ankle dorsiflexion. In the baseline period, subjects walked with no external force at the ankle. In the adaptation period, an elastic was attached and subjects were instructed to 'resist the force and walk normally' for about 30 strides. In the post-adaptation period, the elastic force was removed to test for after-effects. Each subject repeated the adaptation paradigm in a randomly assigned sequence of conditions (peroneal nerve, plantar nerve, no stimulation). Results indicate that adaptation of ankle dorsiflexion and tibialis anterior activation during force field walking was reduced with peroneal nerve stimulation, whereas plantar nerve stimulation had no effects. In conclusion, we showed that cutaneous feedback plays an important role in driving human locomotor adaptation. Identifying salient neural signals that drive adaptation could increase the efficiency of gait rehabilitation. F - Fundamentals of Motor Control 1-F-33 Effects of ageing on attentional cost and internal models of proprioceptive control of movement Matthieu P. Boisgontier1, Isabelle Olivier1, Vincent Nougier1 1 UJF-Grenoble 1 / CNRS / TIMC-IMAG UMR 5525 It is still not clear whether motor control based on proprioceptive information is altered in older adults as compared to young ones. Previous studies showed strong and unexplained discrepancies and did not conclude about a presumed age-related alteration of proprioceptive control. In the present study, it was hypothesized that the attentional cost of proprioceptive control was altered with ageing. It was also hypothesized this presumed age-related alteration of proprioceptive control was associated to an alteration of internal models. To test the attentional hypothesis, one group of young adults and one group of older adults performed a joint position matching task in single and dual-task paradigms with different difficulty levels of the secondary task to assess attentional costs associated to proprioceptive control in these populations. To identify the central mechanisms involved in the proprioceptive control, we proposed to assess the effects of ageing on internal models. To this aim, one group of young adults and one group of older adults performed the same ankle matching task in two speed conditions (self-selected and fast). Error, temporal, and kinematic variables were the dependent variables used to assess the matching performance. Results showed that proprioceptive control was as accurate and as 28 consistent in older as in young adults for a single proprioceptive task at a self-selected speed. However, performing a secondary cognitive task and increasing the difficulty of this secondary task evidenced an increased attentional cost of proprioceptive control in older adults as compared to young ones. The results also demonstrated that internal models of proprioceptive control were altered with ageing. Behavioural expressions of these alterations were dependent upon the considered condition of speed. In the self-selected speed condition, this alteration was expressed through an increased number of corrective submovements in older adults as compared to young ones. This strategy enabled them to reach a level of end-point performance comparable to the one of the young adults. In the fast speed condition, older adults were no more able to compensate for their impaired internal models through additional corrective sub-movements and therefore decreased their proprioceptive control performance.These results advocated for an increased attentional cost for proprioceptive control in older adults as compared to young adults. The present age-related alterations of ankle proprioceptive control also suggested a decreased resistance to attentional and speed stressors in adult ageing and supported the fact that proprioceptive control is involved in the frailty syndrome, i.e., a decreased resistance to stressors, which characterizes older adults. 1-F-34 Mechanisms of oscillatory drive in human motor control Tjeerd Boonstra1, Michael Breakspear1 1 University of New South Wales Oscillatory activity in the neurophysiological drive to the muscle plays an important role in corticospinal control of muscle synergies. In pathological conditions or effortful activity, this may be exaggerated and become evident as a tremor. Under many circumstances, however, oscillatory activity may not be directly noticeable, but can be discerned using spectral analysis of electromyography (EMG) and electroencephalography (EEG). In particular, many studies use corticospinal (EEG-EMG) and intermuscular (EMGEMG) coherence to assess oscillatory input to the muscle. Although many oscillatory components in the input to the muscle have been identified, the physiological mechanisms that bring about these oscillations remain to be identified. The ongoing debate on EMG rectification as a preprocessing step in assessing oscillatory activity underlines our incomplete understanding. A number of studies have addressed the rational of EMG rectification and the effects it has on power and coherence spectra, but no general agreement has been reached. The discussion seems to divide experimental from computational approaches: Most empirical studies use and promote rectification of surface EMG, whereas simulation studies generally argue against rectification. The elucidation of this technical issue therefore directly contributes to our understanding of the mechanisms underlying oscillatory neuronal activity. We use an integrated computational and experimental approach to reconcile experimental and computational approaches on the issue of EMG rectification. We study the mechanisms involved in the generation of common oscillatory activity in a basic model of two MU pools that incorporate key physiological properties. Simulated data are compared to empirical data of intermuscular coherence acquired from healthy subjects during quiet standing. Poster Sessions Full Abstracts In particular, we investigate the role of a heterogeneous motor unit action potential (MAUP) population on the translation of oscillatory activity into surface EMG. We show that a heterogeneous MAUP distribution distorts the transmission of common input and oscillatory components are only observed in rectified EMG. In contrast, using a homogeneous MUAP distribution oscillatory input is evident in both rectified and nonrectified EMG. The experimental data showed that intermuscular coherence was mainly discernable in rectified EMG, hence providing empirical support for a heterogeneous distribution of MUAPs. These findings show that oscillatory components are manifested as modulations of high-frequency EMG content and implicate that the shape of MUAPs is an essential parameter to reconcile experimental and computational approaches. 1-F-35 Linking motor and reward: The correlation between nucleus accumbens and primary motor cortex during goal directed reaching Justin Sanchez1, Eric Pohlmeyer1, Babak Mahmoudi1, Shijia Geng1, Noeline Prins1 1 University of Miami A central role of the brain is to analyze the environment and make decisions that lead to reward and survival. Access to hierarchical levels of processing including sensory, motor, and reward systems have enabled organisms to become more responsive to their environments and ultimately shapes future motor behavior. The use of motor and reward neural processing to carryout goaldirected behaviors has the effect of expanding capabilities for responding to dynamical events in the environment. Our studies seek to develop a deeper understanding of the simultaneous neural coding among the nucleus accumbens (NAcc) and the primary motor cortex (M1) during goal directed reaching tasks. This includes characterization of the fine timing relationships and incremental evaluative feedback from the NAcc and how it relates to excitatory or inhibitory motor activation that is conducive or non-conducive for obtaining reward. To study the relationship, we implanted microwire arrays (50μm tungsten wires, 16 in each target) into the hand-arm region of M1 and into the NAcc of common marmoset monkeys (Callithrix jacchus). The animals were trained to perform a reaching and grasping task. The animals were required to place their reaching hand in a start position. Upon doing this, they were presented with either a desirable food item (worm) or a non-food item (wooden bead). When presented a worm, the monkeys would reach and grasp the food. Alternatively, when presented with a bead the animal would not reach. During these experiments, arm position was tracked and time synchronized with single neuron recordings from both the cortical and subcortical targets. To characterize the coding in both neural structures, all reaching and non-reaching behaviors were time aligned and perievent time histograms were constructed. In these experiments, we found that the time courses of the neuromodulation in the M1 and NAcc as they are related to reaching vs non-reaching are quite different. For example, in rewarding trials, the M1 neurons ramp up their activity during the reach. The firing of NAcc remains low. During non-rewarding trials, the animal does not reach and there is a decrease in M1 activity. This decrease in M1 was concurrent with a persistent increase in the NAcc activity signaling a shutdown in the motor output. To test the temporal coding and representation of both M1 and NAcc during reaching in the paradigm described above, we built linear classification models (Wiener Filter) for just the global reach and non-reach trials in four experimental sessions. For each session, half the trials were randomly selected and used to train the model, which was tested by its classification of the other 50% of the trials. In each case, we scanned the performance using between 0-400ms of data as the classifier input. Interestingly, the M1 classification continued to improve as more data from any arm movement was included in the trial indicating a longer time scale. However, the NAcc classifier accuracy appeared consistent throughout the entire time period, suggesting the time course of the NAcc activity provides short term feedback compared to M1. 1-F-36 Haptic illusions: A window into motor control Alexander Terekhov1, Vincent Hayward1 1 Université Pierre et Marie Curie Haptic illusions: a window into motor control. Alexander V. Terekhov and Vincent Hayward Dexterity and perceptual proficiency requires knowledge about the properties of one's own body. In one school, it is assumed that this knowledge can be approximate and that the brain controls movement through a cascade of stabilizing mechanisms. This view is adopted in the equilibrium point hypothesis. Another school insists on the necessity for the brain to utilize elaborate internal models describing the dynamics of the body and of the external world. It is only under this condition that optimal mappings between sensory inputs and motor outputs can arise. These two conflicting views are successful are successful at explaining many experimental observations. We suggest that the models used by the brain need to be accurate, but only to a point. Attempting to characterize their accuracy is an enticing and challenging task which cannot be solved by motor control studies alone. Often, behaviors can be explained by inaccuracies in the models or by the particularities of the cost functions governing task execution. Haptic perception provides an opportunity to gauge the accuracy of internal models because it often depends on motor activity involving multiple segments. For instance, when experiencing shape by fingertip exploration, the brain must combine information from multiple joints and account for the geometry of the segments engaged in the movements. Our ability to estimate shapes by touch suggests that this process is quite accurate. We can discriminate radii differences of about 1 mm in a stimulus of 60 mm in diameter. We found three haptic illusions related to these questions. The first illusion occurs when exploring a curved surface with a fingertip where different strategies for fingertip orientation are employed. One can move as to maintain the fingertip orientation fixed in space; or one can keep the point of the contact stationary on the skin, thereby causing the fingertip orientation to track the normal of the surface. If the internal model of the body was accurate, then the mode of exploration should not influence the percept of curvature. Yet, experimental studies show that it is not the case. Subjects tend to perceive the same surface to be more or less curved depending on the mode of exploration. The second illusion arises when exploring a concave surface with a thin stick. Provided that the exploratory movements involve rotations of the wrist, subjects tend to perceive the same object to have a larger size when they explore it with a short stick than when they explore it with a longer stick. The illusion becomes weak, or even vanishes, if the stick orientation remains the same during the exploration. Finally, when an object is explored with a thick stick, subjects size judgements depend on the stick thickness. With a thicker stick, a convex object seem larger -- and a concave object seem smaller --- than with a thinner stick. If the subjects correctly used available information about the stick length and width this illusion would not occur. In all these examples, the difference in the perceived radius of the object was of the order of 10 mm for a stimulus diameter of 60 mm. These effects suggests that in spite of the considerable capacity of the haptic modality, the brain 29 Poster Sessions Full Abstracts does not always integrate information with the consequence an internal model of the body or of the environment can be rather imprecise. 1-F-37 Controlling output from the motor cortices: Avoiding unintentional movement using the null space Matthew Kaufman1, Mark M Churchland2, Stephen I Ryu3, Krishna V Shenoy1 1 Stanford University, 2Columbia University, 3Palo Alto Medical Foundation Motor and premotor cortex are involved in the execution of reaching movements, but are also active during preparation of those movements. Given that the arm remains motionless during preparation, we ask: why does preparatory activity not cause movement? How is preparatory activity attenuated from premotor cortex to motor cortex to the spinal cord to the muscles? One possible mechanism is that activity is prevented from escaping each area because output is suppressed by local inhibition. This mechanism is used in the brainstem for saccade control. We have previously searched for 'gating' by inhibitory interneurons in primary motor (M1) and dorsal premotor cortex (PMd) of monkeys. We could not find evidence for inhibitory gating in either PMd (Kaufman et al., 2010, J Neurophys) or M1 (Kaufman et al., 2010, SFN). Specifically, we did not find inhibitory neurons that were highly active during motor preparation but quiet during movement itself. We thus propose and test an alternate model. Since there are many more neurons than muscles, there must be many possible neural states for any given pattern of muscle forces. We therefore predict that there will be 'output-null' dimensions in neural space, such that changes in activity along these dimensions do not alter muscle output. These output-null dimensions could be exploited to permit motor preparation without causing movement. Here, we attempt to identify output-null dimensions using four neural datasets from two monkeys performing a delayed-reach task. A target appeared while the animal fixated. The monkeys were required to withhold movement until a go cue, then reach to the target. Recordings were performed in PMd and M1, using single moveable electrodes or dual chronically-implanted 96-electrode 'Utah' arrays. We tested whether some neural dimensions might be 'output-potent,' identifiable because activity in these dimensions correlates with muscle activity, while other dimensions might be output-null. We predicted that neural activity would preferentially explore the output-null dimensions during preparation, avoiding the outputpotent dimensions in order to avoid causing movement. To test this prediction, we first reduced the dimensionality of both the neural data (to six) and EMG data (to three) using PCA. We then regressed the reduced-D EMG data onto the reduced-D neural data, yielding the putative output-potent dimensions; the remaining dimensions were taken as output-null. Finally, we measured how far neural activity traveled in output-null dimensions vs. output-potent dimensions. As predicted, we found that preparatory activity travels mostly in output-null (non-musclelike) dimensions. This bias toward output-null dimensions was substantial: during preparation the changes in neural activity were 2.4-9.8 times as large in the output-null dimensions as in the output-potent dimensions (p < 0.05 in each dataset). This mechanism therefore appears to explain much of how we can prepare to move while holding still. We further tested whether a similar mechanism might explain how M1 can display less preparatory activity than PMd, even though the two are connected. As predicted, PMd preparatory activity preferentially lay in output-null dimensions with respect to M1, by a factor of 2.2-2.4 (p < 0.05 in both datasets). Selective use of output-null vs. output-potent dimensions may thus constitute an important mechanism for controlling communication between brain areas and output to muscles. 30 1-F-38 Removing motion information reduces the latency of manual movement corrections Leonie Oostwoud Wijdenes1, Eli Brenner1, Jeroen Smeets1 1 VU University Amsterdam If a target instantaneously changes its position while a hand is moving towards it, short latency movement adjustments occur so that the hand ends at the new target position. Instantaneous changes in target position are perceived as if the target moved from one position to the other. If the target changes position during a saccade, the target motion is not perceived, but subjects do adjust for the target displacement. Previously reported response latencies suggest that the latency to correct for a perceived target jump may be faster than the latency to correct for target jumps during saccades. We investigated whether the perception of target motion decreases the latency of movement corrections. Motion perception was disrupted by introducing a 100 ms temporal gap between the two successive target presentations. During the gap there was no visible target. If motion information speeds up responses, we expect longer response latencies when motion information is removed by introducing a gap. Subjects made horizontal pointing movements over approximately 70 cm in a setup with back-projected targets on a large screen. The target either jumped instantaneously 3 cm up or down (step condition) or disappeared and only reappeared at the vertically shifted position 100 ms later (gap condition). We determined the latency of responses to the new target position from the difference between the average vertical acceleration profiles to targets that jumped up and down. The intensity of the response was defined as the maximum of this difference in acceleration. The results show that the latency to respond to the new target position was 9 ms shorter after the gap. The movement corrections in the gap condition were less vigorous than those in the step condition, while there was no difference in movement time. We conclude that movement corrections are not guided by the perception of target motion, but by a change in target position. The faster response latencies in the gap condition might be explained by a mechanism similar to the one causing the gap-effect in initiating eye and hand movements. By our knowledge this is the first demonstration of a gap-effect for fast online movement corrections. 1-F-39 Individual Purkinje cell simple spike discharge is full of errors Laurentiu Popa1, Angela L Hewitt1, Timothy J Ebner1 1 University of Minnesota Although the cerebellum has been hypothesized to play a major role in error processing, evidence for cerebellar error signals remains inconclusive. Most efforts aimed at understanding cerebellar error encoding concentrated on the complex spike discharge and yielded contradictory results. A limited number of studies suggest that the simple spike (SS) activity of Purkinje cells (PCs) could encode error representations. The present study shows that SS discharge encodes performance errors independently of kinematic signals. Two rhesus monkeys were trained to track a pseudorandomly moving target on a vertical screen using a two joint manipulandum. The difficulty of the experimental paradigm induced numerous excursions outside the target that had to be corrected within 500 ms, resulting in a Poster Sessions Full Abstracts behavior that required continuous processing of performance errors. During tracking, we collected extracellular recordings from 120 PCs in the intermediate and lateral zones anterior to the primary fissure. Given that the movement goal is to maintain the cursor within the target, cursor movement relative to the target provides well defined signals related to the sensory consequences of motor performance. Multiple linear regressions including temporal lags from -500 ms to 500 ms were used to evaluate the PC simple spike firing in relation to the following signals: cursor position relative to the target center (XE, YE), cursor distance to the target center (RE), relative direction of the cursor movement to the direction of the target movement (DE), and the difference between the cursor direction movement and the direction of the error position vector (PDE). To insure that the error signals were independent of the kinematic signals, the linear regressions were performed on the residuals obtained after fitting a kinematic model including position, velocity, and speed. In more than 95% of the recorded cells, the SS discharge is modulated by each of the error parameters. The error parameters resolve an amount of firing variability comparable to that explained by the kinematic model. The best R2 adjusted values for the error and kinematic models obtained on the complementary residuals are highly correlated, showing that kinematic and error signals are integrated at the PC level. Analysis of the correlation profiles' properties, show that more than 80% of the PCs have a bimodal fit profile with two local maxima, one at predictive and one at feedback lag values. Moreover, 66% of the bimodal timing profiles show opposite modulations in the SS activity for the feedback and predictive signals, possibly a substrate to generate a sensory prediction error. Remarkably, individual kinematic signals, independent of the error parameters, generate similar bimodal fit profiles and opposite SS discharge modulations between feedback and predictive representations. Therefore, SS discharge encodes multiple error and kinematic parameters, each parameter being represented as a combination of predictive and feedback signals. This study shows that the SS discharge of individual PCs encodes motor errors that are integrated with kinematic signals. The bimodal timing of the individual parameters found in the vast majority of the PCs could provide the cellular substrate for the computation of sensory prediction errors required by forward internal model motor control theories. Supported in part by NIH grants RO1 NS18338, T32 GM008244, F30 NS071686. 1-F-40 Modulation of cerebellar function does not affect the timing or magnitude of motor surround inhibition Anna Sadnicka1, Panagiotis Kassavetis1, John C Rothwell1, Mark J Edwards1 1 UCL Institute of Neurology Background: Surround inhibition is a physiological mechanism to focus neuronal activity in the nervous system. Initially described in the sensory system it has been more recently demonstrated in the motor system using transcranial magnetic stimulation (TMS) over the hand region of the motor cortex around the onset of a small voluntary movement of the index finger. At this time motor evoked potentials (MEPs) from surrounding muscles (abductor digiti minimi (ADM) and abductor policis brevis (APB)) are reported to diminish in size. The neural mechanism underlying motor surround inhibition (mSI) is unknown. We explored the role of the cerebellum in the generation of mSI using transcranial direct current stimulation of the cerebellum (cDC). Method: 12 healthy subjects took part in a cross-over study in which they received sham, anodal or cathodal cDC in a randomised order, each session separated by a week. mSI was tested before stimulation and at 0 min and 20 min after cDC. The magnitude of mSI in ADM and APB was calculated at 0, 50, 100 and 200 msec after the onset of index finger movement (FDI was the active muscle) as a normalised ratio compared to the mean rest MEP of each muscle. Results: We could reliably record mSI in ADM which was found to be greatest at 0 msec and still present at 50 msec. Repeated measures ANOVA demonstrated a significant effect in ADM at time 0 and 50 msec (p<0.0001). Anodal and cathodal cDC did not affect the timing or magnitude of mSI observed in the ADM muscle (p = 0.31). Individual variability of mSI measured on separate occasions is characterised. We did not find evidence of the previously reported mSI in the APB muscle. Conclusions: Modulation of the cerebellum with cDC does not affect mSI as assessed by TMS. Perhaps the phenomenon of mSI is generated across the sensorimotor network and transient weak modulation of one node of this network is not powerful enough to disrupt the mechanism. In addition we found no evidence that APB muscle is inhibited using this paradigm, which is in contrast to previous findings. Further experiments are needed to fully characterise mSI and its adaptability as better understanding of this mechanism may generate new treatments in disorders of motor control such as dystonia where impaired SI occurs. 1-F-41 Excitatory amino acid transporters regionally sculpt the beam-like response in the cerebellar cortex Samuel Cramer1, Wangcai Gao1, Gang Chen1, Timothy J Ebner1 1 University of Minnesota The role of parallel fibers (PFs) in cerebellar physiology remains controversial. Early anatomical and physiological studies of the cerebellum inspired a model whereby PF activation results in post-synaptic excitation of beams of Purkinje cells (PCs), the "beam" hypothesis. However, functional studies have yet to demonstrate activation of PCs along beams of PFs. Instead, experiments suggest that the ascending limb of the granule cell (GC) axon activates patches of PCs, while PFs predominately modulate PC activity. This alternative model is the "radial" hypothesis. Proponents of this model postulate that stronger synapses exist between the ascending limb of the GC axon and PCs, though this is not uniformly supported by experimental evidence. Others have proposed that inhibition by molecular layer interneurons suppresses PF activation of on-beam PCs, producing patch-like responses. Given that PFs are central components in many theories of cerebellar function and yet remain poorly understood, this study reexamined the question of patches versus beams. Employing Ca2 optical imaging in the mouse cerebellar cortex in vivo, electrical stimulation of mossy fibers or GCs evokes a beam-like response. Given the potential problems with electrical stimulation, we also utilized a more selective pharmacological approach to activate GCs. By systematically microinjecting glutamate or NMDA in the cerebellar cortex, we show that a maximal beam-like response is evoked by these agonists in the superficial granular layer. Next we examined the responses to peripheral stimulation. Remarkably, ipsilateral forepaw stimulation evokes a beam- 31 Poster Sessions Full Abstracts like response in Crus I. These results are in contrast to the previously reported patch-like activation of Crus II evoked by vibrissal pad stimulation. These regional differences in response to sensory input prompted further investigation into the underlying mechanism. The GABAA blocker, GABAzine, failed to convert patch-like activity in Crus II to a beam-like response, suggesting that molecular layer inhibition is not involved. However, the excitatory amino acid transporter (EAAT) blocker, DL-TBOA, rapidly converts patch-like activity in Crus II into a beam-like response, suggesting that local extracellular glutamate concentration determines the geometry of cerebellar activation in response to GC input. Finally, given its role as the main EAAT in PCs and its parasagittal and folia dependent expression profile, we investigated the role of EAAT isoform 4 (EAAT4) in determining the response geometry in the cerebellar cortex. Using transgenic mice that express GFP under control of the EAAT4 promoter, we show that peripherally evoked patch-like responses in Crus II are aligned between parasagittal bands of EAAT4. This is the first study to demonstrate in vivo that beam-like responses are evoked in the cerebellar cortex to peripheral, mossy fiber, and GC stimulation. Furthermore, the results show that the response to GC input is highly dependent on extracellular glutamate and its local regulation by EAATs. Supported in part by NIH grants T32 GM008244, T32 GM008471, RO1 NS18338 1-F-42 Tool kinematics planning in humans Tsuyoshi Ikegami1, Gowrishankar Ganesh1 1 NICT/ATR While recent studies have shown that tool usage is not restricted to human activities, the exceptional capability of man to learn using a wide array of tools still distinguishes him from other animals. During use of hand held tools, locating the tip of a tool with respect to one's hand is the first and probably the most fundamental step towards the use of the tool. Humans can perform this localization immediately, such that while holding a tool they can immediately plan new hand trajectories to accommodate for the tool kinematics. How is this localization and planning performed? To answer this question we asked subjects to make arm movements to reach different targets with the tip of 'tools' of different size and orientation which they held in their hand. The subjects were shown their hand position (with the tool) before the movement start but were not given any feedback during or after their movement on their performance. The target was visible throughout the movement trial period. We looked at the error distribution in the repeated reaching tasks to check for the nature of planning and the coordinate system of planning involved in the tool task. Our results show that humans are very good at incorporating the tool orientation into their planning. However, they consistently over compensate for the length of the tool which we show as not to be due to an error in visual perception but due to error in motor planning. Further the length of arm movements were consistently under estimated in the presence of a tool. This poster presents these results and discusses the planning coordinates and planning structure which can explain the observed results. 1-F-43 Intermittency in visual information acquisition in continuous tracking task Yasuyuki Inoue1, Yutaka Sakaguchi1 1 The University of Electro-Communications In a visuo-manual tracking task, hand motion often precedes the target motion, especially when the target motion is periodic with a 32 frequency greater than 0.5 Hz. This suggests that human brain performs predictive or feed-forward motor control even in a "tracking task." In order to continue accurate tracking, however, we have to refer to visual information related to target and hand motions for movement adjustment. Here, it is unclear how the visual information operates in the motor control: Do we refer to visual information continuously or intermittently? If intermittently, when do we catch the information? To answer these questions, we conducted a behavioral experiment where visual information of target was partially removed. We found that hand motion changed when the target disappeared in the specific phase of sinusoidal trajectory. In our experiment, green (target) and red (marker) light spots were presented on a vertical screen (located 2 m apart from the subject), where the marker position reflected the hand position on a one-dimensional sliding rail. The subject was asked to move the right hand so that the red marker tracked the green target as accurately as possible. The target moved sinusoidally in a vertical direction and its frequency was 1.2 Hz. Amplitude of the target movement was 20 cm on the screen, which corresponded to 6.7 cm hand movement. In the control condition, the target was visible throughout the trial. In the vision removal condition, the target disappeared while it was located within a specific phase range. The position of the removal range was 0 (top), 90 (center), 180 (bottom) or 270 (center) degrees and its width was 60 or 90 degrees. The marker was always visible regardless of the conditions. In the control condition, the phase of hand motion was generally advanced with that of target, but their phase difference fluctuated periodically: The average phase difference was the largest roughly around center points of the sinusoids (i.e., where the target velocity was highest), whereas it was nearly zero around the turning points (i.e., where the target motion reversed). This confirms that our brain performs the tracking task in a predictive manner, but suggests that the predictive motor control was performed in a non-stationary manner. In the vision removal condition, on the other hand, this tendency remarkably changed, and it depended on the phase of the vision removal. Especially, removal around one turning point (top/bottom) increased the variance of phase difference in the other turning point (bottom/top). This suggests that out brain refers to visual information intermittently synchronized with the motion phase. 1-F-44 Preparing to grasp pleasant and unpleasant objects Laura Oliveira1, Luis Aureliano Imbiriba2, Maitê Mello Russo1, Erika de Carvalho Rodrigues3, Anaelli Aparecida Nogueira-Campos1, Mirtes Garcia Pereira4, Eliane Volchan1, Cláudia Domingues Vargas1 1 IBCCF/UFRJ, 2EEFD/UFRJ, 3UNISUAM, 4UFF Prediction of the shape and size of objects is incorporated in the planning of actions toward them. Here, we examine if the valence of a to-be-grasped object also exerts an influence on movement planning. Readiness potential, an event-related marker of motor preparation, was recorded by means of electroencephalogram before grasping pleasant, neutral and unpleasant objects (chocolate, clip papers and cockroach for e.g.). Items used were balanced in weight and placed inside transparent cups to prompt a similar grip among trials. To appraise significant differences between the mean values of readiness potential amplitude, statistical analysis was performed using a three-way repeated measures ANOVA with channel (C3, C4 and Cz), wave segment (Early readiness potential and Late readiness Poster Sessions Full Abstracts potential) and category (pleasant, unpleasant and neutral) as factors. A main effect was found for category (p<0.01). Post-hoc analyses revealed that the mean readiness potential amplitude for the unpleasant condition was significantly higher than that of the neutral condition. Furthermore, the amplitude was significantly lower for the pleasant condition than for the neutral condition. No other main effect or interaction was significant. Thus, we show for the first time that the valence of the object to which an action is directed affects the sensorimotor cortex activity preceding its grasping. Smaller readiness potential amplitudes preceding the interaction with pleasant objects could imply in the recruitment of pre-set motor repertoires, whereas higher amplitudes found for unpleasant objects could emerge from a discrepancy between the required action and the object's aversiveness. Theories of motor control propose that planning involves the prediction of motor commands outcomes. Our results indicate that motor planning also encodes the valence of an object with which one is about to interact. 1-F-45 Do humans prefer to see their grasping points? Dimitris Voudouris1, Eli Brenner1, Jeroen BJ Smeets1 1 VU University Amsterdam In order to grasp an object the digits need to be placed at suitable positions on its surface. The selection of such grasping points depends on several factors. Here we examined whether being able to see one of the selected grasping points is such a factor. Subjects grasped large cylinders or oriented blocks that would normally be grasped with the thumb visible and the index finger occluded by the object in question. An opaque screen that hid the thumb's usual grasping point was used to examine whether people would choose a grip that was oriented differently to maintain vision of the thumb's grasping point. A transparent screen served as control. Occluding the thumb's grasping point made subjects move more carefully (e.g. larger grip aperture) and choose slightly different grip orientation. However, the change in grip orientation was much too small to keep the thumb visible. We conclude that humans do not particularly aim for visible grasping points. 1-F-46 Cortical silent period duration and its implications for surround inhibition of a hand muscle Sahana Kukke1, Brach Poston1, Rainer Paine1, Sophia Francis2, Mark Hallett1 1 National Institutes of Health, 2University of Maryland Surround inhibition is a well-known mechanism in sensory system physiology whereby an activated neuron decreases the activity of immediately adjacent neurons to sharpen the localization of stimulus information (Blakemore et al., 1970). This process appears to be a fundamental pattern of neural organization because it operates in various forms in practically every sensory system (Nabet and Pinter, 1991). In the motor system, neural processes analogous to surround inhibition have also been identified (Hallett and Khoshbin, 1980) in which excitatory drive is focused to muscles responsible for a given movement (agonist muscles) while suppressed in muscles not relevant to the movement (surround muscles). However, the specific physiological mechanisms responsible for the generation of surround inhibition in healthy human subjects are currently unknown. The purpose of this study was to determine the contribution of GABAB receptor mediated intracortical inhibition as assessed by the cortical silent period (CSP) to the generation of surround inhibition in the motor system. Eight healthy righthanded adults (5 women and 3 men, 29.8 ± 9 years) gave consent to participate in this transcranial magnetic stimulation (TMS) experiment. The task was to isometrically contract the abductor digiti minimi (ADM, surround muscle) muscle at a constant force level, and perform a concurrent index finger flexion movement (agonist contraction) upon hearing a go tone. TMS was applied to the contralateral primary motor cortex at four times before and during the index finger movement. In response to the TMS pulse, the motor evoked potential (MEP) amplitude and the CSP duration of the ADM muscle were measured. The significance of the timing of TMS on the outcomes was tested using the non-parametric Friedman test (ADM MEP), and a repeated measures oneway ANOVA (log-transformed ADM CSP duration). Post hoc testing with Bonferroni adjustment for ADM MEP and with the Dunnett adjustment for ADM CSP duration was used to compare each of the three testing times after the go tone to the testing point before the go tone. Results of this study confirmed the presence of surround inhibition; the ADM MEP was decreased during index finger movement initiation compared to the ADM MEP prior to the go tone (p = 0.0141). In the presence of surround inhibition, an increase in ADM CSP duration would implicate GABAB receptor mediated intracortical inhibition as a potential mechanism of surround inhibition. On the contrary, our results demonstrated the CSP duration of the ADM decreased during index finger movement initiation compared to the CSP duration prior to the go tone (p = 0.0004). Therefore, our findings indicate that GABAB receptor mediated intracortical inhibition as measured by the duration of the CSP does not contribute to the generation of surround inhibition in ADM. Similar to previous studies (Beck and Hallett, 2011), these results were able to exclude the possible contribution of a specific cortical pathway to surround inhibition, but unable to identify the pathway responsible for the phenomenon. Future work is planned to examine other cortical inhibitory and excitatory pathways that may be responsible for surround inhibition in the human motor system. 1-F-47 Grip force control strategies during slow load variations Thibault Giard1, Jean-Louis Thonnard1, Philippe Lefèvre1 1 University College London The grip force (GF) adaptation strategies used to manipulate an object during fast load perturbations (for instance, caused by collisions) have already been described in the literature (Serrien et al. 1999, White et al. 2011). However, very few studies, have dealt with slow load variations. In this respect, it has been demonstrated that there is an important difference in grip force control between (i) the manipulation of an object in presence of a tangential torque and (ii) the manipulation of an object without tangential torque (Crevecoeur et al. 2011). The main aims of this study are to characterize the grip force control strategies during slow load variations as well as to bring new clues to better understand the results about the compensation for torques during object manipulation. In order to fulfill these objectives, the GF response to slow increments in the tangential force (TF) and to slow increments in tangential torque (TT) were tested separately and compared. The difference in strategy between the two conditions and the reaction time were analyzed. A robotic arm has been used to produce a linear variation of the load. Two 3-dimensional force and torque sensors have been adapted on the end effector of the arm which was held by the subjects (n = 6) using the precision grip. They were blindfolded and were required to keep the end effector vertically and to avoid any slip. The force was applied in three different directions: upward (TF variation), forward (TT variation) and backward (TT variation). Three durations 33 Poster Sessions Full Abstracts were used to reach the maximal force (1.5N in rotation and 9N in translation): 10, 20 or 30 seconds. Each subject performed 4 repetitions for each condition. Two distinct GF control strategies were observed : a "smooth strategy" and a "stepwise strategy". The first one was characterized by a smooth GF variation scaled to the load variation. In the second one, important and sudden increases of GF (steps) were present and formed a discontinuous staircase increase of GF. In the TF condition, the average number of steps during one trial was 0.63 while in the TT condition it corresponded to 1.74. This significant difference (p-value = 1.7x10-¹¹) means that the "smooth strategy" and the "stepwise strategy" were preferentially used in the TF condition and the TT condition respectively. The mean reaction times were respectively 7% and 33% of the total duration of the force increase in the TF condition and the TT condition (p-value = 2.7x10⁻³³). It means that the subjects were able to detect the force variation during the translations faster than during the rotations. This slower detection in the TT condition might arise because of the type of event triggering the grip force update and might explain the difference of strategy used in each condition. This study highlights the main grip force control strategies used during slow load variations : a "smooth strategy" and a "stepwise strategy". Moreover, it confirms the presence of a difference in grip force control when a tangential torque is present. Indeed, in presence of a tangential torque, both the strategy used and the reaction time, were significantly different from the condition without torque. 1-F-48 Tactile detection of slip: Fine characterization of skin deformation during the onset of slip Benoit Delhaye1, Philippe Lefèvre2, Jean-Louis Thonnard1 1 Institute of Neuroscience - Université catholique de Louvain, Institute of Information and Communication Technologies, Electronics and Applied Mathematics 2 In daily life, we manipulate objects that have different physical properties (mass, surface, shape). Most of the time, we succeed in this task and prevent the objects dropping from the hand. This is explained by the fine coordination between the load force (LF), the tangential force due to the object weight and inertia, and the grip force (GF), the normal force applied to the objects to counteract the LF. This fine coordination is possible with the help of the cutaneous feedback from the fingertips mechanoreceptors. These respond to the stresses and strains created by the deformation of the skin during object grasping and manipulation. Measuring the fingertip skin deformation before and during slipping may thus contribute to better understand how we can prevent the objects from slipping during their manipulation. In this study, we used an imaging system to record the fingertip deformation during tangential sliding movements of the fingertips on a glass. The constraints were applied passively to the fingertip of subjects (n=4) in six different directions : front-rear translations, left-right translations and clockwise-counterclockwise rotations. A robotic system controlled the normal force applied to the fingertip (three normal forces were tested : 0.5, 1 and 2 N) and the tangential speed (three speeds were tested: 5, 10 and 20 mm/s in translation and 20, 40 and 80 °/s in rotation). The contact forces and torques were recorded. Each condition was repeated three times. Using computer vision techniques, the contact area was extracted from the images and the relative velocity field between the glass and the fingertip contact area was computed during the whole sliding phase. An ellipse was fitted to the contact area during the whole slip in order to measure the displacement (for translations) and the tilt angle (for rotations) of the contact area. 34 Results showed that the skin deforms in a complex way from a full stuck state to a full slipping contact. During this transition, while the tangential force was monotonically increasing, the skin progressively slipped on the plate, beginning from the border of the contact area and ending at the central zone, and forming a velocity gradient from the border to the center. This behavior was observed for both translational and rotational movements. This transition from a full stuck state to a full slip was accompanied by a reduction of the contact area, which then stayed constant during full slip. The ratio between reduced area (during full slip) and full area (before the onset of the slip) is 0.70 ± 0.14 (mean ± std) for front-rear translations, 0.65 ± 0.15 for leftright translations and 0.90 ± 0.12 for rotations. The skin displacement, due to the stiffness of the fingertip, was measured by computing the displacement of the center of contact during the transition phase. This displacement was significantly larger in lateral movements (3.65 ± 1.49 mm) in comparison with front-rear movements (2.58 ± 1.01 mm). The direction of rotation did not affect the absolute value of the tilt angle significantly. 1-F-49 Observing lifting errors modulates corticospinal excitability Gavin Buckingham1, Jeremy D Wong1, Paul L Gribble1, Melvyn A Goodale1 1 The University of Western Ontario Using Transcranial Magnetic Stimulation (TMS) to induce Motor Evoked Potentials (MEPs) during passive observation, it has been demonstrated that merely watching an action modulates cortico-spinal excitability. This action observation system is often interpreted as a low-level mechanism to prepare the observer to perform the actions they are passively viewing. Recently, it has been demonstrated that the sensorimotor system encodes the fingertip force requirements of an observed lift - watching lifts of a heavy object elicits a larger MEP than observing lifts of a light object (Alaerts, Swinnen & Wenderoth, 2010, European Journal of Neuroscience). However, the feedforward way in which humans parameterize their fingertip forces means that errors are often made when lifting objects. Almost no research has examined the way in which the sensorimotor system responds to the passive observation of common overestimation- and underestimation-style lifting errors. To examine corticospinal modulation in response to the observation of errors, we applied single-pulse TMS to the left primary motor cortex of individuals while they passively watched others lift up large and small cubes of equal mass. In half of these videos, the actors made the errors that lifters normally make when confronted with these stimuli (i.e., overestimating the weight of the large cube and underestimating the weight of the small cube), whereas in the other half of the videos, the actors lifted the cubes with the appropriate (and identical) forces. When observing error-free performance, individuals' cortio-spinal excitability was tailored to the size of the cubes being lifted: watching lifts of the large cube elicited significantly larger MEPs than watching lifts of the (identically-weighted) small cube (p<.001). Thus, when the lifting kinematics were equivalent, cortico-spinal excitability was driven by object size. This size-related modulation appeared to be abolished when individuals observed the underestimation- and overestimation-style lifts of these same cubes: observing underestimation-style lifts of the small cube elicited an MEP of a similar magnitude to that seen when observing overestimation-style lifts of the large cube (p=.58). The elimination of this size-related modulation Poster Sessions Full Abstracts may be due to (1) the kinematics of the error lifts giving the visual impression that the cubes have different weights (i.e., that the large object appeared to be lighter than the small object), and/or (2) an automatic sensorimotor response to engage more appropriate grip and lift forces in the observer (i.e., preparing the observer to pre-empt the errors they have just observed). These suggestions are discussed in relation to the improvements seen in actual lifting behaviour after observing object lifting errors (Buckingham et al., 21st annual meeting of the Society for the Neural Control of Movement, 2011). 1-F-50 Asymmetrical Modulation of corticospinal excitability as a function of emotional sounds along with ear laterality Naeem Komeilipoor1, Fabio Pizzolato1, Andreas Daffertshofer2, Paola Cesari1 1 University of Verona, 2VU University Amsterdam Emotional sounds are essential to our survival as they give hue to the mental pictures of events, thus enable us to decipher the surroundings and form effective behavioral responses. Transcrainial Magnetic Stimulation (TMS) studies have evidenced the link between action readiness and emotional processing, reporting an increased activity in the motor cortex during emotional experiences. However, the majority of these studies employed preferentially visual and neglected audio stimuli. Moreover, data were usually recorded from just one hemisphere at the time. Hence, in the present study, we employed TMS to clarify whether non-verbal emotionally-characterized sounds processed separately by the left and right ear, would modulate motor evoked potential (MEP), recorded from hand muscle in an asymmetrical fashion. Overall, we found that the motor cortex significantly increased its activity in response to unpleasant as compared to pleasant and neutral sounds. Interestingly, the motor system excitability was asymmetrically modulated as a function of the stimulus valence; unpleasant stimuli resulted in a significant higher facilitation of motor potentials evoked in the left hemisphere, while pleasant stimuli yielded a higher activation in right one. Furthermore, TMS induced higher MEPs when listening to unpleasant sounds with the Left ear as compared to the Right ear, which complements previous findings indicating the left ear as selectively reactive to emotional contents. The increment of the left ipsilateral motor system excitability for the unpleasant sounds was presided over the dominant hand, suggests the presence of a biological preference for a direct motor-auditory pathway depicted to the processing of threatening auditory stimuli. This system might have developed to allow for faster movements revealing a more efficient way for a "fight or flight" reaction. Taken together, our findings contribute to the evolutionary perspective on the role of emotions on action readiness. 1-F-51 A neural basis for hand muscle synergy in primate spinal cord Kazuhiko Seki1, Tomohiko Takei1 1 National Institute of Neuroscience Grasping is a highly complex movement, which requires the control of 27 hand and arm muscles. Electromyographic (EMG) studies have shown that activities of the hand muscles during grasping can be explained with a combination of few basic components ("muscle synergy") that could be used by the CNS for the grasping control. However, the neural mechanism that is responsible for forming the muscle synergy is still unknown. Here we explored how the spinal premotor interneurons (PreM-INs) are involved in the forming of the muscle synergy. Twenty-three PreM-INs were recorded from lower cervical segments (C6-T1) in two macaque monkeys performing a precision grip task. EMG activities were simultaneously recorded from 12 finger and wrist muscles, and muscle synergies were extracted from them by using non-negative matrix factorization that decompose EMGs into predefined number of synergy weights and their temporal activities [1]. Coefficient of determinations indicated that a linear combination of three synergies account for the > 85% of variance of original EMGs. The distribution of the weights of each synergy was consistent within each monkey (R2=0.89±0.17). The postspike effects of PreM-INs on muscles were quantified by spike-triggered averaging (STA) of rectified EMGs. PreMINs showed divergent effects in more than one muscles (2.1±1.5 muscles), forming the muscle fields [2]. Comparison between the spatial distribution of synergy weight and muscle field of PreM-INs indicated that the muscle field of PreM-IN was preferentially matched with one of the extracted synergies (preferred synergy) and a similarity between neuron's muscle field and weight of preferred synergy was significantly higher than those expected from chance level. Moreover, the temporal activity of PreM-INs had a significantly higher correlation with the preferred synergy than that with the non-preferred synergies. These results suggest that the muscle synergy in primate grasping can be formed by the system that involves the spinal PreM-INs. [1] Cheung, V.C.K., et al., Stability of muscle synergies for voluntary actions after cortical stroke in humans. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA, 2009. 106(46): p. 195638. [2] Takei, T. and K. Seki, Spinal Interneurons Facilitate Coactivation of Hand Muscles during a Precision Grip Task in Monkeys. J Neurosci, 2010. 30(50): p. 17041-50. 1-F-52 A neural correlate of arousal in the subthalamic nucleus facilitates force production Anam Anzak1, Alek Pogosyan1, Huiling Tan1, Thomas Foltynie2, Patricia Limousin2, Ludvic Zrinzo2, Marwan Hariz2, Keyoumars Ashkan3, Wesley Thevathasan1, Marko Bogdanovic1, Alexander Green1, Tipu Aziz1, Peter Brown1 1 University of Oxford, 2Institute of Neurology, 3Kings College Hospital Loud auditory stimuli have been shown to increase force and rate of development of force in maximal hand grips in both healthy subjects and patients with Parkinson's disease (PD) (Anzak et al, 2011. Eur J Neurosci; 34(1) p.124-32). We reasoned that phasic arousal could be the underlying mechanism driving this phenomenon. To this end, electrophysiological evidence of a neural correlate of arousal, present in motor cortico-subcortical circuitry, and scaling with force improvements, was sought. Simultaneous local field potentials (LFPs) were recorded from surface EEG and bilateral deep brain stimulation (DBS) electrodes implanted in the subthalamic nuclei (STN) of 7 patients with PD, whilst off and on dopaminergic medication. Patients gripped a force dynamometer as quickly and strongly as possible in response to a visual cue, which was accompanied by an auditory tone at one of five different sound levels. Levels were selected at random and ranged from very quiet to very loud. An evoked potential, which increased in amplitude in response to louder auditory cues, was found to be focally generated in the STN. Its latency was very short (peak between 50-100ms from cue onset). Force and rate of development of force (averaged over 0-100ms from cue onset) also increased with louder cues. The amplitude of all three variables was similar whether patients were off or on dopaminergic medication. The peak amplitude of the evoked potential correlated with the average initial force 35 Poster Sessions Full Abstracts (spearman's ρ=0.359, P=0.002) and rate of development of force (ρ=0.307, P=0.010) across sound levels. 1-F-54 Artificially evoking physiological finger tremor suggests mechanical origin The evoked potential recorded in the STN was similar in morphology and latency to the P50 - an evoked potential normally recorded in surface EEG, and believed to be a correlate of ascending arousal originating from the cholinergic reticular activating system (Reese et al, 1995. Prog Neurobiol; 47(2) p.105-33). The independence of our STN P50 from L-DOPA state is in line with such an origin. This non-dopaminergic system may lend itself for therapeutic manipulation in PD, either pharmacologically or through mimicry of the evoked potential with a more targeted and specific form of DBS. Carlijn Vernooij1, Raymond F Reynolds1, Martin D Lakie1 1-F-53 Dynamic control of lower extremities declines with aging Emily Lawrence1, Veronica Stern1, Mark Lyle1, Carolee Winstein1, Philip Requejo2, Francisco J Valero-Cuevas1 1 University of Southern California, 2Rancho Los Amigos National Rehabilitation Center The ability to control ground reaction forces in the lower extremities is necessary for balance, locomotion and fall prevention. We examined the question of why older adults fall more than younger populations by assessing dynamic coordination of the lower extremities independently of muscle strength. We used the Lower Extremity Coordination (LEC) test-an extension of the Strength--‐Dexterity (S--‐D) test known to quantify dynamic finger control-- to measure dynamic control of the lower extremities. [1,2] The test consists of compressing a slender spring prone to buckling using the foot with the goal to sustain the highest force possible, where compression makes the spring increasingly more unstable. Compression force was measured a 1--‐axis load cell (Transducer Techniques, Temecula, CA) and data were sampled at 2000Hz, processed, and displayed as real--‐time feedback to the subject with custom MATLAB (The Mathworks, Natick, MA) software. To minimize the effects of leg strength and mitigate fatigue, we chose spring parameters (i.e. stiffness and slenderness) such that spring instability occurred at low forces. The largest sustained compression force is representative of the maximal sensorimotor ability to stabilize the leg in contact with the unstable surface at submaximal force levels. Nine young adults (25--‐35) and 15 older adults (55--‐95), all physically fit and active in fitness regimens or recreational sports, took part in this multi--‐site study. The average maximal compression forces for the best 3 out of 25 trials from each subject were lower for the older adults (111.32±10.3N vs. 124.93±9.3N; p<0.03). The maximal compression force recorded for the older adult population (average weight = 750N) [3] was approximately 15% of their body weight compared to 20% for the young adult population (average weight = 620N). This shows that the known propensity for falls in the older adults is also associated with reduced sensorimotor ability to stabilize the leg during dynamic tasks--and mirrors recent findings [1] that young women, who are at higher risk of non--‐contact ACL tears, have lower scores than age--‐matched young men. This novel detection of lower sensorimotor processing capabilities for leg control at low forces in older adults provides new insight into the potential neuromechanical contributors to falls, and opens new avenues to investigate their underlying mechanisms and clinical countermeasures. References: 1. Mark A. Lyle, Francisco J. Valero--‐Cuevas, Robert J. Gregor, Christopher M. Powers, The lower extremity dexterity test as a measure of leg dynamical capability, J Neurophys, In Review. 2. Valero-Cuevas FJ, Smaby N, Venkadesan M, Peterson M and Wright T., The strengthdexterity test as a measure of dynamic pinch performance. J Biomech. 2003 36(2): 265-270. 3. Health Calculator and Charts, 2011 Steven B. Halls Professional Corporation, August 2011 <www.halls.md>  36 1 University of Birmingham Introduction People cannot move their fingers smoothly or even hold them perfectly stationary; some degree of jerkiness is always present. Researchers studying what causes this physiological tremor can be roughly divided into two groups: those arguing for neural or spinal oscillations and those arguing for a mechanical resonance. This last view is strengthened by a recent paper demonstrating a decrease in the frequency of hand tremor post-movement unaccompanied by corresponding changes in EMG. We studied the mechanical resonance of the finger and associated muscles of healthy subjects by artificially evoking various sizes of tremor using white noise torques or electrical muscular stimulations as an input. Using this novel way of studying tremor, we could examine its amplitude and frequency over different movement sizes while bypassing the nervous system. Method Acceleration of the relaxed middle finger of eight healthy subjects was measured while we artificially evoked tremor by electrically stimulating the extensor digitorum communis muscle or by applying torques directly to the finger with a torque motor. We used six different intensities of white noise signals for both stimuli and torques to study tremor over a wide range of movement sizes. Intensities were individually determined based on acceleration response, which ranged from hardly visible to a response of ~7.5 m/s2. In addition, we included a condition in which subjects were asked to actively hold their finger in a middle position or track a very low frequency sine wave. When not stimulating the muscle, we also measured EMG of the extensor muscle. For each trial, we calculated the FFT of the white noise input, the measured acceleration and, where applicable, the rectified EMG. In addition, the input-to-acceleration gain and the EMG-to-acceleration gain were calculated. Results The white noise input signals all showed an evenly distributed frequency spectrum, which implies similar spectra for acceleration and gain. The frequency spectra of the input-to-acceleration gains resulting from both input modalities showed a peak at ~20 Hz for low intensities. Higher electrical stimulation intensities decreased this peak to ~8 Hz while higher torque intensities led to a broad spectrum between 8 and 20 Hz with a minor peak at 8 Hz. The EMG signals measured while applying torques had a flat frequency spectrum. Actively holding the finger in a middle position resulted in an EMG-to-acceleration gain peaked at ~20 Hz while the tracking task resulted in a peak at ~8 Hz. EMG spectra in these conditions were fairly flat with a slight peak around 12 Hz. Conclusion The presence of an acceleration frequency peak with a white noise input suggests that it is not caused by central oscillations. It seems likely that a resonance is involved. The fact that there are two distinct frequencies suggests that there may be two modes of resonance. We speculate that the high frequency mode involves the finger and tendon and the low frequency mode involves the muscle. We have shown that during movement wrist tremor frequency Poster Sessions Full Abstracts decreases. Finger tremor shows a reduction that is even bigger but more complicated. Controlling the finger in the transition from static to movement must therefore include big computational problems. 1-F-55 Stochastic resonance improves sensorimotor performance of the finger Rumyana Kristeva1, Ignacio Mendez-Balbuena1, Elias Manjarrez1, Juergen Schulte-Moenting2, Frank Huethe1, Joshua A Tapia3, Marie-Claude Hepp-Reymond4 1 Univ. Freiburg Dept. Neurology, 2Univ. Freiburg Inst. Biomed. Biometry, 3BUAP Instituto de Fisiologia, 4Inst. Neuroinf., Univ. Zürich and ETH Zürich Several studies about noise-enhanced balance control in humans support the hypothesis that stochastic resonance can enhance the detection and transmission in sensorimotor system during a motor task. The purpose of the present study was to extend these findings in a simpler and controlled task. We explored whether a particular level of a mechanical noise (0-15 Hz) applied on the index finger can improve the performance during compensation for a static force generated by a manipulandum. The finger position was displayed on a monitor as a small white point in the center of a green circle. We considered a good performance when the subjects exhibited a low deviation from the center of this circle and when the performance had less variation over time. Several levels of mechanical noise were applied on the manipulandum. By means of the mean variation in the position we observed an inverted U-like graph of the performance versus the input noise level in all subjects (8/8). We compared the performance between zero noise (ZN), optimal noise (ON) and high noise (HN). The mean variation was significantly lower during ON than during ZN or HN. The findings suggest that the application of a tactile-proprioceptive noise can improve the stability in sensorimotor performance via stochastic resonance. Possible explanations of this motor improvement can be a suppression of the physiological tremor and/or increased sensitivity of the muscle spindle afferents. 1-F-56 model Movement automatic guidance by direct internal Valerie Gaveau1, Olivier Sillan1, Calude Prablanc1 1 INSERM Movements planed in peripheral vision are inaccurate, but processes of correction occur during motor execution and guide the movement towards its goal (Goodale et al, 1986, Pelisson et al 1986, Prablanc & Martin 1992). This guidance is largely nonconscious, automatic and pre-emptive to voluntary control (Pisella et al 2000). In this study we want to test the hypothesis that this automatic guidance of the movement depends on a comparison between a predictive sensory feedback signal and an updated signal of the target position, rather than a comparison between the simultaneous visual information of the target and the hand. This prediction is the output signal of a "direct" internal model designed before movement execution and supplied both by initial information on the visual and kinesthetic position of the hand and by proprioceptivo-motor commands. Thus, under normal conditions, the predicted hand position and the target visual feedback signals integrated during the execution of the movement are consistent. In order to test the hypothesis of a closed relationship between automatic guidance of the hand and the output of the internal model of the same effector, we conducted a first experiment in which the internal model was biased prior to movement (by giving a visual distortion signal about the hand position); this misestimated position would lead to a distorted internal model representation of the hand. Then, at movement onset, normal vision of the hand was restored and we assessed whether the system is able or not to produce appropriate automatic corrections. Two hypotheses were put forward: 1) if the automatic guidance of the movement is based on internal model output, then guidance will be skewed because the predictor was flawed; 2) if automatic guidance and internal model are independent (e.g. guidance depend on comparison of the simultaneous visual information of target and hand), then guidance will be appropriate. To further test our predictions, we conducted a second experience in which internal model was not biased but we introduced a visual feedback discordance (the target will be displaced at movement onset). No comparison of the simultaneous visual information of target and hand was possible. Two alternative predictions follow: 1) if the error is automatically corrected in the absence of hand visual feedback and without lengthening movement duration, automatic correction must be attributed to the direct action of the internal model; 2) if the error is not corrected, the error processing requires a visual comparison between target and hand position. Our results demonstrate a tight link between automatic guidance of the hand and its internal representation: the automatic guidance is strongly dependent upon the integrity of the internal model of hand position. G - Theoretical & Computational Motor Control 1-G-76 Methods for the study of modularity in muscle activities: A unifying model for extracting spatial and temporal modules from EMG signals Bastien Berret1, Ioannis Delis1, Stefano Panzeri1, Thierry Pozzo1 1 Istituto Italiano di Tecnologia Motor control has long been hypothesized to be facilitated by the presence of modularity in the motor system, i.e., the brain may presumably generate a wide repertoire of movements by combining a small number of pre-existing modules. The nature of these modules has been examined from different viewpoints. In recent years, several studies have focused on modularity in muscle space, aiming to identify invariant patterns of muscle activity across a variety of motor behaviors. To do so, a number of efficient algorithms that perform dimensionality reduction on electromyography (EMG) signals have been developed. However, different models make different assumptions. The previously used models differ substantially in a) the invariant quantities that are assumed to be encoded in the central nervous system (CNS), b) the degree of compression of EMG data and c) their ability to capture motor variability across tasks and/or repetitions of the same task (i.e. what we call an episode). In the present study, we propose a new model giving a low-dimensional yet flexible and relevant representation of motor patterns. Our model is moreover compatible with most of the existing ones. We assume the existence of both spatial and temporal modules as the building blocks of motor control, in agreement with a huge amount of literature. Hence in our model any single muscle pattern can be expressed as a double linear combination of such modules. Low-dimensionality results from the fact that only a finite number of scalar parameters (combining N spatial modules with P temporal modules) are assumed to be controlled by high-level centers in order to generate an adequate motor pattern and achieve a given task at hand. Flexibility results from the double summation that allows 37 Poster Sessions Full Abstracts mixing temporal modules with any of the spatial modules and vice-versa. Relevance is because a number of neurophysiological studies have reported evidence for each type of module separately and that a large repertoire of tasks can indeed be performed reliably on a single-trial basis. The unifying aspect of the model is due to the fact that the spatial modules correspond to the so-called synchronous or time-invariant synergies and the temporal modules to the so-called muscle activation patterns, premotor drives or motor primitives. Furthermore, it can be shown that time-varying synergies, i.e. spatiotemporal muscle patterns, can actually be built from the spatial and temporal modules we assume, suggesting that they may constitute more primal building blocks of the CNS. We thus developed a specific algorithm based on an episode-based tri-factor non-negative matrix factorization method. It extracts simultaneously spatial and temporal modules from rectified EMG signals. The proof of convergence follows simply from existing theorems related to non-negative matrix factorization. The method has been successful when applied to synthetic data, proving its effectiveness. When applied to real EMG data recorded during goal-directed reaching movements, its performance was compared to other classical models. Results show that our model provides a good trade-off between lowdimensionality and biological plausibility in terms of single-trial task decoding scores. 1-G-77 Methods for the study of modularity in muscle activities: A single-trial task-decoding metric to evaluate muscle synergy models Ioannis Delis1, Bastien Berret1, Thierry Pozzo1, Stefano Panzeri1 1 Istituto Italiano di Tecnologia Muscle synergies, i.e., invariant coordinated activations of groups of muscles, have been proposed as the building blocks with which the central nervous system (CNS) constructs the patterns of muscle activity utilized for executing movements. In particular, the muscle synergy hypothesis states that the CNS performs a motor task by recruiting a certain number of synergies and combining them in the appropriate way. Several efficient algorithms that extract synergies via dimensionality reduction techniques have been developed in recent years. Yet, little is known about the extent to which the combination of those synergies can describe each individual task in a set given the variability in the data. Here we conceive and develop a novel computational framework to address this question. The procedure, which is based on singletrial task decoding from electromyography (EMG) recordings, quantifies how well the tasks can be identified based on a small given number of muscle synergies and determines the minimal set of synergies that describes all task-related variability of the activity of multiple muscles. We validate this method on plausibly simulated datasets, and we illustrate its merits on EMG data recorded during goal-directed reaching movements. We show that, unlike previous analysis methods, our algorithm succeeds in determining correctly and robustly the set of synergies needed to capture all task related variations of EMGs. We finally show how this method can be useful for comparing and/or validating different classes of muscle synergy models. 1-G-78 Safety margins and variability in a redundant object manipulation task Christopher Hasson1, Dagmar Sternad1 1 Northeastern University The human body is fundamentally redundant, with many more neurons, muscles, and joints than are necessary to perform most actions. Redundancy also exists at the task-level when there are multiple ways to reach a spatial goal in a given time, such as when placing a cup of coffee on a coaster. Although many movement strategies may lead to the same goal, some 38 accommodate task execution variability better than others through larger safety margins. How safety margins are shaped by redundancy and execution variability is addressed in a task that modeled transporting a cup of coffee. Eighteen subjects used a manipulandum to transport a virtual cup containing a ball ("coffee") to a target without losing the ball. Nine subjects were asked to complete the cup transit in a comfortable target time of two seconds (a redundant task by instruction, with infinite solutions), and nine were asked to transport the cup in minimum time (a non-redundant task with one explicit cost). Three hypotheses were tested: H1) in the minimum-time task subjects converge to a single optimal strategy, while in the target-time task they choose different strategies. H2) in the minimum-time task subjects decrease safety margins to optimize movement time, but in the target-time task they increase safety margins. H3) in both tasks subjects modulate safety margins according to their execution variability. The safety margin was defined as the ball energy relative to the ball's escape energy. Execution variability was quantified by the inter-trial standard deviation of the total energy of ball and cup. Results showed that both groups developed individualized strategies with practice (counter H1). The minimum-time group decreased their safety margins, while the target-time group increased safety margins (H2). In the target-time group changes in safety margins were correlated with changes in execution variability: smaller variability decreases over practice had larger safety margin increases (H3). In contrast, in the minimum-time group such a relation was observed only at the end of practice, not across practice. These results show that when learning a redundant object manipulation task subjects increase their safety margins and shape their movements in accordance with their changing variability. 1-G-79 Can we learn what the brain optimizes? Alexander Terekhov1, Vincent Hayward2 1 Université Pierre et Marie Curie (Paris 6), 2Institut des Systèmes Intelligents et de Robotique, UPMC – CNRS If we ask someone to reach her or his nose with the index finger, we do not expect her or him to wrap the arm beneath the knee, although this option is clearly feasible. Somehow, we find certain movements to be more appropriate than others. When there is something we want or need to do, we prefer to do it in a specific way. This observation suggests that the brain associates a cost with every movement and tends to minimize this cost during motor behavior. The question of what is being minimized by the brain has been examined for the last 50 years after having being first raised by Nubar and Contini (1961), who suggested a candidate function made of a weighted sum of squared moments. Since then, numerous candidate cost functions were proposed for the whole spectrum of motor activities and at different levels of analysis, ranging from the activation of the individual motor units within a single muscle to the whole body trajectory control during walking. The question at hand is typically addressed in terms of static optimization and/or optimal control problems in deterministic as well as in stochastic settings. It is somewhat disheartening that almost identical motor behaviors can result from different cost functions, each able to fit experimental data equally well. With this in mind, we ask the question of whether it is possible, at least hypothetically, to say for sure what the brain optimizes. Imagine for the sake of argument that we could measure the activation of every single mechanoreceptor and the stretch of every single muscle fiber in as many motor tasks as needed. Would this Poster Sessions Full Abstracts knowledge allow us to determine the cost function(s) used by the brain? If the answer is negative, then such result would immediately cast serious doubts on any attempt to find what the brain optimizes. On the other hand, a positive answer would encourage researchers to investigate the question in simpler tasks. The problem of finding a cost function from experimental data is called "inverse optimization". The central question addressed in the proposed presentation will be to discuss whether an inverse optimization problem can have a unique solution. We will formulate a theorem of uniqueness for a class of inverse optimization problem relevant to human motor control. This theorem provides conditions on experimental data whose satisfaction guarantees that the cost function can be determined unambiguously. The limits of applicability of the theorem will be also discussed. To illustrate the discussion we will consider the inverse optimization approach to describe behavior during prehension and multiple finger pressing tasks. We will provide examples of how the violation of the conditions of the theorem induces non-uniqueness in solving the problem. We will also report the recent results obtained for grasping. In particular, we will show how the cost function determined from the experimental data for the four-finger grasp can explain the experimental data for the three-finger grasp. It will be concluded that yes, theoretically, cost functions optimized by the brain can be found. The question of their meaning, however, must be left to the researcher. 1-G-80 Implementation of stochastic feedback control and Bayesian nonlinear filtering in spiking neuron populations Atiyeh Ghoreyshi1, Terence Sanger1 1 University of Southern California States of things in nature, such as the position or velocity of an object, or the angle of one's elbow, are usually continuous in time and in space. Their neural representations in the brain, however, are not continuous either in time or in space. External variables are coded in the brain by spikes in populations of neurons. Thus, there is a limitation in both spatial and temporal resolutions of neural representations. Besides, we often encounter "unfamiliar" or "uncertain" situations, in which we have to observe, decide, and navigate. What makes our brain so highly efficient and accurate in communicating with and navigating through various environments, and yet, most elaborate robots with ample resources fail when put in a fairly noisy environment? This makes one wonder if our traditional structure of thinking about estimation and control as such is too "rigid" in handling uncertainty or stochasticity, which is an intrinsic property of neural behavior and response. A new framework for dealing with such problems has been developing recently(1). In this framework, states of a system are considered random variables represented by their probability density functions (pdfs) rather than as explicit deterministic variables, and all estimation and control operations are performed on state pdfs. The Fokker-Planck equation is used for the forward propagation of pdfs in time, the Zakai/Kushner's type equations for incorporating observations into the pdf temporal propagation, and proper control parameters are calculated given specific control operators and cost functions. This framework is powerful and efficient in handling nonlinear behavior, uncertainty, and ambiguity in the system or environment(1). In this work, we demonstrate how computations on pdfs can be implemented in populations of spiking neurons and what can be achieved doing so. Our neural network includes a receptor layer that provides state observations; a sensory cortex layer that, through nonlinear Bayesian filtering, computes internal state estimates; and a motor cortex layer that calculates motoneuron commands via stochastic feedback control calculations involving the cost function, current internal state estimates, and control operators. For the first time, we have combined a spike-based stochastic estimation algorithm with a spike-based controller. We show that this combination provides efficient and smooth control in the presence of state and parameter uncertainty, and can provide on-line optimization of trajectories with respect to a known cost function. The real-time calculation of control variables and desired trajectories greatly reduces the required computational resources. We demonstrate realtime tracking simulation results, where the actuator accurately follows a time-varying trajectory based on a given cost/utility function. We also demonstrate how our implementation predicts behavioral motor control experimental results, such as Trommershauser's experiments(2). These examples suggest the plausibility of this approach both in theory and in practice, in terms of understanding and utilizing the underlying mechanisms of human motor control. (1) T. D. Sanger (2011). "Distributed Control of Uncertain Systems Using Superpositions of Linear Operators". Neural Computation, 23 (8), 1911-1934. (2) J. Trommershäuser, L. T. Maloney, & M. S. Landy (2003). "Statistical decision theory and trade-offs in the control of motor response". Spatial vision, 16(3-4), 255-275. 1-G-81 Learning rate modulation vs. dimensionality reduction as a mechanism for structural learning Alkis Hadjiosif1, Maurice A Smith1 1 Harvard University The rate in which we learn to adapt to new environments can be greatly affected by our previous experiences with similar environments. Studying point-to-point reaching movements subjected to visuomotor (VM) transformations, Braun et al. (2009) found that exposing subjects to a series of randomly varying visuomotor rotations could facilitate learning subsequent visuomotor rotations but not visuomotor shears. The authors described this as an example of structural learning: repeated exposure to transformations of the same class - there, rotations of varying angles - reveals the underlying structure of the class, specifically facilitating adaptation to that class. The idea put forward was that transformations within a single class represent a low dimensional manifold within the high dimensional space of all possible transformations that could be encountered, and that learning the structure of the class (i.e. the location of the low dimensional manifold) could make the process of within-class motor adaptation considerably more efficient by reducing the dimensionality of the learning problem. An alternative to this idea is that the rate of learning is modulated - facilitated or inhibited - by the statistical features of the experienced environment. Here we tested two differential predictions of these competing hypotheses: First, the statistics of the environment would predict that negatively correlated versus positively correlated switching between different VM rotation angles should produce different changes in the rate of learning for VMs. In contrast, dimensionality reduction would predict that both should facilitate VM rotation learning because the environment would be limited to a single class of transformations in both cases. We thus compared adaptation rates before and after exposure to a VM rotation structure in two conditions: one characterized by a strong correlation between the imposed transformation from one trial to the next (PC condition) and another one in which the transformation in one trial was negatively correlated to the next (NC condition). The PC condition showed a substantial upregulation of rotation learning rates (mean±st. error across subjects: 0.129±0.014 vs. 0.314±0.066, p<0.015), whereas the NC condition showed no difference in learning 39 Poster Sessions Full Abstracts rates for rotation before and after the inconsistent structure (0.080±0.014 vs. 0.084 ±0.011, p>0.7). This suggests that the statistical properties of the exposure to a structure rather than the mere presence of exposure to the structure determines whether learning rate facilitation can occur. If so, a prediction would be that the dimensionality reduction associated with exposure to a structure should not play key role in learning rate facilitation. We then looked at the ability of an environmental exposure to facilitate learning rate increases for transformations outside of the experienced structure for the PC condition. We found that when transformations consisting of various combinations of gain changes and rotations were studied, transformations away from the learned structure containing non-zero gain changes showed similar facilitation of learning rates compared to pure rotation transformation (p>0.42 between all cases). This shows that learning rate facilitation was not limited to the learned structure. Together these findings suggest that environmental statistics rather than dimensionality reduction accounts for the structural learning of visuomotor transformations. 1-G-82 Augmentation of perceived visual error improves control and enhances retention of a discrete task Dagmar Sternad1, Meghan E Huber1, Anastasia Kyvelidou1 1 Northeastern University Previous studies show that the control of movement, in particular its accuracy and variability, benefit from learning under erroraugmented conditions. The present study examined how the enhancement of perceived visual error can increase accuracy and decrease variability in motor performance. Importantly, we show how this improved performance persists for five days. Using a virtual throwing task, skittles, subjects manipulated a lever arm with a single-joint elbow movement and released a virtual ball that traversed a concentric force field to hit a target. The ball's trajectory and hitting success were fully determined by the release angle and velocity at ball release. The task is redundant as different combinations of release angle and velocity lead to identical results and the set of successful solutions describes a nonlinear manifold. Previous work showed that subjects' variability aligns with the direction of the solution manifold and remains non-zero even in skilled performers that reached a plateau in result accuracy and variability. We tested whether 1) skilled subjects could further improve their strategy when the perceived visual error is augmented and whether 2) this improvement persists when augmentation is removed. To manipulate perception of visual error the threshold of a success signal about the target center was decreased. Twelve subjects trained under normal task conditions until their skill performance reached a plateau (session 1, 3 days); subjects trained with the augmented visual error perception (session 2, 3 days); augmentation was removed (session 3, 5 days). Two hypotheses were tested: 1) Skilled subjects decrease their variability and increase their accuracy in session 2 as compared to session 1 in order to compensate for the augmentation. 2) Subjects return to their initial more variable and less accurate strategy in session 3 when task constraints were relaxed by removing the augmentation. Results showed that subjects improved their accuracy and reduced their internal variability to compensate for the tighter task constraints. In contrast to hypothesis 2, however, subjects maintained their low variability and high accuracy for 5 days when the success threshold was reestablished to its initial value. These results suggest that subjects are sensitive to their variability and reduce it in the presence of tighter task demands. Importantly, the effects of increased accuracy and reduced variability persist even after removal of this enhanced visual error perception. These results have implications for training and rehabilitation as signal 40 detection and control processes in the sensorimotor system may be enhanced with augmented visual error perception. 1-G-83 Interaction of sensory uncertainty and motor variability during reaching: A simulation study Gregory Apker1, Christopher Buneo1 1 Arizona State University Reaching movements are inherently variable, a result of noise in sensorimotor processing associated with the planning and execution of limb movements. In this context, planning related noise represents variability in the encoding of reaching parameters associated with specifying a motor plan, dependent in part on the anisotropic nature of sensory uncertainty. Execution noise describes variability in the processes associated with generating the appropriate motor output, and manifests largely as reaching variability along the movement vector. During natural reaching, these noise processes interact throughout movement to influence patterns of variability. Growing behavioral evidence suggests that the brain coordinates sensorimotor processes to minimize this variability, which has fostered the belief that the brain exploits the characteristics of sensory and motor noise to optimize reaching performance. To evaluate this claim, we developed a feedback control model augmented with a Kalman filter to assess the influence of anisotropic sensory uncertainty (planning noise) on endpoint control of reaching. Multiple simulations were performed with distinct characteristics of feedback variability: Zero feedback noise, isotropic feedback noise, and noise representative of known visual and proprioceptive feedback uncertainty. In addition, the model was developed to integrate multiple feedback inputs to assess the effects of multimodal feedback control on movement variability, and thus additional simulations were performed to evaluate the effect of optimal feedback integration on endpoint variability. Simulated reaching performance with isotropic feedback noise yielded patterns of variable error similar to those expected of pure execution noise (zero feedback noise). On the other hand, anisotropic feedback noise significantly affected predicted endpoint variability. Specifically, the orientation of 95% confidence ellipses calculated for simulated endpoints indicated a combined effect of feedback variability and execution noise. Conversely, calculated aspect ratios of these ellipses reflected a strong influence of the characteristics of feedback noise. Multimodal feedback control also produced distinct patterns of endpoint variability. In fact, predicted performance in this condition reflected a unique combination of feedback and execution noise. In all cases, total error volume was consistent with a ?near optimal? combination of planning and execution noise (Faisal and Wolpert, 2009). These results suggest that under optimal sensorimotor control endpoint variability arises as a result of the interaction of sensory/planning noise with execution noise. Further, we found that this interaction is significantly affected by both the size and the anisotropic nature of sensory feedback uncertainty. 1-G-84 Synaptic changes in strength at the hippocampal formation during the acquisition of classical eyeblink conditioning in behaving rabbits Alejandro Carretero1, Renny Pacheco1, Jose Maria Delgado1, Agnès Gruart1 1 División de Neurociencia Despite of the high number of works focused in elucidating the role of the hippocampus in learning and memory processes, this point is still a matter of discussion. In this Poster Sessions Full Abstracts work we show data of hippocampal recordings from conscious rabbits during the classical eyeblink conditioning. Briefly, four groups of rabbits were used, one of them for baseline recordings, one for pseudoconditioning, and the other two for classical eyeblink conditioning using delay or trace paradigms. The conditioned stimulus (CS) consisted of a tone, whilst the unconditioned stimulus (US) consisted of an air puff. In addition, two more groups of rabbits were prepared. In one of them, electrophysiological recordings of hippocampal field EPSPs (fEPSPs) were carried out whitout applying any conditioning stimulus, to see the putative effects of enviromental influences at the different hippocampal synapses (Baseline); and, in the other, a pseudoconditioning paradigm was used (CS and US stimuli presented at random) to check if uncoupled stimuli evoked the same synaptic changes than in their coupled presentation. A pair of electrical pulses were applied to the perforant path (PP), or CA3, during the CS and before the US in the case of delay paradigm, or during the CS-US interval for trace paradigm, in the CS for pseudoconditioning, and in a fixed interval of time for baseline. The slope of evoked fEPSPs did not change across baseline sessions. In contrast, an increase, or a decrease, tendency was observed in conditioning sessions depending on the synapse and on the paradigm used. Surprisingly, the pseudoconditioning protocol provoked changes in the PP to DG, CA3, CA1 and in the CA3 to CA1 and cCA1 synapses, similar in magnitude to these obtained with the trace paradigm, but with relevant differences in some nodal points across the acquisition curve. These results show that a constant context do not evoke significant changes in strength at the hippocampal synapses, in contrast with changes evoked by relevant but uncoupled stimuli, and those evoked because of the associative learning proccess. 1-G-85 Correlates of desirability in the primary motor cortex of primates Joseph Francis1, Brandi Marsh1, Marcello DiStasio1, Aditya Tarigoppula1 1 SUNY Downstate Medical School Reward-modulated neural activity is an important component of conditioned behavior, motor planning, and plasticity, with ample evidence of its influence on behavior and physiology. Signals of reward conditions are observed across the motor system, interacting with systems governing action selection, trajectory planning, and motivation to generate motor efforts. These signals also offer the possibility of use as performance feedback to unsupervised controllers whose goal is to maximize reward. Such a controller is much more flexible in its ability to choose component actions that achieve larger goals than one trained with a supervised learning algorithm. Though this type of controller can quite reasonably be hypothesized to exist in the primate brain, here we propose its use in-silico in a brain-machine interface (BMI) paradigm. A BMI operating under the control of a reinforcement-learning (RL) agent requires defined rewards that the agent's goal is to maximize. We suspect that brain-derived signals of states' relative desirabilities may be able to serve as useful rewards to an RL agent that has control over movements of a robotic system. We will present results indicating the presence of reward signals in the ensemble activity of neurons recorded in the primary motor cortex of macaques performing a delayed center-out reaching task, where a color cue informs the animal during the delay period if it will be a rewarded trial or not. We pruned all of our data before further analysis so that there were no significant differences in kinematics between these two types of movements. By examining mean spiking activity of M1 neurons over pre- and post-movement time periods in principle component space, we demonstrate a clear decision boundary separating rewarded and non-rewarded movements. We also demonstrate the feasibility of using functional near-infrared spectroscopy (fNIRS) to access hemodynamic signals of state desirabilities, a source of complementary reward signals for an RL algorithm, which we have found in both the monkeys and humans. This work establishes a set of signals that are available in commonly-used BMI systems (motor system neural spike recordings and non-invasively recorded hemodynamic activity) whose properties we aim to define further, allowing for higher-rate feedback of richer information to RL motor prosthetic interfaces. These signals are of known biological importance, and exploring their utility in BMI research will also contribute to our understanding of the natural process of motor planning. 1-G-86 A context-dependent process mediates decay in motor adaptation James Ingram1, J Randall Flanagan2, Daniel M Wolpert1 1 University of Cambridge, 2Queen's University Motor learning has been extensively studied using novel force-fields which perturb the arm during reaching movements and induce adaptation of the motor commands. This adaptation is context-specific, being confined to the movement direction in which the perturbation is experienced, with limited generalization to novel directions. Several state-space models have been developed to capture this pattern of adaptation, modeling how errors experienced in one movement direction lead to the progressive acquisition of a context-specific representation of the perturbing dynamics. Recently, we reported a similar pattern of context-specific adaptation to the familiar dynamics of everyday objects. In our task, subjects rotate a virtual hammer-like tool around the grasp point. In this case, adaptation involves learning the parameters of the dynamics (such as the mass), allowing subjects to keep the grasp point stationary during the rotation. When the object is presented at different visual orientations, adaptation is confined to the local orientation at which the dynamics are experienced, with limited generalization to novel orientations. Previously, we developed a multiple-context state-space model, with a generalization function tuned to visual object orientation, which reproduced this contextdependent behavior. Existing state-space models of motor adaptation typically include two update terms which describe how the adaptation state changes from one trial to the next. The first term, the retention factor, determines how much of the adaptation state from the current trial is carried over to the next trial. Its value is always less than one, such that the adaptation state tends to decay passively from one trial to the next. The second term, the learning rate, determines how much of the error from the current trial is used to update the adaptation state on the next trial. In order to capture context-dependent behavior, multiple adaptation states are modeled. In this case, the error is weighted by a context-dependent tuning function, such that errors have the greatest influence on the state associated with the current context, decreasing progressively for contexts which are further removed. Thus, the contextdependent behavior of previous models has been implemented by applying a contextual tuning function to the learning rate whereas the passive trial-by-trial decay associated with the retention factor has been assumed to be context independent. Using our virtual object manipulation task, we tested this assumption that the decay associated with the retention factor is context independent. Subjects adapted to the dynamics of the object at a single orientation and then performed multiple blocks of error-clamp trials at one of five probe orientations, including the original training orientation and four novel orientations. After each probe 41 Poster Sessions Full Abstracts block, subjects were re-exposed to the dynamics at the training orientation. Because kinematic errors are essentially eliminated on error-clamp trials, any de-adaptation induced by probe blocks must be the result of passive trial-by-trial decay. Contrary to the assumption of all previous models, we found that de-adaptation was greatest for probe blocks presented at the training orientation, decreasing progressively as the relative probe orientation increased. These results show that context-dependent processes, traditionally thought to apply only to error-driven adaptation, also mediate decay in motor adaptation. 1-G-87 Evolution of grip and object representations in the MI-PMv circuit: A neural trajectory analysis 1 1 2 Carlos Vargas-Irwin , Lachlan L Franquemont , Michael J Black , John P Donoghue1 1 Brown University Neuroscience Department, 2Max Planck Institute for Intelligent Systems Dimensionality reduction applied to neural ensemble data has led to the concept of a 'neural trajectory': a low-dimensional representation of the evolution of the network state over time (for example, see Afshar et al. 2011). We have developed a novel algorithm to generate neural trajectories based on spike train similarity. The algorithm combines spike train similarity metrics (Victor & Purpura, 1996) with stochastic neighbor embedding using t-distributions (van der Maaten and Hinton, 2008) to map spiking patterns into a low-dimensional 'similarity space'. In this space, network activity at a given time can be represented by a single point, and distances between the points denote similarities between ensemble firing patterns. In our approach the low dimensional space is defined in terms of relative similarity measures between a given spike train and a set of baseline spike trains recorded at relevant times during the task. This method allows us to use a set of key spike trains as landmarks that define the neural space in a task-relevant manner. Time-evolving neural activity can then be tracked in relation to these known points. We have applied this technique to track the evolving states of neuronal ensembles in PMv and MI through different phases of a cued grasping with instructed delay (CGID) task. In this task, two different objects are presented using a turntable. Each object can be lifted using two different grip strategies (including power grip, which is common to both objects). By illuminating the object for one second before providing the grip instruction, we can separate neural responses associated with object presentation from those associated with choosing a grip strategy. The resulting neural trajectories for PMv display a basic re-organization of the neural space reflecting the information available to the monkey at different time points: During the object presentation phase, neural trajectories associated with the same object grow closer together. We interpret this pattern as the activation of combinations of grip affordances associated with the target object. Once the grip cue is provided, the neural trajectories re-arrange themselves, transitioning from an object-based to a grip-based organization (with similar grips producing more closely matched activity patterns). We interpret this pattern as the selection of a particular grip strategy from the possible grip affordances invoked by the visual presentation of the object. The PMv neural trajectories can be contrasted with similar plots derived from MI data. In MI, responses to the visual presentation of the object are reduced. The neural trajectories from this area therefore tend to remain undifferentiated until the grip strategy is chosen, without going through the object-related clustering observed in PMv. There is still grip related information during the delay period, but it tends to emerge at a later stage than PMv. This suggests that information transfer between PMv and MI mainly happens only after a grip strategy has been selected from among the possible grip affordances evoked by the target object. Our results also show that similar grips aimed at different objects are represented by 42 statistically different network states. This observation could be explained by a partial activation of the competing grip affordances associated with the target object. 1-G-88 Trial-by-trial assessment of separate learning processes during motor adaptation Jordan Taylor1, Melissa A Burney1, Jacob L Wilson1, John W Krakauer2, Richard B Ivry1 1 University of California, Berkeley, 2Johns Hopkins University Visuomotor rotations have commonly been employed to assess trial-by-trial adaptation of the motor system. Recent work suggests that multiple learning processes operate in visuomotor adaptation paradigms. In addition to error-based processes associated with adaptation of a visuomotor map, participants may employ strategies to facilitate task performance. Recent studies show that in such conditions, error-based learning continues to operate, even though this may lead to a deterioration in task accuracy. Modeling work suggests that this phenomenon arises because adaptation processes use an error signal defined by the difference between expected and actual sensory feedback, whereas strategic processes are modified with information related to measures of task accuracy. The issue of strategy change has been generally neglected in the motor learning literature. Insight into this problem may benefit by considering work in decision-making, and in particular, the contribution of exploratory behavior to maximize reward. Strategy change and exploration both involve sampling the environment to uncover the relationship between a potential action and its outcome. In the current study, we systematically manipulated information in the visual display to make obvious the relationship between visuomotor rotations and potential strategies that could offset the rotation. Participants learned to make 10 cm reaching movements to visual targets with a 45° visuomotor rotation. In a control condition, a singletarget was presented, similar to displays used in most adaptation studies. For the other two conditions, the workspace included visual landmarks -- circles that surrounded the target location, and the spacing between the landmarks was either sparse or dense. In all three conditions, vision of the limb was occluded and only endpoint feedback was provided in the form of a cursor when the hand passed 10 cm. No explicit instructions were given about the rotation or strategies that might facilitate learning. While the presence of landmarks did not appear to affect the final state of adaptation (similar aftereffect magnitude), there were notable differences during training. In the two conditions with landmarks, performance gains were significantly faster. Moreover, the trial-by-trial variability differed between groups, with variance being largest for the groups with landmarks, suggestive of an exploratory strategic process. This strategic process was sensitive to the spacing of the visual landmarks, with sparse spacing associated with high trial-by-trial variance and dense spacing associated with low trial-by-trial variance. These results suggest that the time course of the strategic process is influenced by the training environment, offering a method to manipulate strategy-use directly during training. We will discuss how trial-by-trial strategy adjustment can be observed independently from prediction error-based learning and incorporated into models of visuomotor adaptation. Poster Sessions Full Abstracts SESSION 2 A - Adaptation & Plasticity in Motor Control 2-A-1 Cerebellar rTMS disrupts fast learning motor adaptation process Robert Hardwick1, Jon S Kennedy1, Chris Miall1 1 University of Birmingham, UK Smith, Ghazizadeh and Shadmehr (2006: PLoS Biology) propose that two interacting processes contribute to short term adaptation of reaching movements; a 'fast process' that is quick to respond to error but has poor retention, and a 'slow process' that has a weak response to error but strong retention. While behavioural evidence indicates that the fast process shares resources with the declarative memory system (Keisler and Shadmehr, 2010: Journal of Neuroscience), our understanding of which brain structures contribute to these processes is still relatively limited. Here we present data from an rTMS experiment indicating that the cerebellum is a key contributor to the fast process. Participants interacted with a robotic arm manipulandum to control the movements of an on screen cursor, performing point to point reaching movements from a near start position to a distant target. After receiving 10 minutes of rTMS over either the vertex (in a control condition) or the right lateral cerebellum, participants performed reaching movements under differing curl fields induced by the robot. In accordance with previously established protocols (Smith, Ghazizadeh and Shadmehr, 2006: Keisler and Shadmehr, 2010), participants were first exposed to a long block of 120 clockwise curl field trials, after which they completed a brief second block of 15 counterclockwise curl field trials. Curl field adaptation and after effects were assessed using error clamp trials; the robot generated a virtual wall that only allowed movement in a straight line from the start position to the target location, and the forces that participants produced against the wall were measured to determine the extent of their adaptation. Single and dual state models were fit to the error clamp trials collected from both the vertex control group and cerebellar stimulation group. The Akaike information critereon corrected (AICc) was calculated for the two models based upon chi-squared best fits for each participant. These data were subjected to repeated measures analysis for each model type (single or dual state) for the two groups (vertex and cerebellum). This analysis revealed that while the use of a dual state model was justifiable for the vertex control group, a single state model was better able to explain performance of the cerebellar stimulation group. Further analysis indicated that cerebellar stimulation specifically disrupted the activity of the fast process that normally underlies motor adaptation. These data indicate that the cerebellum is a key contributor to the fast process that underlies normal motor adaptation, and the experimental approach utilised here highlights the ability of neurostimulation techniques to identify brain structures that contribute to modelled neural processes. 2-A-2 Transfer of motor learning between the two arms Robert van Beers1, Eli Brenner1, Jeroen B Smeets1 1 VU University Amsterdam When we learn a movement task with one hand, does this learning transfer to the other hand? Several studies have addressed this question by examining how learning to make reaching movements in the presence of a perturbation with one hand transfers to the other hand. The results depend on the perturbation used. Whereas transfer is generally large for visuomotor perturbations (e.g., Imamizu and Shimojo 1995), transfer is at most partial for dynamic perturbations (e.g., Criscimagna-Hemminger et al. 2003; Galea et al. 2007). These studies also found conflicting results about the coordinates (intrinsic vs. extrinsic) in which transfer occurred. This suggests that the amount and coordinate system of transfer are specific for the perturbation used. The aims of the present study were to determine whether there is also intermanual transfer that occurs independent of perturbations, and if so, whether this occurs in intrinsic or extrinsic coordinates. We therefore modified the paradigm developed by van Beers (2009) to study intermanual transfer in the absence of perturbations. Ten subjects made reaching movements from starting positions to visual targets. They could not see their hand during the movements, but received visual feedback about the movement endpoint immediately after each movement. The experiment consisted of blocks of 50 movements in which they alternated between movements with the right and the left hand. The start and target location for each hand were the same for all 25 movements in a block. To determine whether transfer occurred in intrinsic or extrinsic coordinates, we tested two target layouts. In the Symmetric condition, start and target locations for the two hands were mirror images of each other relative to the body midline. In the Parallel condition, start and target locations were the same for both hands, except for a slight lateral offset. We analyzed the autocorrelation of the movement endpoints to infer whether an error made by one hand led to an adjustment of the planning of the next movement of the other hand. The lag 1 autocorrelation expresses the statistical relationship between the endpoints of one hand and the consecutive endpoints of the other hand. In the Symmetric condition, we found negative lag 1 autocorrelations for both the extent and the direction of the movements. In the Parallel condition, the lag 1 autocorrelation of the movement extent was also negative, but that of the movement direction was zero. Since a negative lag 1 autocorrelation indicates transfer of error correction between the hands, these results provide evidence for intermanual transfer of learning in intrinsic coordinates. The negative autocorrelation for only the movement extent in the Parallel condition is consistent with this as the target movements for the two hands in this condition had the same amplitude but different directions. We conclude that there is error-driven intermanual transfer of motor learning in the absence of perturbations, and this transfer occurs in intrinsic coordinates. References Criscimagna-Hemminger SE, Donchin O, Gazzaniga MS, Shadmehr R (2003) J Neurophysiol 89, 168176 Galea JM, Miall RC, Woolley DG (2007) Exp Brain Res 182: 267-273 Imamizu H, Shimojo S (1995) J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform 21, 719-733 van Beers RJ (2009) Neuron 63, 406-417 2-A-3 Sensori-motor adaptation to a novel force field does not transfer to the non-exposed arm in aged adults and a proprioceptively-deafferented patient Fabrice Sarlegna1, Marvin Dufrenne2, Lionel Bringoux2, Jean-Louis Vercher1, Christophe Bourdin2 1 CNRS & Aix-Marseille University, 2Aix-Marseille University Everyday, human adults display a surprising ability to adapt to multiple changes, in the environment or in their own sensorimotor system, which may perturb the efficiency of the behaviour. In the present study, we assessed the roles 43 Poster Sessions Full Abstracts of visual and proprioceptive signals to adapt to a change in the dynamic properties of the upper limb. It is well established that proprioception is an important source of information for the maintenance of an adapted control of limb dynamics (Ghez & Sainburg 1995; Sober & Sabes 2005). However, it remains unclear whether proprioception is crucial for the generalization and the transfer of newly learned patterns of coordination. Thus, the starting question of our study was "Does interlimb transfer of force-field adaptation depend on intact proprioception?". We addressed this issue by testing the ability of older adults, whose proprioception is known to be impaired relative to young adults (Goble et al. 2009), and a deafferented patient who has been deprived of limb proprioception for years (Cooke et al. 1985). All subjects were asked to seat on a platform and reach with their right, dominant hand toward visual targets with full vision. The platform could rotate and thus create a novel force field (Lackner & DiZio 1994) as the Coriolis force acted on the moving arm. We mainly replicated the procedure described by DiZio & Lackner (1995) and tested the left hand performance in the normal force field before (pre-test) and after (post-test) the right hand was exposed to the altered force field for 90 reaching movements (platform rotation = 120°/s). Our preliminary findings extend those reported by DiZio & Lackner (1995) by showing that in young adults, sensori-motor adaptation transfers to the non-exposed arm, supporting a great body of literature. Thus, in the present study, young adults saw, and felt, their first reaching movements with the right hand being perturbed by the altered force field. Young adults rapidly adapted and restored a straight hand path to the target despite the platform rotation. Once the rotation stopped, young adults had to reach with the left hand. Here, initial movement direction was shifted with respect to that in the pre-test (before rotation). In contrast, we observed no such interlimb transfer when the sixty-year old deafferented patient was tested, even though she was able to rapidly adapt to the altered force field with vision (see also Sarlegna et al. 2010). Moreover, no significant interlimb transfer was observed in healthy, sixty-year old subjects (who also were able to adapt to the altered force field). In other words, there was no shift in initial movement direction of the left hand between the pre- and the post-test for any of the sixty-year old subjects. Before elaborating on the factors that could explain the observed difference in interlimb transfer between young and older adults and the possible role of proprioception, we need to determine whether the lack of shift in left hand movement direction represents a failure to transfer sensorimotor adaptation due to ageing or, in contrast, whether the absence of after-effects highlight the expertise of older adults to use contextual cues to avoid performing "maladapted" reaching arm movements in the normal force field. 2-A-4 More realignment for imposed than for naturally occurring biases Jeroen Smeets1, Katinka van der Kooij1, Robert J van Beers1, Willemijn D Schot1, Eli Brenner1 1 VU University Does the nervous system make lasting corrections for intersensory mismatches? Conflicting answers to this question have been given. Research imposing a sensory mismatch has provided evidence that the nervous system realigns the senses, reducing the mismatch. At the same time, research exploiting natural intersensory biases provided evidence that the nervous system does not realign the senses. It is unclear whether this difference is due to a difference in experimental approach or whether corrections to natural and imposed biases are different. Here, we directly compare how the nervous system corrects for natural biases and imposed mismatches. Subjects moved a hand-held cube to virtual cubes appearing at random locations in 3D space. We alternated test blocks where subjects moved in complete darkness with 44 feedback blocks where we rendered a cube based on the position of the hand-held cube. The first test block allowed us to measure natural biases, whereas subsequent test blocks allowed us to measure realignment to feedback. In feedback blocks, we imposed an eye-centered rotation of plus or minus five degrees on the visual feedback, creating a mismatch between vision and proprioception. We either provided feedback during the movement (continuous feedback) or after the movement had ended (terminal feedback). In this paradigm, endpoint errors are caused by a combination of natural biases and the imposed rotation. Taking advantage of the imposed rotations (-5, 5) canceling each other, we could decompose errors into a component in the direction of a subject's natural bias and a component in the direction of the imposed rotation. We found that there was much more realignment for the imposed mismatch than for the natural biases. This difference in realignment was found with terminal as well as with continuous feedback. Thus, the nervous system corrects differently for imposed and natural mismatches. 2-A-5 Transcranial direct-current stimulation (tDCS) over somatosensory cortex modulates synaptic mechanisms involved in classical eyeblink conditioning in rabbits Javier Márquez-Ruiz1, Rocío Leal-Campanario1, Raudel Sánchez-Campusano1, Claudia Ammann1, Behnam MolaeeArdekani2, Fabrice Wendling2, Giulio Ruffini3, Agnès Gruart1, José María Delgado-García1 1 University Pablo de Olavide, 2INSERM, U642 - Université de Rennes, 3Starlab Barcelona SL Transcranial direct-current stimulation (tDCS) is a noninvasive brain stimulation technique that has been successfully applied for modulation of cortical excitability. Although the effects of weak direct-current stimulation on the excitability of the central nervous system were reported decades ago in acute animal experiments, it is only during recent years that its clinical use in humans has been promoted. TDCS is capable to induce changes of neuronal membrane potentials in a polarity-dependent way. When tDCS is long enough synaptically-driven after-effects are induced. Since mechanisms underlying these effects are largely unknown, the development of experimental animal models to test the immediate and after-effects induced by tDCS at different cortical areas and its implications in complex cerebral processes is compellingly needed. In order to address these objectives, we determined in a first series of experiments whether simultaneous tDCS applied to the somatosensory cortex could modify the characteristics of local field potentials (LFPs) evoked in the vibrissa S1 area of alert behaving animals by air-puff stimulation of the contralateral whisker pad or electrical stimulation of the ipsilateral ventroposterior medial (VPM) thalamic nucleus. Here we show in behaving rabbits that tDCS applied over the somatosensory cortex modulates cerebral cortical processes consequential to localized stimulation of the whisker pad or of the corresponding area of the ventroposterior medial (VPM) thalamic nucleus. Longer stimulation periods indicate that post-stimulation effects were only observed in the somatosensory cortex after cathodal tDCS. In a second series of experiments, we checked whether simultaneous tDCS could also modulate the acquisition of a well-known model of associative learning, namely the classical conditioning of eyelid responses, when stimulation of whisker pad was used as conditioned stimulus. Consistently with the polarity-specific reported effects, the acquisition of a classical eyeblink Poster Sessions Full Abstracts conditioning was potentiated or depressed by the application of anodal or cathodal tDCS respectively, when stimulation of whisker pad was used as conditioned stimulus, suggesting that tDCS modulated the sensory perception process necessary for associative learning. Finally, we also studied the putative mechanisms underlying immediate and after-effect of tDCS observed in the somatosensory cortex. Pairs of pulses applied to the thalamic VPM nucleus (mediating sensory input) during anodal and cathodal tDCS suggest that tDCS modifies thalamocortical synapses at presynaptic sites. In addition, we show that blocking activation of adenosine A1 receptors prevents the long-term depression (LTD) evoked in the somatosensory cortex after cathodal tDCS. In conclusion, results reported in this study confirm earlier studies in humans regarding the effects of tDCS on cerebral cortex, highlight the potential of this technique to modulate associative learning, and demonstrate the participation of adenosine A1 receptors in its selective actions on cortical circuits. 2-A-6 tDCS modulates adaptation strategies in a myoelectric-controlled interface task Claire Schofield1, Kianoush Nazarpour1, Andrew Jackson1 1 Newcastle University Myoelectric-controlled interfaces (MCIs) provide a unique opportunity to study mechanisms of motor learning and adaptation as they allow the manipulation of visuomotor mappings at the level of individual muscles. We investigated behavioural responses to perturbation of an MCI mapping which was redundant and therefore permitted different adaptation strategies. We also examined whether we could influence subjects' strategies using transcranial direct current stimulation (tDCS) to modulate the excitability of primary motor cortex (M1). In an MCI task, electromyogram signals from multiple muscles are assigned a direction of action (DoA) in order to control the movement of a cursor in 2D space. Subjects quickly acquire feedforward control of the cursor to reach targets that appear in a centre-out design. Subjects exploit task redundancy by distributing effort across multiple muscles, resulting in broad muscle tuning functions that are peaked at the DoA. After training subjects on a bimanual MCI mapping using four muscles in each hand, we introduced a visuomotor perturbation consisting of a 90° rotation of the DoAs for half of the muscles in each hand. From the tuning functions we examined how the preferred direction (PD) of rotated and un-rotated muscles changed in response to movement errors caused by the perturbation. A range of strategies could compensate for a local perturbation to the DoAs of only some muscles. At one extreme, global re-aiming would rotate the PD of all muscles by 45° to compensate for the average rotational error introduced by the perturbation. However, since the muscle PDs are then no longer aligned to the DoAs, greater effort is required to reach targets. The optimal solution is local re-mapping, whereby the PD of rotated muscles is shifted by 90°, while the un-rotated muscles remain unchanged. During the initial perturbation, subjects received unilateral anodal or cathodal tDCS over M1 (24 subjects, 8 each for anodal, cathodal and no stimulation). Anodal stimulation resulted in increased use of the suboptimal, global re-aiming strategy in the hand contralateral to stimulation. By contrast, cathodal stimulation caused a greater tendency towards optimal, local re-mapping and improved recovery of the original mapping when the perturbation was reversed. Previously, anodal tDCS has been found to be beneficial for motor learning. In contrast, in our experiments we found instead that cathodal tDCS enhanced the acquisition of an optimal adaptation strategy. The difference may be that adaptation to local MCI perturbations requires subjects to identify the changes at the muscle level that cause global movement errors. We suggest that increased excitability in M1 produces an incorrect solution to this 'credit assignment' problem, because suboptimal adaptation in muscles that do not contribute to errors is enhanced. As such, while tDCS may enhance adaptation in some circumstances, care must be taken in generalising these results to more complex situations where tDCS may cause the motor system to converge on suboptimal behaviours. 2-A-7 Modular learning of different kinematic perturbations: A model-based approach Laura Patanѐ1, Francesco Nori1, Bastien Berret1, Alessandra Sciutti1, Giulio Sandini1 1 Istituto Italiano di Tecnologia Reaching, apparently one of the simplest human behaviors, is actually the result of a quite complex process. In fact, when we reach for an object our central nervous system needs to transform sensory signals into the proper muscle activations to perform the task. This process is often thought to rely on internal models, i.e. neural representations of the underlying sensorimotor transformations. A question still under debate is how the nervous system builds and adapts these sensorimotor maps and, in particular here, we investigate whether or not the CNS exploits of the modular structure of the forward kinematic internal model. Assuming that modularity may represent an interesting trade-off between a-priori information and adaptability/flexibility within the central nervous system, the present study explores the presence of kinematic modularity in the context of planar reaching movements. More in detail, we tested the prediction that modularity should imply a faster learning rate for perturbations compatible with the existing modules than perturbations that are incompatible. To investigate this question we immersed 11 subjects on a virtual reality environment where arm movements were recorded by a manipulandum and remapped onto a screen. We confronted human subjects with two different kinematic perturbations of comparable difficulty: one compatible with the natural kinematic modules (or intra-modular) and one incompatible (extra-modular). We observed that human subjects adapt faster to intra-modular perturbations, thus providing evidence in favor of the adoption of a modular strategy by the central nervous system. Our modularity analysis relies on a model that allows to predict how subjects behave when they only have partial knowledge of the remapping they have been exposed to. We first validated a model (relative) that allows us to predict how a subject moves when he assumes a certain model but he is exposed to a remapping (i.e. different model). A relative model, based on the visual input (i.e. the relative positions of visual starting point and target on the screen), has been shown to be much better than an absolute (totally proprioceptive) model, confirming previous results on trajectory adaptation to a nonlinear visuomotor transformation (see J. Flanagan and A. K. Rao, 1995) for our experimental protocol. Then, exploiting this knowledge, we started to address the problem of modular learning in order to answer the following question: when we learn the extra-modular mapping, do we try to exploit our existing modules or not? This makes a clear prediction for two different learning strategies: the error-based strategy, that would just consist of attempting to reduce the error trial after trial, and the modular-learning approach, that would predict that subjects first try to perform the new task with pre-existing modules, before learning the necessary new ones. 45 Poster Sessions Full Abstracts 2-A-8 Inter-manual transfer in a neuromotor interface: An age-controlled study a desired trajectory, when interleaved with active movement trials, improve motor learning? Sabine Gretenkord1, Kianoush Nazarpour1, Andrew Jackson1, Janet Eyre1, Sara Graziadio1 Subjects were instructed to reproduce both the time-varying position and velocity of novel hand trajectories in a 2D horizontal plane, while grasping the handle of an InMotion2 robot arm. Subjects underwent 3 days of training, 90 movement trials per day. During each training session subjects were shown the target trajectory at regular intervals, interleaved every 3 trials. For one group of subjects, these interleaved demonstration trials consisted of visual training alone. That is, a cursor was presented that traveled along a visual representation of the target trajectory. A second group of subjects received visual and proprioceptive training simultaneously. This group was presented with the same visual stimulus but in addition their passive limb was moved through the target trajectory by the robot using servo control. The robot synchronized the passive motion of the subject's hand with the motion of the visual cursor. 1 Newcastle University 'Inter-manual transfer' (IMT) describes an improvement in performance in a unimanual motor task as a result of contralateral hand training. Previous imaging studies have observed increased bilateral cortical activation in the ageing population. This 'hemispheric asymmetry reduction in older adults' (HAROLD) may be advantageous when motor learning is to be transferred from one limb to another and thus be beneficial for stroke rehabilitation. We investigated 'inter-manual transfer' in an agecontrolled study with two aims: first, to clarify the existence and magnitude of IMT in older adults, and second, to explore the hypothesis that IMT may be more pronounced with greater age. We studied right-handed subjects from two age groups: young adults (10 subjects aged 20-35, mean: 28.4, range 26-32) and older adults (13 subjects aged over 55, mean: 69.9, range: 5582). Subjects made repeated movements of a myoelectric cursor towards a target, receiving scores that reflected the distance from the target during the hold period. Cursor position was determined by smoothed, rectified electromyogram (EMG) from two intrinsic hand muscles. Controlling muscles were either in the right or left hands acting orthogonally in the task space. The study was conducted in a crossover design: we assessed left hand performance before and after a period of either right hand training, or a resting period of similar length during which a video was shown. We analysed the scores and also the distance to the target at the beginning of the hold period. In the young adult group, the right hand training led to a 39% increase in left hand score, in the older adult group the percentage increase was higher (72%). Analysis of the distance parameter showed that the right hand training improved the left hand performance by 28% in the young adults group and by 35% in the older adults group. Cursor control with right hand improved with training in both groups comparably in terms of both score (44% in group 20-35 and 42% in group 55+) and distance (41% in group 20-35 and 19% in group 55+). In addition, we found a correlation between the right hand training and inter-manual transfer in the young adults group. However, this correlation was not found in the older adults group. Our results revealed that inter-manual transfer occurs in both age groups. The 55+ age group showed an even higher relative improvement than the young adults group. We showed that training of the dominant hand can improve motor function in the non-dominant hand in an age-controlled experiment. This effect might prove beneficial for stroke rehabilitation, as the training of the paretic hand is often a tiring and frustrating experience, and in the most severe cases impossible. We therefore speculate that training of the non-paretic hand could improve motor function in the paretic hand. 2-A-9 Proprioceptive training on a desired trajectory improves motor learning Jeremy Wong1, Dinant A Kistemaker1, Paul L Gribble1 1 University of Western Ontario Recent work has investigated the link between motor learning and sensory function in arm movement control. Force-field learning results in reliable changes to sensed limb position (Ostry et al. 2010). Learning to reach to visual targets quickly and accurately, in the absence of a force-field, results in improvements in proprioceptive acuity (Wong et al., 2011). These findings are consistent with the idea that reducing motor error through learning may result in more precise estimates of limb position. Here, we sought to investigate the reverse: does proprioceptive training on 46 After each active movement trial the difference between the subject's movement and the target trajectory was measured. Data suggest that passive proprioceptive training on the desired trajectory results in benefits to motor learning, both in terms of both position and timing error. Surprisingly, an additional group of subjects who were asked to actively move during demonstration trials, with robot assistance, did not show the same benefits to reduction in position error. These results support the notion that passive proprioceptive training improves motor learning. Here we also report data on motor learning retention, measured 2 months post training. References Ostry DJ, Darainy M, Mattar AA, Wong J, Gribble PL. Somatosensory plasticity and motor learning. J Neurosci. 2010 Apr 14;30(15):5384-93. Wong JD, Wilson ET, Gribble PL. Spatially Selective Enhancement of Proprioceptive Acuity Following Motor Learning. J Neurophysiol. 2011 May;105(5):2512-21. 2-A-10 Capacity of LTP-like plasticity is essential for motor learning Gabriela Cantarero1, Rebecca O'Malley1, Pablo A Celnik1 1 Johns Hopkins Medical Institution Plasticity of synaptic connections in the primary motor cortex (M1) is thought to play an essential role in learning and memory. In humans, training a motor skill results in an occlusion of (i.e. reduced capacity for) LTP-like plasticity, where the magnitude of occlusion is proportional to the retention of the motor skill. Previous work has speculated that occlusion is essential for consolidating a previously learned motor task; however, whether the capacity to undergo LTP-like changes is essential for learning a subsequent novel motor task remains unknown. We have previously shown that training 2 motor skills in an interleaved practice order (IPO) results in superior learning retention and occlusion of LTP-like capacity. In contrast, training 2 motor skills in a blocked practice order (BPO) results in impaired retention of learning and normal LTP-like aftereffects (i.e. lack of occlusion). We hypothesized that the capacity to undergo LTP-like changes following learning in a BPO would result in a renewed ability to learn and retain a novel 3rd motor skill, whereas occlusion following learning in an IPO schedule would result in a subsequent impairment to learn and retain a novel 3rd motor skill. We compared training and retention of a naïve 3rd motor task after subjects trained 2 motor skills in either a BPO or IPO Poster Sessions Full Abstracts schedules. We found that after training in a BPO subjects were capable of learning and retaining a third naïve motor skill, whereas subjects who trained first in an IPO were then impaired in their retention of the 3rd novel motor skill. These results suggest that the capacity to undergo LTP-like changes prior to training is essential for learning a new task, without it retention is impaired. hand grasping function, relies on structural and functional connectivity in both ipsilesional and contralesional parietofrontal pathways involved in visuomotor information processing. Extant integrity of this structural network may serve as a predictor of response to longitudinal therapeutic interventions geared towards training SMR in the lesioned brain. 2-A-11 Parietofrontal visuomotor pathway integrity after stroke determines volitional sensorimotor rhythm (SMR) modulation skill 2-A-12 Can older adults learn new dynamics? Ethan Buch1, Amir Modir Shanechi2, Alissa D Fourkas1, Cornelia Weber3, Niels Birbaumer3, Leonardo G Cohen1 1 National Institute of Neurological Disorders & Stroke, NIH, Washington University School of Medicine, 3University of Tübingen 2 Rehabilitation strategies that actively engage the extant motor system are more successful at alleviating motor deficits following stroke (Wang et al., 2010). Currently available interventions however, such as constraint-induced movement therapy (CIMT) or active forms of robotic-assisted upper limb therapy, exclude patients with severe motor deficits due to their minimum requirements for residual motor function. Alternative paradigms based on motor imagery, the conscious rehearsal of egocentric motor actions without overt motor output, have been proposed as a means of actively engaging the motor system in these patients (Zimmermann-Schlatter et al., 2008). Longitudinal brain-computer interface (BCI) training based on motor imagery of affected hand grasping results in improved control of ipsilesional sensorimotor rhythm (SMR) modulation after chronic stroke (Buch et al., 2008). However, the mechanisms underlying a patient's ability to use motor imagery to successfully modulate SMR are not known. Here, we investigated the impact of individual patient's lesion pathology on functional and structural network integrity related to this volitional skill. Magnetoencephalography (MEG) data was acquired during BCI training. Functional connectivity between each MEG sensor was estimated, and used to derive a functional network graph. A structural network model was also constructed, and local estimates of extralesional gray and white matter microstructure determined from T1-weighted and diffusion-weighted MRI data. We used a graph theoretical approach to assess how properties of distributed network architecture related to skill acquisition across our patient group. We found that inter-individual variability in patients' lesion characteristics resulted in differential changes to both functional and structural network characteristics that were related to SMR modulation skill. Patients displaying greater functional global costefficiency, a measure of overall information integration throughout the distributed functional network, achieved greater levels of skill (p<0.05). An analysis of lesion damage to structural network connectivity revealed that changes in nodal and edge betweenness centrality, a measure that characterizes the importance of a brain region or fiber pathway for integrating visuomotor information between frontal and parietal cortical regions and related thalamic nuclei, correlated with skill for both the ipsilesional M1 (p<0.05) and superior longitudinal fascicle (p<0.01), respectively. Finally, white matter microstructure integrity in regions of the contralesional SLF adjacent to primary sensorimotor and posterior parietal cortex, as well as co-localized regions of grey matter volume, positively correlated with SMR modulation skill (p<0.05, FDR corrected). These collective results suggest that volitional modulation of ipsilesional SMR acquired through BCI training, which can be used to control a mechanical hand orthosis supporting affected Alaa Ahmed1, Helen J Huang1 1 University of Colorado Older adults tend to move more slowly and with greater muscle coactivation (1). These age-related changes may indicate a reduced ability to learn new dynamics and/or adapt to a changing dynamic environment. The purpose of this study was to compare learning of novel dynamics in older adults with learning in younger adults. We used the well-studied motor learning task of reaching in a viscous curl force field while holding the handle of a robotic arm (2). We hypothesized that older adults would learn less well than younger adults. We tested 12 older adults (73.4 ± 5.5 yrs) and 15 younger adults (23.8 ± 4.7 yrs) as they performed goal-directed reaching movements. Seated subjects controlled a robot handle to move a cursor from a home circle to a target circle 20 cm away. The cursor, home, and target circles were displayed on a computer monitor suspended vertically in front of the subjects at eye-level. Subjects performed blocks of 200 null trials (no forces, Null 1), 250 force trials (Force 1), another 250 force trials (Force 2) and 200 null trials (Null 2). During the force trials, the robot applied a perturbing force perpendicular and proportional to the hand velocity (gain = 20 Ns/m), creating a curl force field. Also, one in five trials was a catch trial during which anticipatory force was measured (i.e. a force channel). We quantified learning using movement error and a learning index based on anticipatory force. Movement error was quantified as the maximum perpendicular deviation of the handle from a straight line path between the home and target circles. The learning index was equal to ratio of the estimated gain, b, to the actual gain of 20 Ns/m. The estimated gain, b, was obtained from a least-squares fit of the perpendicular force profile to the ideal force profile based on the hand velocity (Fx = b*Vy). When just reaching with no robot forces (Null 1), older and younger adults had similar movement errors (p = 0.313). Upon first experiencing the force field, both groups had large movement errors. Younger adults were able to rapidly reduce movement error from 8.78 ± 0.51 cm to 1.71 ± 0.09 cm by late Force 1 whereas older adults only reduced movement error from 7.98 ± 0.40 cm to 2.47 ± 0.22 cm. The movement errors of the older adults at late Force 1 were significantly greater than the younger adults (p = 0.002). As learning continued in Force 2, both groups reduced movement errors further, but older adults continued to have larger movement errors than the younger adults (older = 2.28 ± 0.23 cm, younger = 1.36 ± 0.11 cm, p < 0.001). The learning index revealed that by late Force 1, older adults had learned 51.8 ± 6.4% of the actual gain which was significantly less than the learning observed in younger adults (74.5 ± 4.2% of the gain, p = 0.005). By the end of the learning period, late Force 2, both groups had higher learning indices but older adults still learned significantly less (older = 64.0 ± 4.8 %, younger = 83.8 ± 2.3%, p < 0.001). The results demonstrate that older adults can learn novel dynamics, but learn less well compared to younger 47 Poster Sessions Full Abstracts adults. This suggests that age-related changes in movement control may be a strategy to compensate for impaired learning. Supported in part by NIH 5T32AG000279. 1. Seidler-Dobrin, He, & Stelmach, Motor Control (1998) 2. Shadmehr, Mussa-Ivaldi, J Neurosci (1994) 2-A-13 The role of the cerebellum in force-field adaptation Damien Pastor1, Adrian Haith2, Yves Rossetti1, Reza Shadmehr2, Jacinta O'Shea3 1 Lyon Neuroscience Research Center INSERM U1028, 2Johns Hopkins University School of Medicine, 3FMRIB, University of Oxford, John Radcliffe Hospital Patient lesion data have demonstrated that the cerebellum is a critical structure for the adaptation of reaching movements. Here we used cerebellar Transcranial Direct Current Stimulation (TDCS) in neurologically intact individuals and tested the effect of increasing (anodal) and decreasing (cathodal) cerebellar excitability on learning and retention of force field adaptation with the right hand. Stimulation of the right cerebellum was applied during adaptation (2mA, 25min) and altered motor learning symmetrically: anodal TDCS caused faster learning, while cathodal TDCS slowed learning. Both error reduction curves and the development of after-effects were significantly altered by TDCS. Specifically, stimulation altered the magnitude of trial-bytrial movement plan updating based on error feedback from the previous trial. Anodal TDCS caused greater proportional error updating, whereas cathodal TDCS reduced the update magnitude. After 24 hours, in the sham condition, subjects retained a motor memory of the acquired adaptation. This was more labile in the cathodal condition and significantly more stable in the anodal condition. These results suggest that the cerebellum drives motor adaptation by a process of trial-by-trial error updating and that the cerebellum is an important structure for initiating the development of long-term motor memories. 2-A-14 Neurophysiological constraints for biological plausibility of machine learning Martin Nilsson1, Fredrik Bengtsson2, Carl-Fredrik Ekerot2, Henrik Jörntell2 1 Swedish Institute of Computer Science (SICS), 2Lund University It is soon 70 years since the first paper on a mathematical model of a neuron was published (McCulloch and Pitts, 1943). This paper sparked an explosive interest in neural networks and machine learning. Although this research was originally inspired by neurophysiology, it gradually evolved into a research field in its own right, not necessarily limited by requirements of biological plausibility. Presently, the field has become highly sophisticated, and has generated techniques for machine learning and adaptive systems pervasive in engineering and industrial applications. Also neurophysiology research has made great strides forward, but along different lines. At this point, it is interesting to ask what results in machine learning can contribute to a better understanding of biology. For instance, at least in vivo, direct forms of back-propagating multi-layer learning appear to be unlikely. Further, each neuron seems to have only a single output, which implies that if operating as an adaptive filter (Widrow and Hoff, 1962), it can either output reconstructed input or error, but not both. On top of this, all neurons display more or less continuous probability distributions of interspike intervals, with a wide spectrum of variances. How are the complex adaptive behaviours observed possible under such severe conditions? Any tentative principle of adaptivity must allow neurons to bootstrap 48 automatic sensorimotor association, i.e. connect sensor input to appropriate motor output; to implement stable feedback control of limbs; and to construct synergies, the combination of elementary muscle movements into a lowerdimensional subspace of aggregated movements. We discuss these issues in the context of central nervous system communication and control of movement, with an emphasis on the spinal cord, the cerebellum, and the precerebellar structures, which have served as sources of inspiration for some of the historically most popular adaptive control paradigms (Marr, 1969; Albus, 1971). A micro-circuit perspective ("bottom-up") is used rather than a psychophysics one ("top-down"), and the preliminary conclusion is that, yes, adaptive control by the central nervous system is indeed possible, but only barely so. Nature is ingenious. This presentation describes modeling research by MN, based on neurophysiological in vivo experiments by FB, CFE, and HJ. References: McCulloch,W.S. and Pitts,W.: A logical calculus of the ideas immanent in nervous activity. Bull. Math. Biohys., vol. 5, pp. 115-133. 1943; Widrow,B., and Hoff,M.E., Jr.: Adaptive Switching Circuits. IRE WESCON Conv. Rec., Pt. 4, pp. 96-104. 1962; Marr, D.: Vision. W.H. Freeman. 1969; Albus, J.: Theory of cerebellar function. Mathematical Biosciences, Volume 10, Numbers 1/2, February 1971, pp. 25-61. 1971. 2-A-15 Dual processes in prism adaptation Evidence from computational modelling and transcranial direct current stimulation Jon Kennedy1, Damien Pastor2, Valerie Gaveau2, Matthieu Kandel2, Yves Rossetti2, Chris Miall1, Jacinta O'Shea3 1 University of Birmingham, 2University of Lyon, 3University of Oxford The shift in perceived visual locations caused by wearing wedge prism goggles induces adaptation that consists of multiple empirically dissociable components (Redding, Rossetti, & Wallace, 2005; Pisella et al., 2004). These include changes in visual and proprioceptive perception, and strategic changes in pointing behaviour relative to those percepts. Perceptual changes generalize across entire stimulus dimensions, whereas strategic changes show a typical associative generalization gradient (Bedford, 1993). Perceptual changes also generalize across time (i.e. they persist), whereas strategic changes are less durably retained (Redding & Wallace, 1993). Smith, Ghazizadeh & Shadmehr (2006) have shown that a model with two states, one which learns and forgets quickly, and one which learns and forgets slowly, can explain memory savings and other learning phenomena in a force field adaptation procedure. Such a model has also proved effective in explaining saccadic adaptation phenomena (Ethier, Zee, & Shadmehr, 2008). Can such a model successfully explain performance in a prism adaptation task? We used a 10 degree wedge prism adaptation design with blocks of open loop pointing (a standard method of measuring aftereffects of adaptation in prism procedures; Redding et al., 2005) interleaved throughout closed-loop pointing adaptation, and found that a two-state model was a significantly better fit to the data (after adjusting for model complexity, using the chi-squarebased small-sample-corrected Akaike Information Criterion, AICc; Burnham & Anderson, 2002) than a one-state model, across 25 healthy participants. To reflect what is known about the empirically dissociable components of the overall Poster Sessions Full Abstracts adaptive response in prism adaptation, we also tested on this data set a model with two qualitatively distinct learning modules, one which learnt from a perceptual error signal (the prism displacement, less the current perceptual adaptation), and one which learnt from a behavioural error signal (performance error; the visual distance of the end-point of the movement from the target). This model, with the same number of parameters as the Smith et al. (2006) two-state model, provided a significantly better fit to the data. With the same procedure, and ten new healthy participants, cathodal, anodal, or sham (repeated measures) transcranial Direct Current Stimulation (tDCS) was applied to the cerebellum prior to exposure to the wedge prisms. Marked disruption of retention was observed in the cathodal tDCS condition, compared to the anodal and sham tDCS conditions, as manifested by participants showing less adaptation to the prisminduced shift during exposure to it, with more rapid loss of aftereffects during interleaved open-loop pointing blocks, and more rapid return to pre-adaptation performance during wash-out. The dual state model's ability to account for the effects of tDCS are presented and discussed. 2-A-17 A shared mechanism underlying the use of visual and proprioceptive information in reaching and grasping 2-A-16 Changes of phase synchrony in motor learning during whole versus parts practice Karen Bourns1, Francisco Colino1, Darian Cheng1, Keith Brewster1, Brendan Cameron1, Gordon Binsted1 Pablo Burgos1 1 1 Universidad de Chile Pablo Burgos, Christian Arellano, Pedro Maldonado. Program of Physiology and Biophysics, ICBM, Faculty of Medicine, University of Chile, Santiago, Chile. (Pabloburgos@med.uchile.cl) Objectives: The proposal for a modular organization for motor learning (MOSAIC model), reported evidence in tasks of kinematics and kinetics adaptation first with respect to the ability of the nervous system to learn parts of a motor task and then integrate these parts (composition), and second the ability to use parts of a motor task after learning it in an integrated manner (decomposition). The learning of new sensorimotor internal models explain these adaptations and would be a possible explanation for a refinement in sensorimotor networks when we practice, what could be evidenced with the decline of some measure of long-distance synchronization during learning. The aim of this study is to report changes in phase locking value (PLV) in electroencephalographic signals (EEG) in both the composition condition (practiced in parts) and the condition of decomposition (total practice) during motor learning of a continuous task of visuomotor and kinetics adaptation. Methods: Using a computer and an analog gamepad, 10 people learned a continuous visuomotor task (video game), which was to advance by different routes without colliding with stationary and moving obstacles. People had to learn to move with his left hand the character without colliding with obstacles, but the gamepad axes had rotated 90 degrees clockwise (visuomotor adaptation), and also had to learn to shoot various bullets with his right hand to destroy obstacles, but the gamepad lever had an elastic resistance higher than usual (adaptation kinetics). One group (n = 5) practiced 4 days, 30 trials of 90 seconds in an integrated manner (visuomotor kinetic adaptation), and another group (n = 5) practiced in parts (15 trial of an adaptation and then 15 trials of the other). On the fifth day all were evaluated in the integrated task. We recorded the performance in the game, the movements on the joystick and continued activity of 32-channel EEG (Biosemi). Results: Learning is evident in both groups, under the conditions of decomposition and composition, with significant improvements in performance in visuomotor and kinetic adaptations. Furthermore, between day 1 and day 5 in the variables of distance traveled, number of crashes, and number of objects destroyed. The best average performance in the task of evaluation of the day 5 was for the whole group for both kinetic and kinematic adaptation. The phase synchrony decreases between day 1 and day 5 for all subjects between occipital and frontal electrodes in Beta band. No significant differences between groups were found. Conclusions: Our results are consistent with other studies is proposing to lower the EEG phase synchrony as a global mechanism and a marker of learning. The findings are also consistent with studies that suggest a modular organization of learning and motor control,MOSAIC model. The practice of a motor task generates sensorimotor memories or internal models that allow you to refine the use of largescale cortical networks. Probably the greatest activation for stages of motor consolidation in striatal and cerebellar networks, and premotor cortex, reported in other studies, could explain the decline in large-scale synchrony of EEG. University of British Columbia Okanagan Little is known about adaptation to acute onset vision loss. Based on the seminal work of Pascual-Leone (see: Merabet & Pascual-Leone, 2010) significant cortical plasticity is known to take place during prolonged visual deprivation. Here, we use a two-experiment study to investigate adaptations in processing visual and proprioceptive information in acute onset vision loss. Experiment 1: Visual input was removed for 2-hr to simulate acute onset shortterm vision loss. Subjects underwent visual deprivation on two separate occasions; adaptations in grasping behaviour were assessed in one session, and adaptations in cerebral blood flow velocity (CBFv) during the other. Behavioural: Subjects performed 80 trials of a reaching and grasping task pre- and post-deprivation (160 trials). Proprioceptive control (No-Vision) was used for the first 40 trials, followed by 40 trials using visual control (Vision). Vision was removed using PLATO goggles (Translucent Technologies, Inc.); goggles were occluded for the duration of No-Vision trials. In all trials subjects grasped a circular target in response to an auditory tone. Prior to the initiation of each trial, the subject's arm was passively moved to the target location and returned to the start position by an experimenter. Kinematic measurements (e.g. limb position, grip aperture) were obtained using 3 infrared markers (thumb, forefinger, and wrist) and an Optotrak Certus (Northern Digital, Inc.). CBFv: Using transcranial Doppler ultrasound (TCD), CBFv was measured at the posterior cerebral artery (PCA) and middle cerebral artery (MCA) pre- and post-deprivation, and at 30minute intervals during, while subjects performed a standard neurovascular-coupling task (see: Aaslid, 1982; Ainslie & Duffin, 2009). Results of Experiment 1 show significant modulation of kinematic behaviour and regional CBFv of the MCA and PCA following 2-hr of acute vision loss. Experiment 2: Visual input was removed for 8-hr to simulate acute onset long-term vision loss. Pre- and post-deprivation subjects performed the same grasping task as Experiment 1. In addition to the grasping task, subjects completed an oddball detection task pre- and post-deprivation. SSEPs & VEPs were recorded in response to tactile and visual stimuli. Both tasks involve detecting stimulus onset to either the right or left index finger. For one block, stimulus probability was 0.8 right (non-target stimuli) and 0.2 left (oddball stimuli), and opposite in the second block. During the 8-hr deprivation, subjects performed normal daily 49 Poster Sessions Full Abstracts activities with the assistance of an experimenter. Each subject also participated in a control day, where a pre- and post-test was performed around an 8-hr period of normal daily activities with full vision. Modulation of kinematic behaviour and variations in ERP amplitudes and latencies observed in Experiment 2 will be compared to the results of Experiment 1 and discussed in relation to current theories in cortical plasticity. It seems a possible shared physiological or attentional mechanism may underlie processing of visual and proprioceptive information for action. 2-A-18 Changes in muscle synergies during adaptation to novel visuomotor transformations 1 1 2 Denise Berger , Reinhard Gentner , Timothy Edmunds , Dinesh K Pai2, Andrea d'Avella1 1 IRCCS Fondazione Santa Lucia, 2University of British Columbia A long standing hypothesis is that the CNS generates motor output via flexible combinations of muscle synergies. A modular architecture would allow for efficient and fast adaptation to visuomotor transformations, i.e., changes in the mapping between motor output and visual feedback, because it reduces the number of parameters to learn. We previously reported that during isometric reaching in a virtual environment ("virtual surgery") adapting to a novel visuomotor transformation that is incompatible with the set of muscle synergies required by the task is more difficult than adapting to a compatible transformation. In contrast to previous studies of muscle synergies reporting lowdimensionality in the motor output, we provided evidence for modularity from testing the prediction that a truly modular controller cannot easily adapt to perturbations which are incompatible with the modules. Here we present new data and results on the changes in muscle synergy structure during adaptation to virtual surgeries. Subjects sat with their hand and forearm in a splint attached to a force transducer positioned on a desktop in front of them. The subjects' view of their hand was occluded by a LCD monitor displaying a virtual scene with a desktop and a spherical cursor matching the position of the hand. The task required moving the cursor to reach a target in one of eight directions in the horizontal plane. Cursor displacement was computed either from the recorded forces (force control) or the forces estimated with a linear EMG-to-force mapping of the EMGs recorded from several shoulder and elbow muscles (EMG control). Data collected from an initial block in force control were used to estimate the EMG-to-force matrix used for EMG control in the rest of the experiment and to extract time-invariant muscle synergies using a non-negative matrix factorization algorithm. EMG control allowed to perform virtual surgeries by altering the virtual forces generated by the muscles simulating complex rearrangement of the tendons. All surgeries were constructed by introducing a rotation in muscle space and did not affect the forces that could be generated by activation of individual muscles. However, only incompatible surgeries remapped forces associated with the synergies along a single direction in force space, thus affecting the force generation capability of a synergistic controller. With respect to our previous experimental protocol, we decreased the trial duration and increased the total number of trials in each surgery phase. We found that subjects with practice significantly increased the number of successful trials during the compatible surgery but not during the incompatible one. The average error in the initial movement direction decreased during both surgeries. However, the learning rate during the compatible surgery was significantly higher and led to significant less errors at the end of the perturbation. We then investigated the changes in synergy structure by computing the error in the reconstruction of the muscle patterns of each experimental block with the synergies extracted before any surgery. We found that muscle patterns could be captured by the initial synergies throughout all blocks in baseline and during the 50 compatible surgery. On the contrary, the reconstruction error decreased significantly during the incompatible perturbation, suggesting that changes of synergy structure occurred during the incompatible surgery. 2-A-19 Bihemispheric transcranial direct current stimulation enhances skill learning and transfer to the untrained hand Jörn Diedrichsen1, Sheena Waters-Metenier1, Masud Husain1, Tobias Wiestler1, Jörn Diedrichsen1 1 University College London Transcranial direct current stimulation (tDCS) to primary motor cortex (M1) has been shown to facilitate motor skill learning in the contralateral hand (Reis at al. 2009), putatively by increasing neural plasticity in the tissue underlying the anode. Particularly impressive benefits are exhibited when tDCS is implemented using a bihemispheric montage, with the cathode placed over the ipsilateral M1 (Vines et al. 2008). However, it has not been established whether (a) there is an adverse impact on learning processes in the hemisphere beneath the cathode or (b) tDCS-induced plasticity has a negative effect on the performance of other non-trained skills. To explore these two issues, we investigated the influence of bihemispheric tDCS on two motor learning tasks, including (1) the 'sequential finger task' (SFT), which involved the rapid production of sequences of five isometric finger presses and (2) the 'configural finger task' (CFT), which required simultaneous presses of certain sets of fingers while immobilising other fingers (i.e. movements analogous to those executed in generating a chord on a piano or guitar). We aimed to (a) investigate the effects of bihemispheric tDCS on the learning of these motor skill tasks, (b) determine if there is a negative effect of tDCS on the transfer of skill from the trained hand to the untrained hand, and (c) investigate whether there is a link between tDCScoupled training and attenuated performance in untrained motor skill modalities. Forty-four young, healthy, righthanded participants were pseudo-randomly assigned to one of four groups (SFT-tDCS, SFT-sham, CFT-tDCS, and CFTsham). Baseline skill in the two motor tasks was assessed in all subjects. Subsequently, they completed four days of lefthand training in either the SFT or CFT, with real or sham stimulation. During tDCS, which was executed in a doubleblind fashion, the anode was placed above the contralateral, right M1, and the cathode over the ipsilateral, left M1. In each training day, participants practiced the same four sequences or configurations for an hour, and tDCS was administered for the first 25 min. Training was followed by three testing sessions without tDCS, at 5, 12, and 33 days after completion of training. For both the SFMT and CFMT, the movement times were measured, while accuracy demands were held constant. After training, the movement times required to execute sequences and configurations were, respectively, 40% and 32% faster in individuals who received anodal tDCS relative to sham. This advantage was preserved even four weeks after the final tDCS training session. Importantly, across both the SFT and CFT, individuals who received tDCS additionally exhibited enhanced training-induced transfer of skill to the right (untrained) hand (i.e. the hand associated with the cathodemodulated ipsilateral M1). Finally, tDCS-coupled training in one task was not associated with impairment in performance in untrained motor tasks and, in some cases, actually was linked to improved motor performance. These results add to the emerging literature that supports a powerful translational capacity of tDCS for the treatment of Poster Sessions Full Abstracts motor deficits incurred as a result of stroke or traumatic injury. Given the unexpected finding of an enhanced transfer of skill to the untrained hand, it will subsequently be important to establish the neural underpinnings of such facilitation via functional and structural MRI. 2-A-20 Dominant arm is not always dominant: A hidden excellence of non-dominant arm in adapting to dynamical environments resulting from contralateral arm movement Atsushi Yokoi1, Masaya Hirashima1, Daichi Nozaki1 may be related to the functional specialization of each arm during bimanual movement (e.g., prime actor and supporting role for dominant and non-dominant arm, respectively). 2-A-21 Partial learning of object dynamics based on fingertip forces in the absence of kinematic errors Frederic Danion1, Jonathan S Diamond2, Randy J Flanagan2 1 Universite de la Mediterranee, 2Queens University 1 Graduate School of Education, The University of Tokyo It is believed that a greater control ability of dominant arm makes it play a leading role in bimanual movements like opening a bottle of wine. Here we challenge this widespread notion from the viewpoint of ability of motor adaptation. Specifically, we show that during bimanual movement the non-dominant left arm has a greater ability in adapting to dynamical environments resulting from the motion of the right arm than vice versa. In the first experiment, right-handed subjects trained to perform simultaneous bimanual reaching movement in forward direction (0 degree) under the presence of a velocity-dependent force field imposed on either left or right arm (n = 8 for each experiment). After the 80 trials of training session, the degree of adaptation was assessed using error clamp trials interleaved in every other trial. The movement directions of the untrained arm were randomly selected from 8 directions (0, 45, 90, 135, 180, 225, 270, 315 deg) to evaluate how the degree of motor adaptation changed as the movement direction of the untrained arm became distant from the training direction (0 degree). It should be noted that the reach direction for the trained arm was maintained the same direction (i.e., forward direction) throughout the experiments. Consistent with our recent study (Yokoi et al., J Neurosci 2011), regardless of the arm trained, we observed a Gaussian-like generalization pattern that was modulated with the reach direction of the untrained arm. A novel finding in this study was that the modulation amplitude of the generalization function was significantly larger when the training was performed with the left arm (Left arm: 48.4±10.3 %; Right arm: 36.0±10.2 %; p = 0.015, one-sided t-test), indicating the dominance of the right arm in terms of the greater influence on the motor learning of the opposite arm. However, the greater influence that the left arm received could be interpreted as its greater flexible ability to the dynamical environments changing with the right arm's movement. To test this prediction, the second experiment was designed so that the same forward reaches of the left/right arm were exposed to conflicting (clockwise: CW and counter-clockwise: CCW) force fields associated with different movement directions of contralateral arm [i.e., (0 deg: CW), (90 deg: CCW), (180 deg: CW), (270 deg: CCW)]. The order of the 4 movement configurations was randomized, and the error clamp trials were randomly interleaved in one of 7 trials. A state-space model (Yokoi et al., J Neurosci 2011) whose parameters were estimated using the results of the first experiment predicted the excellent adaptation ability of the left arm in this task. As predicted from this simulation, the non-dominant left arm outperformed the dominant right arm; the amount of the aftereffects averaged across 4 movement directions of the untrained arm was significantly greater for the right-handed subjects trained with the left arm (n=4) than those trained with the right arm (n=4) [F(1,6) = 6.096, p < 0.05]. These results suggest that the dominant arm is not always superior to the non-dominant arm. The non-dominant arm has the hidden superior ability specialized in adapting to mechanical interaction resulting from the contralateral arm's movement. This Humans can learn to perform accurate movements when grasping objects with a variety of novel dynamics and it is thought that this involves acquiring an internal model of the dynamics of the object. Kinematic errors (i.e., discrepancies between predicted and actual movement trajectories) are considered central for such adaptation (Smith et al. 2006; Shadmehr et al. 2010; Wolpert et al. 2011). However, when moving a grasped object with novel dynamics, participants also experience kinetic errors in the form of mismatches between actual and predicted load forces acting on the hand or fingertips. Here we investigated the contribution of such kinetic errors to the learning of object dynamics under conditions in which significant kinematic errors were removed. Participants were asked to transport an object with novel dynamics (a rotary velocity-dependent force field) between targets in a horizontal plane. They grasped the object with the tips of the index finger and thumb contacting vertically aligned horizontal surfaces located on the top and bottom, respectively. Sensors measured the forces applied by the digits. The object was attached to a lightweight robotic device that generated load forces in the horizontal plane. A second robotic device was attached to a cuff around the wrist and could generate a force channel that constrained the movement of the hand to a straight line. The experimental group of participants completed a set of movements in the force channel followed by a second set with the channel removed. The control group only completed a set of movements without the channel. We reasoned that if load forces at the fingertips are sufficient to enable learning of object dynamics, then participants in the experimental group should adapt their movements (and grip force) more quickly to the load (when the channel is removed) than naïve participants in the control group. When participants in the experimental group transported the object in the channel, they learned to modulate grip force in anticipation of load force which suggests that they acquired a component of the object's dynamics. However, when these participants subsequently transported the object with the channel removed, they exhibited substantial kinematic errors that were similar to those exhibited by the participants in the control group who did not previously experience the object in the channel. Nevertheless, participants who previously experienced the object in the channel continued to effectively modulate grip force when the channel was removed. These results indicate that in the absence of kinematic errors, participants can learn a component of the object's dynamics, used to control grip force, based on kinetic errors. However, this knowledge is apparently not accessible or usable for controlling the movement trajectory when the channel was removed. 2-A-22 Cerebellar modulation of human associative plasticity Masashi Hamada1, Nagako Murase1, Anna Sadnicka1, Joseph M Galea1, Mark J Edwards1, John C Rothwell1 1 UCL Institute of Neurology 51 Poster Sessions Full Abstracts Objective: To test whether the response to paired associative stimulation (PAS) could be influenced by modifying the excitability of cerebellum. Background; In the PAS protocol, the relative timing of cortex and peripheral stimuli is usually between 21.5 and 25 ms. Considering the onset of the N20 response, an interval of 21.5 ms implies a relatively direct sensory input to cortex, whilst the 25ms timing allows for more indirect sensory pathways to be involved. We hypothesized that concurrent cerebellar transcranial direct current stimulation (cDC, 2 mA), which can modify cerebellar excitability (Galea et al., 2009), might modulate the response to PAS, particularly at longer PAS intervals given that some somatosensory input may reach motor cortex via a cerebellar projection. Methods: Subjects were twelve healthy volunteers. We tested PAS at 25 ms (PAS25) simultaneously with anodal, cathodal, or sham cDC. We also tested PAS21.5 with anodal or sham cDC. Motor evoked potential (MEP) was measured before and after the intervention.Results; We found that plasticity induced by PAS25 was blocked by concurrent anodal or cathodal cDC. In addition, the effect was timing specific since plasticity induced by PAS21.5 was not blocked by anodal cDC. Conclusions: This study provides evidence that the cerebellum is involved in PAS-induced plasticity in a timing-specific manner. It has been generally accepted that PAS at short intervals (21.5 or N20 latency) and PAS25 share similar mechanisms in terms of induction of human associative plasticity. Instead, the present results provide evidence that PAS21.5 and PAS25 have different characteristics with important implications for research which uses PAS to investigate the pathophysiology of neurological disorders, or the effects of behavioural learning. by comparing with an unpaired t-test the mean response at sites 0cm, 1cm, 2cm and 3cm from the centre of the grid (i.e the contralateral hotspot) for ipsilateral and contralateral muscles. Results: Bilateral MEP's were seen in all 20 subjects. Of note there was no significant difference between athletes and controls for contralateral muscle representation. This is important as it establishes that peripheral factors such as muscle size do not explain observed differences. In ipsilateral muscle the mean response at 1 cm from the hotspot was 246%+/-21(mean +/-SEM) for athletes and 107+/-11 for controls (p=<0.001), at 2 cm 266 +/-17 for athletes and 108 +/-7 for controls (p=<0.001) and at 3cm 219 +/-14 for athletes and 75+/-15 for controls (p=<0.001). Discussion: We show that athletes have a cortical region surrounding the contralateral muscle representation that is able to produce ipsilateral responses. Athletes drive plasticity to increase the motor cortex map of ipsilateral muscles when bilateral axial muscle activation is important. This knowledge provides the athlete with a basis for alternative training strategies, and provides the neurologist with a therapeutic target in the intact hemisphere after brain injury. 2-A-24 Role of reuniens-medial prefrontal cortex and reuniens-hippocampal CA1 pathways in associative learning Juan Carlos López Ramos1, Lyndell Eleore1, Rafael GuerraNarbona1, José María Delgado-García1 1 2-A-23 Cortical plasticity in elite athletes: Lessons for brain injury rehabilitation 1 1 1 Damon Hoad , Lekshmi Desikan , Poppy Flanagan , Sam Shribman1, Paul Hammond1, Richard Greenwood1, John Rothwell1 1 Institute of Neurology Introduction: Muscles that habitually perform a bilateral activation receive a stronger bilateral corticospinal innervation. Athletes perfecting control of a movement that involves muscles acting bilaterally may alter the cortical representation of that muscle. With training a single cortical origin may provide a stronger bilateral control. Transcranial Magnetic Stimulation (TMS) provides a non-invasive means of mapping the cortical representation and characterizing corticospinal control of a muscle. Using TMS we demonstrate a pattern of cortical reorganization whereby ipsilateral projections from one hemisphere are strengthened in elite athletes, having implications both for training athletes and treating brain injury patients. Methods: Canoe-polo players who had accumulated over 10,000 hours of training and represented their country in world-cup competition were compared to healthy controls. EMG was recorded from abdominal External Obliques, ipsilateral and contralateral to the side of cortical stimulation. Background activation at 10% of Maximum Voluntary Contraction was produced. Neuronavigation guided the TMS coil to points on a scalp grid 1cm apart, centred on the site of optimal stimulation (Hotspot) for the contralateral muscle. Each point in the surrounding cortex was then stimulated at 120% of the active motor threshold (aMT). Analysis: EMG was rectified and Motor Evoked Potentials (MEP) were averaged for each grid point. MEPs were normalized to mean+1S.D. of background activity. Normalised responses for each grid coordinate expressed as mean %Background Activity were pooled for the two groups. The pooled maps were analysed 52 Universidad Pablo de Olavide We studied here the interactions between short- and longterm plastic changes taking place during the acquisition of a classical eyeblink conditioning and following high-frequency stimulation (HFS) of the reuniens nucleus in behaving mice. For classical eyeblink conditioning, five-month-old animals were implanted with two pairs of electrodes in the upper eyelid of the left eye, one of them aimed at the supraorbital nerve for the presentation of electrical stimuli, and the other in the ipsilateral orbicularis oculi muscle to record its EMG activity. Additional bipolar stimulating electrodes aimed at the reuniens nucleus and a monopolar one aimed at hipoacampal CA1 or at infralimbic area of the prefrontal cortex were implanted to study the involvement of that pathways in cognitive processes. Synaptic changes in strength were studied at the reuniens-medial prefrontal cortex (mPFC) and the reuniens-CA1 synapses. Input/output curves and a paired-pulse study enabled determining the functional capabilities of the two synapses and the optimal intensities to be applied at the reuniens nucleus during classical eyeblink conditioning and during long-term potentiation (LTP) evoked by HFS of the nucleus. Animals were conditioned using a trace paradigm, with a tone as conditioned stimulus (CS) and an electric shock to the trigeminal nerve as unconditioned stimulus (US). A single pulse was presented to the reuniens nucleus to evoke field EPSPs (fEPSPs) in the mPFC and the CA1 area during the CS-US interval. No significant changes in synaptic strength were observed at the reuniens-mPFC and reuniens-CA1 synapses during the acquisition of the eyelid conditioned responses (CRs). However, two successive HFS sessions carried out during the first two conditioning days significantly decreased the percentage of CRs, but without evoking any concomitant LTP at the indicated synaptic sites. HFS of the reuniens nucleus also prevented the proper acquisition of an object discrimination task. A subsequent study revealed that HFS of the reuniens Poster Sessions Full Abstracts nucleus evoked a significant decrease of paired-pulse facilitation lasting for up to three days. Thus, reuniens nucleus projections to prefrontal cortex and hippocampal circuits could participate in the acquisition of associative learning tasks by the modulation of selective presynaptic mechanisms. William Lytton1,3, Samuel A. Neymotin1, George L. Chadderdon1, Cliff C. Kerr1,2 Joseph T. Francis1 2-A-25 Analysis in individual differences in learning curves reveals greater variability for short time-scale motor adaptation Few attempts have been made to model learning of sensory-motor control using spiking neural units. We trained a 2-degree-of-freedom virtual arm to reach for a target using a spiking-neuron model of motor cortex that maps proprioceptive representations of limb position to motor commands and undergoes learning based on reinforcement mechanisms suggested by the dopaminergic reward system. A 2-layer model of layer 5 motor cortex (M1) passed motor commands to the virtual arm and received proprioceptive position information from it. The reinforcement algorithm trained synapses of M1 using reward (punishment) signals based on visual perception of decreasing (increasing) distance of the virtual hand from the target. Output M1 units were partially driven by noise, creating stochastic movements that were shaped to achieve desired outcomes. Yohsuke Miyamoto1, Maurice A Smith1 1 Harvard University Individual differences in motor learning are widely acknowledged but not well understood. Some of these differences can be attributed to variations in attentional state, genetic predisposition, or white matter anatomy. However, previous work on individual differences has generally focused on overall learning ability, leaving unexplored individual differences in the progression of learning during the course of training. Here we examine the temporal structure of inter-individual differences in the shape of learning curves in a motor adaptation task. This may give insight into understanding the mechanisms that underlie individual differences in motor learning ability. We examined learning curves from 194 subjects during exposure to a novel force field environment. Subjects performed rapid 10cm point-to-point reaching movements, and learned to produce forces that counteracted a velocity-dependent curl force field with an amplitude of 15N/(m/s). In order to identify the structure of the variability in these learning curves, we performed principal components analysis on the force field adaptation data. This datadriven analysis examines the covariance of learning levels, with each trial treated as a separate dimension, and each subject providing separate observation of each dimension. This analysis identifies the set of orthogonal axes (the principal components) which most efficiently explain the variance of the data. The first principal component (PC1) represents the shape along which individual learning curves differ most. Surprisingly, we found that this single principal component, PC1, by itself accounted for greater than 37% of the total variability in individual learning curves - more than triple the amount accounted for by the next largest principal component (PC2). This indicates the presence of a single dominant temporal structure that characterizes a large fraction of the individual differences in motor adaptation that we observed. We also found this principal component to have an interesting shape. If the main source of variability in learning curves were signal-dependent noise the variability would echo the shape of the learning curve itself. Instead, however, we found that the shape of the first principal component rapidly increases to a maximum within the first few trials, and then gradually decreases a small amount over the remainder of the training period. Strikingly, the shape of this principal component resembles the fast learning process in the two-state model described by Smith et al 2006. This study showed that two adaptive processes with different time scales underlie motor adaptation. The slow process responds weakly to error but retains information well, whereas the fast process responds strongly but has poor retention. A regression analysis reveals the shape of PC1 is remarkably well explained by a linear combination of the hypothesized shapes of the fast and slow processes (R^2=0.995). Notably, the we find that the contribution of the fast process is significantly greater than that of the slow process, (p<0.01) suggesting that individuals differ mostly in terms of their capacity for short time-scale learning and are more stereotyped in their capacity for long time-scale learning. 2-A-26 Reinforcement learning of 2-joint virtual arm reaching in motor cortex 1 SUNY Downstate Medical Center, 2School of Physics, University of Sydney, 3Kings County Hospital The virtual arm consisted of a shoulder joint, upper arm, elbow joint, and forearm. The upper- and forearm were each controlled by a pair of flexor/extensor muscles. These muscles received rotational commands from 192 output cells of the M1 model, while the M1 model received input from muscle-specific groups of sensory cells, each of which were tuned to fire over a range of muscle lengths. The M1 model had 384 excitatory and 192 inhibitory event-based integrate-and-fire neurons, with AMPA/NMDA and GABA synapses. Excitatory and inhibitory units were interconnected probabilistically. Plasticity was enabled in the feedforward connections between input and output excitatory units. Poisson noise was added to the output units for driving stochastic movements. The reinforcement learning (RL) algorithm used eligibility traces for synaptic credit/blame assignment, and a global signal (+1=reward, 1=punishment) corresponding to dopaminergic bursting/dipping. Eligibility traces were spike-timingdependent, with pre-before-post spiking required. Reward (punishment) was delivered when the distance between the hand and target decreased (increased). RL learning occurred over 100 training sessions with the arm starting at 15 different initial positions. Each subsession consisted of 15 s of RL training from a specific starting position. After training, the network was tested for its ability to reach the arm to target from each starting position, over the course of a 15 s trial. Compared to the naive network, the network post-training was able to reach the target from all starting positions. This was most clearly pronounced when the arm started at a large distance from the target. After reaching the target, the hand tended to oscillate around the target. Learning was most effective when recurrent connectivity in the output units was turned off or at low levels. Best overall performance was achieved with no recurrent connectivity and moderate maximal weights. Although learning typically increased average synaptic weight gains in the input-to-output M1 connections, there were frequent reductions in weights as well. Our model predicts that optimal motor performance is sensitive to perturbations in both strength and density of recurrent connectivity within motor cortex and that therefore the wiring of recurrent connectivity during development might be carefully regulated. 53 Poster Sessions Full Abstracts Acknowledgments: research supported by DARPA grant N6600110-C-2008 2-A-27 The influence of prior experience and symbolic cueing on human path integration Stefan Glasauer1,2,3, Paul Maier1,2, Frederike H. Petzschner1,3 1 Institute for Clinical Neurosciences, 2Bernstein Center for Computational Neuroscience, 3Integrated Research and Treatment Center for Vertigo, Ludwig-Maximilian University Munich, Germany Actions based on current sensory input are frequently shaped by prior experience. As we have shown, human visual path integration behaviour in a simple homing path integration task can be described as the optimized result of an optimal probabilistic combination of the current sensory input and short-term prior experience gathered over the preceding trials (Petzschner and Glasauer, J Neurosci 31, 2011). The observed trial-to-trial modifications in performance are explained by a rapidly adapting experience-dependent prior that does not depend on error feedback. Here, we investigate whether additional information given by symbolic verbal cues can influence the path integration behaviour and how the observed results can be explained by probabilistic modelling. Subjects were asked to produce and reproduce travelled distances in a virtual environment in three different conditions. (1) In the ‘two-range’ condition distances were drawn from two sample ranges (first 5-13m, then 11-19 m or vice versa), which were overlapping for two distances (11, 13m). (2) In the ‘onerange’ condition, the same distances were presented but in randomized order. (3) In the ‘cued’ condition distances were presented in the exact same order as in the ‘one-range’ condition, but each trial was preceded by the verbal cueing (two cues, “The following distance will be short” or “… will be long”). The verbal cue was always valid and corresponded to the two ranges of distances of the ‘two-range’ condition. No further information on the meaning of ‘short’ and ‘long’ was provided. Subjects received no feedback on their performance in any condition. Our results show that subjects are able to utilize the additional symbolic information given in the ’cued’ condition to modify their estimate of self-displacement. In the ‘cued’ condition, even though the order of sample distances was exactly the same as in ‘one-range’, the reproduction of overlapping distances significantly depended on the cueing. In contrast, distance reproduction in the ‘one-range’ condition showed a uniform behaviour. As expected from our previous work distance reproduction for the ‘two-range’ condition depended significantly on the sample range. To explain our results, we propose a model of distance estimation by iterative Bayesian inference based on our previous work that optimally combines 1) the current noisy sensory input, 2) a prior expectation of the presented distance adaptively adjusted within each trial, and 3) the discrete symbolic cues, the calibration of which is learned over trials. We conclude that the probabilistic modelling approach to understanding ‘cognitive’ influence, such as prior experience or symbolic cues, on action production can lead to simple but powerful models, which can explain a whole range of previous psychophysical findings. Acknowledgements: supported by the BMBF (grants IFB 01EO0901 and BCCN 01GQ0440) 2-B-33 The negative BOLD homunculus: Different contributions of negative and positive BOLD to the somatotopic representation in motor homunculi in humans Noa Zeharia1, Tamar Flash2, Amir Amedi1 1 Hebrew University of Jerusalem, 2Weizmann Institute of Science One of the most important attributes encoded in motor homunculi is somatotopy - encoding movements of different effectors by different neurons and the organization of the representation in a gradual spatial pattern. However, mapping the motor somatotopic representation in a noninvasive, accurate and detailed manner and across cortical and sub-cortical homunculi in the human brain encountered some difficulties, due e.g. to the extensive overlap between the representations of different body parts. In contrast to motor somatotopic mapping, fMRI retinotopic mapping is usually achieved using a periodic design and phase-locking analytic approaches. This form of analysis, suitable for mapping gradually shifting representations, is considered the classical means for defining early visual areas. We applied this method to the motor system, using periodic and event-related experiments incorporating bilateral/axial movements of 20 organs from tongue to toes. We report detailed mototopic imaging maps in various homunculi. In addition, we further addressed the issue of somatotopic representation inspecting positive and negative BOLD signals. A crucial attribute in movement encoding is the adequate balance between suppressing unwanted muscle activations and activating relevant ones. We studied the different contributions of positive and negative BOLD to movement encoding across M1 and the SMA. In addition to the positive BOLD, significant negative BOLD was detected in M1, but not in SMA. Its spatial pattern was neither homotopic (i.e. negative BOLD in the same location as the positive BOLD, only in the ipsilateral M1), random, nor converging for various organs. Rather, it was organized somatotopically across the entire homunculus, in an inverse manner to the positive BOLD, thus creating a "negative BOLD homunculus". The neuronal source of negative BOLD is still debated; M1 provides a unique system in which different regions receive their blood supply from different arteries, ruling out "blood stealing" explanations. In addition, MVPA demonstrated that positive BOLD in M1 and SMA and negative BOLD in M1 contain somatotopic information, enabling prediction of the moving organ from within and outside its somatotopic location. We conclude that fast and accurate motor somatotopic representations can be constructed using phase-locked analysis methods, and that negative BOLD plays a role in somatotopic representation in M1, but not in the SMA, perhaps due to the different roles of these two area in movement suppression. This study was supported by the EU FP7 TANGO. 2-B-34 Eye movement characteristics during ball catching Benedetta Cesqui1, Francesco Lacquaniti1, Andrea d'Avella1 1 B - Integrative Control of Movement 54 IRCSS Santa Lucia Fundation To successfully catch a fast ball it is important to keep the eyes on it. Tracking a moving target improves the ability to predict its future positions and helps directing the hand to Poster Sessions Full Abstracts the interception point within the available time interval. How the CNS integrates visual information, an efferent copy of the oculomotor command, and prior knowledge of the target dynamics (e.g. gravitational acceleration) to control interceptive movements is an important and open question. We have recently reported large differences in catching hand kinematics among individuals with similar performance level, suggesting that the CNS might adopt different but equally successful control strategies. Here we investigated the characteristics of eye movements during catching of fast flying balls and their relationship to the observed inter-individual kinematic differences. Eight subjects with different catching success rates were instructed to catch a ball projected from a distance of 6 m by an actuated launching apparatus with different flight characteristics (four flight times, ranging from 0.55 to 0.85 s and two arrival heights). The spatial position of the ball and of several markers placed on the subject head and body were recorded with a motion capture system. The orientation of the eyes in the head was recorded using a head-mounted video-oculography system. A dedicated calibration procedure was developed to estimate gaze direction in space using head and eye tracking data. To minimize inaccuracies due to small movements of the eye tracker with respect to the head, we also corrected for rotational drifts on a trial-by-trial basis. Overall, we were able to track gaze direction with a mean angular error of less than 1°. In all subjects, we observed a similar sequence of eye movements starting with catch-up saccades followed by smooth pursuit up to ~120 ms prior to interception. The duration of the catch-up phase, the duration of the pursuit phase, the number of the saccades, and the amplitude of the initial saccade increased with flight time and arrival height. However, most eye movements parameters varied across subjects. Moreover, in each condition, individual difference in eye movement parameters were not clearly related to the individual performance levels suggesting that non-oculomotor factors might underlie the observed inter-individual kinematic differences. 2-B-35 Rapid updating of the time-course of the visuomotor reflex gain to task demands Michael Dimitriou1, David W Franklin1, Daniel M Wolpert1 1 University of Cambridge Previous studies have shown that visual information about the location of the hand is incorporated into continuous online motor control. Specifically, perturbations of the visual location of the hand during reaching movements induce appropriate rapid feedback responses. We examine the time-course of the modulation of the visuomotor feedback gain during reaching. Subjects held a robotic manipulanum and received visual feedback through a veridical display which obscured vision of the hand. To measure the feedback gain we perturbed the visual location of the hand perpendicular to the principal axis of movement and measured the induced response. We measured the gain at different locations within the movements while subjects reached to either a 'near' or a 'far' target. We show that the visuo-motor gain shows a systematic modulation over the time-course of the reach with the gain peaking around the middle of the movement and dropping rapidly as the target is approached. There were differences in gain for the same location of the hand for reaches to the near and far targets. In a second experiment, the target suddenly jumped from the 'near' location to the 'far' location and vice versa during the movement. We measured the reflex gain simultaneously with the perturbations of the target position and show that the gain was updated towards that associated with the new position of the target. We conclude that feedback responses to perturbations of the visual location of the hand are determined by the distance to the target and are flexibly adjusted to accommodate online modifications to changes in target location. 2-B-36 Nonlinear interactions between visuomotor responses to hand and target motion: Evidence against the difference vector model Sae Franklin1, Alexandra Reichenbach2, Jörn Diedrichsen2, David W Franklin1 1 University of Cambridge, 2University College London Goal-directed reaching movements are guided by visual and proprioceptive feedback from the target and from the hand. The classical view on feedback control is that the brain extracts estimates of target and hand positions from a visual scene, and then calculates a difference vector representing the distance between them. This difference vector is then used by the motor system to produce the necessary motor commands for any required online correction. However, this intuitive theory has never been supported by direct experimental evidence. An alternative view would be that these two feedback systems (responding to estimated errors or disturbances in hand and target location) are independent, only producing combined responses at the level of the muscle activity. Here we directly examine the theoretical predictions by simultaneously studying the rapid visuomotor responses to hand and target motion, elicited by unpredictable shifts in visual hand and target position. Specifically, the difference vector theory predicts similar sized responses to any combination of hand and target motion that produces identical sized difference vectors. On the other hand, independence of the two feedback mechanisms would predict that the response to combined hand and target motion would be equal to the summation of the responses to hand and target motion separately. Because the feedback responses to hand and target motion exhibit a non-linear saturation with respect to the amplitude of perturbation, these two theories make different predictions. Subjects made reaching movements to a target while grasping a robotic manipulandum. Visual feedback of both the hand position (cursor) and target position was provided in the plane of movement using a virtual reality setup. On random trials (probe trials), the cursor, the target, or the combination of cursor and target were laterally displaced visually for 250 ms before returning to the actual hand or target position. The single displacements were either 1.5 or 3 cm while the combined displacements were 3 cm. On all probe trials, the hand trajectory was constrained by a mechanical channel produced by the robot in order to measure the force resulting from the corrective response. We found that the response to the combined hand and target motion did not agree with either the difference vector theory or that of independent feedback systems but produced responses in between these two predictions. Overall, these results provide evidence against the simple difference vector model, and suggest that the two visual feedback systems of hand and target motion elicit partially independent responses that are integrated in later stages of the sensory-motor system. 2-B-37 Cortical activity differentiates automatic and controlled processes in a speeded response switching task Douglas Cheyne1, Paul Ferrari1, James A Cheyne2 1 Hospital for Sick Children, 2University of Waterloo Human action involves a combination of controlled and automatic behavior. In choice response tasks requiring fast, yet accurate responding, errors are thought to arise 55 Poster Sessions Full Abstracts because of a failure in executive control, due either to brief lapses in attention or inadequate inhibition (or modification) of automatic by controlled processes. In either case, temporal constraints in rapid serial tasks may preclude timely intervention by controlled process. Neuroimaging can help disentangle brain networks involved in these two aspects of motor control. We combined MEG recordings with spatial filtering techniques to examine cortical activity during a response switching variant of the Sustained Attention to Response Task. Subjects made a default button press to randomly presented digits 1 to 9 (duration = 250 ms, ISI = 1150 ms) with the exception of an infrequent target digit '3', which required switching the response finger or hand on 20% of the trials. Twelve right-handed subjects performed both bimanual (right to left, or left to right hand) and unimanual (right index or right middle finger) versions of the task (1500 trials per condition). Whole-head 151 channel MEG was recorded at 625 samples/s. Beamforming source reconstruction was used to examine the time course of oscillatory brain activity time-locked to movement onset and co-registered with structural MRI for group averaging. Errors were frequent (28-30%) and error RTs were significantly shorter (290-320 ms) than correct switch RTs (447480 ms) or non-target (default) RTs (337-373 ms) across all tasks (p < 0. 01 for all comparisons). We observed differences in oscillatory brain activity associated with correct switch responses compared to errors. Theta band (48 Hz) activity was observed in the right middle frontal gyrus for correct switches, beginning after cue onset and peaking before movement onset in all tasks, i.e., independent of both side of movement and direction of switching. Interestingly, this activity significantly decreased in 'fast' compared to 'slow' switches (shortest versus longest 1/3 RTs). Theta oscillations were also observed on error trials bilaterally in the medial frontal and anterior cingulate cortex, peaking 150 ms after movement onset, likely reflecting error processing. As expected, suppression of beta band (15-25 Hz) activity was observed in the motor cortex preceding all movements, but began prior to cue onset, indicating automatic motor preparation beginning immediately after the previous response. Notably, in the bimanual task this beta suppression was bilateral for correct trials, but lateralized contralateral to the default hand in error trials, suggesting a failure to prepare the (ipsilateral) switch hand in advance. These results suggest that speeded choice response tasks may involve an automatic, yet biased preparation for movement that predicts errors even prior to cue presentation. In contrast, controlled processes involved in inhibition of the prepotent default movement and/or selection of the alternate response are reflected by non-effector specific theta activity in right frontal brain regions, which does not begin until after response cue processing. These separate brain networks likely work in parallel under task demands of fast responding, and may underlie concomitant subjective experiences of unintended errors due to incorrect preparation, or reduced sense of 'effort' during fast correct responses. Supported by NSERC (RGPIN 184018-09). 2-B-38 Spatiotemporal characteristics of muscle patterns during ball catching Mattia D' Andola1, Benedetta Cesqui1, Alessandro Portone1, Francesco Lacquaniti1, Andrea d'Avella1 1 Santa Lucia Foundation Intercepting a moving target requires accurate visuomotor control of the effector. For rapidly moving targets, visual information must be combined with prior knowledge of target motion characteristics to overcome visuomotor delays and predict the right interception position and timing. What strategy the CNS adopts to perform such challenging computation is an open question. We have 56 recently shown that the right time and place of the collision is not univocally specified by the CNS for interception under loose task constraints: for a given target motion different but equally successful solutions can be adopted by different subjects. To gain insights on the control of interception and the role of individual factors, we investigated the pattern of arm muscle activity during catching of fast balls flying in three dimensional space. We recorded and analyzed arm kinematics and surface EMGs from 16 arm muscles in 6 subjects catching lightweight balls projected from a distance of 6 m by an actuated launching apparatus with three mean flight times (550, 650, 750 ms) and two mean arrival heights (below and above the shoulder height). We identified spatiotemporal characteristics of the muscle patterns of individual subjects that were invariant across ball flight conditions by decomposing the EMG waveforms, smoothed and averaged over repetitions in the same condition, into combinations of time-varying muscle synergies, coordinated recruitment of groups of muscles with specific activation profiles. In addition, the timing and amplitude coefficients of the time-varying synergies captured the changes in the patterns across conditions. We found that two synergies explained on average 73% of the data variation (range 6683%). Consistently across subjects and conditions, the timing of one synergy was aligned to the ball launch and the timing of the other synergy was aligned to the time of ball impact with the hand. The spatiotemporal organization of both synergies showed similar features in all subjects, with a strong activation of elbow and shoulder flexor muscles in the onset-related synergy and a strong activation of elbow and shoulder extensor muscles in the impact synergy. However, the relative level of activation of elbow extensors and flexors in the impact synergy varied across subjects. In particular, subjects who stopped the hand at the impact point showed a higher level of co-contraction than subjects who kept moving the hand after the impact. Moreover, there were differences in the onset of the launch synergy related to the different individual timing of the initial response. These results suggest that the control of interceptive movements involves an initial component in response to the visual stimulus and a predictive component, timed to the impact, driving the hand to the interception point. 2-B-39 Catching something we don't see. How can that be done? Gianfranco Bosco1, Delle Monache2, Francesco Lacquaniti2 1 University of Rome Tor Vergata, 2University of Rome Tor Vergata / IRCCS Santa Lucia Foundation Prediction is a fundamental aspect of the control of fast interceptive actions, since sensory-motor delays can be source of spatial/temporal errors and potential instability. Prediction of the future motion of a visual target (i.e., visual extrapolation) is even more critical when objects in the foreground occlude the target, making moment-to-moment visual feedback transiently unavailable. Manifold sources of information may contribute to this predictive process. Visual memory about the target motion before the occlusion, for example, is known to represent a major source of information but there is also evidence that implicit knowledge about invariant features of the environment, like gravity, may contribute. In order to get further insight on how different information may be integrated into a predictive representation of the target trajectory for the control of the interceptive action, we manipulated specific features of a computer-generated visual environment representing the flyball play of the baseball game. Twenty-four subjects were instructed to intercept the fly-ball trajectories by moving Poster Sessions Full Abstracts interactively a cursor on the screen with a computer mouse and by pressing the mouse button to indicate the interception time. In two experimental sessions, about 30 days apart, fly-ball trajectories were either fully visible or occluded for three possible time intervals (750, 1000 and 1250 ms) before the ball reached the interception point. Natural ball motion imposed by earth gravity could be perturbed, in separate trials, with the effects of either weightlessness (0g) or enhanced gravity (2g) at times such that, for occluded trajectories, 500 ms of perturbed motion were visible before ball disappearance. Moreover, separate groups of subjects performed in reversed order the experimental sessions with either fully visible or occluded trajectories in order to examine the possibility that previous visual experience with the non-natural trajectories could also contribute to the interception of the invisible targets. These groups of subjects showed, in fact, significantly different patterns of interceptive responses across ball laws of motion. When intercepting occluded targets, subjects without prior visual experience of the perturbed trajectories showed responses very consistent with an expectation of the effects of natural gravity. On average, these subjects estimated correctly the responses to 1g trajectories, timed earlier and underestimated the 0g trajectories while they showed late and overestimated responses to the 2g trajectories. Conversely, subjects with previous visual experience of the perturbed trajectories showed systematic anticipation and underestimation of the 0g trajectories, but rather similar interception errors for both 1g and 2g trajectories. This suggests that implicit knowledge of the perturbed motion, developed during the first session with fully visible targets, contributed significantly to the interception of occluded targets by reducing the degree to which subjects relied on presupposed effects of gravity on the ball motion. In conclusion, predictive estimates for the interception of invisible targets reflect a-priori knowledge of the visual environment based mainly on an internal model of the effects of earth gravity, although implicit representations of arbitrary features noncongruent with a natural setting, which may be developed through visual experience, might also contribute. 2-B-40 A model of visuomotor coordination and submovements in three dimensional object interception Sang Hoon Yeo1, Martin Lesmana1, Debanga R Neog1, Dinesh K Pai1 1 University of British Columbia We propose a generative model of three dimensional object interception that includes many observed features of the human sensorimotor system. The sensory part simulates a simplified model of active vision with uncertainty. We first simulate noisy visual stimuli from a thrown target based on a probabilistic model of foveal and peripheral vision. The acquired stimuli are then used to estimate the position and velocity of the target using a Bayesian framework with an internal model. For given estimates of the state and error covariance of the target, the corresponding eye movement is planned online. Our model implements both saccades and smooth pursuit, and the decision to switch between them. The motor part of the model implements a hand movement controller. The hand trajectory is synthesized by combining submovements with typical bell-shaped velocity profiles. The physical parameters for submovements are determined based on the current estimates of the target and the corresponding gaze behavior. We propose that the eye and hand movements share the same motor program, by which a submovement of the hand is triggered in synchronization with the gaze shifts following sensory events. Also, the direction and amplitude of each submovement are determined by the current gaze behavior, matched to open loop and closed loop phases of hand movement control. This framework enables us to efficiently simulate coordinated and realistic visuomotor behavior in object interception. We also describe preliminary data measuring eye and body movements during ball catching which appear to be consistent with the model. 2-B-41 Firing pattern of spinal interneurons mediating a variety of segmental reflex pathway in awake, behaving monkey : A new hypothesis GeeHee Kim1, Tomohiko Takei1, Kazuhiko Seki1 1 National institute of Neuroscience Organization of spinal reflex pathway in vertebrate is well established in the anaesthetized or decerebrated animals, but there are no direct evidence showing their function during voluntary movement. To address this issue, we identified spinal interneurons (INs) involving segmental reflex pathways and examined their activities in three monkeys performing a wrist flexion and extension task. A tungsten microelectrode was used to record the activity of INs (C4-T1). Electromyographic activities (EMGs) were recorded from wrist flexor and extensor muscles and nerve cuff electrode was implanted to the deep radial nerve (DR). The INs that responded within a segmental latency of 1ms to the DR electrical stimulation were identified as first-order INs (FO-INs) from DR afferent. Outputs of these INs to muscles were identified by measuring the postspike (Psp) effects from the result of spike-triggered averaging of EMGs. In total, 78 INs were identified as the FO-INs from DR, and 32 INs of them showed significant Psp effects to one or more muscles. Next, we analyzed activity of the INs mediating each spinal reflex pathway during the task. Significant modulation of the IN's activity was evaluated by comparing the average firing rate during movement period with that of rest period. Most of the FO-INs (29/32) exhibited significant modulation during flexion and/or extension movement. This result strongly suggests that spinal INs mediating segmental reflex contributes to control muscle activities during voluntary movement. Next, the input-output pattern of FO-INs and their activity during voluntary movement was compared in detail. Their input-output relations were categorized into 6 patterns: A) Psp facilitation (PspF) to extensor (n=18), B) Psp suppression (PspS) to extensor (n=3), C) PspF to flexor (n=3), D) PspS to flexor (n=4), E) PspF both to extensor and flexor (n=1), F) PspF to extensor & PspS to flexor (n=3). Among these FO-INs, taskrelevant firing pattern was observed in the four classes of INs (A,B,C, and D). For example, FO-INs with PspF to extensor (autogenic excitation: A) showed activity exclusively during extension trials (15/18), especially, the activity significantly increased throughout the extension torque compared to the FO-INs without PspF in any tested muscles (p<0.01). This result suggests that disynaptic, excitatory reflex pathway that mediating autogenic facilitation of extensor muscle is involved in the maintenance of static muscle force during voluntary movements. FO-INs with PspS to extensor (autogenic inhibition: B) significantly activated during flexion and showed significant suppression of firing rate during extension (2/3). This result suggests that disynaptic, inhibitory reflex pathway that mediating autogenic suppression of extensor muscle is involved in the facilitation of agonistic and suppression of antagonistic muscle force. Further, FO-INs with PspF to flexor (reciprocal excitation: C) showed significant facilitation of firing rate during both flexion and extension (2/3), and FO-INs with PspS to flexor (reciprocal inhibition: D) also bi-directionally activated (2/4). This result may suggest that disynaptic, reciprocal reflex pathway is involved in the control of joint stiffness irrespective to movement direction. In conclusion, we 57 Poster Sessions Full Abstracts propose that spinal reflex mediating proprioceptive input contribute to form a pattern of muscle activity of both agonist and antagonist in a task-relevant way. 2-B-42 Analysis of spinal interneuron circuitry in lowlevel motor control of cat forepaw Henrik Jörntell1, Fredrik Bengtsson1, Pontus Geborek1, Anton Spanne1 1 Lund University The cervical enlargement of the spinal cord represents a massive piece of neural tissue primarily devoted to the low level motor control of the muscles of the arm and hand. In primates as well as in felines, corticospinal effects mediated over the spinal premotor circuitry can have a substantial role in the motor control of armhand muscles. This circuitry has the anatomical prerequisites for managing synergy control and is directly, powerfully and dynamically updated by peripheral sensory feedback from skin and muscles afferents. It consists of different types of last order interneurons and the network they form together with each other and other indirectly connected premotor neurons. We have performed in vivo whole cell patch clamp recordings from these neurons in the cat cervical spinal cord in an attempt to systematize its neuron types with respect to synaptology of corticospinal and peripheral inputs, spike firing dynamics and anatomical location. Based on these data we design models of the synaptic connectivity and dynamics of the spinal premotor circuitry. Our aim is to characterize its principles of operation relevant for synergy control, utilization of biomechanics and object interactions. 2-B-43 Agency assignment for automatic responses in reaching movements Alexandra Reichenbach1, David W Franklin2, Jörn Diedrichsen1 1 2 University College London, University of Cambridge Both cursor and target displacements elicited rapid corrective responses. However, we found systematic differences in the responses to cursor and target displacements that could not be explained by the perceptual differences between these stimuli. In experiment 1, the corrective motor responses indicated an equal or higher sensitivity of the motor system to cursor displacements than the perceptual system. With 4 distractors, the motor responses clearly differentiated between cursor and distractor displacements, while perceptual judgements were at chance. In contrast, perceptual sensitivity to target displacements was always higher than motor sensitivity. In experiment 2, the perceptual judgements of cursor displacements improved with additional movement-related information, resulting in similar performance in perceptual and movement tasks. We found no such benefits for target displacements. Furthermore, only the motor system showed clear erroneous responses to target distractor displacements, but not to cursor distractor displacements. In sum, the tracking mechanisms for hand and target exhibit distinct characteristics when challenged with distracting visual events. This suggests that the filtering is achieved by two different mechanisms. We suggest that hand tracking is mediated by an assignment mechanisms, which binds together the motor plan and its visual consequences. This mechanism provides accurate and interference resistant motor performance, and improves perceptual judgements. In contrast, the mechanism that tracks the movement goal is independent of movement-related processes. 2-B-44 Cervical kinematics and neuromuscular activity of whiplash patients in the early stage and 6 months after trauma Pierre-Paul Vidal1, Jennyfer Lecompte1, Baptiste Sandoz2, Sophie Bancho3, Danping Wang1, Sebastien Laporte2 1 Reaching in a crowded visual scene constitutes a difficult challenge for the visuo-motor system. To achieve accurate online control, the visuo-motor system has to track visual information from two sources: the target of the reach and the position of the hands. The visuo-motor system reacts rapidly and appropriately to changes in target or hand position, demonstrating that it accomplishes both tasks efficiently. In everyday situations with a rich visual scene, information arising from distracting, irrelevant objects has to be filtered out, increasing the demands on tracking. We hypothesise that tracking of hand and target is achieved by two separate mechanisms. To investigate this, we examined how well human participants can achieve their reaching goal in a crowded visual scene despite changes in target or hand position. To control for perceptual differences between the tasks (stationary targets vs. moving cursors), we added a perceptual task with identical visual stimuli. Participants performed bimanual reaching movements to two visual targets while grasping robotic manipulanda. Two cursors rendered on a screen above the hands indicated the positions of the hands. For each hand, we presented 1-4 distracting visual objects, resembling either the targets or the visual cursors. During the reach, either one of the movement relevant objects (cursor or target) or one of the distractors was displaced laterally while the involuntary corrective response was measured. In the control task, participants indicated which of the objects had been displaced. In experiment 1, the perceptual and reaching tasks were performed separately. In experiment 2, participants did the perceptual task directly after each reaching trial, such that one could use the efferent or afferent information from the movement for the perceptual judgement. 58 CNRS, 2Arts et Metiers ParisTech, 3Institut pour la Recherche sur la Moelle épinière et l’Encéphale Whiplash injury and subsequent disorders have been extensively investigated in the literature. Even if most individuals recover within a few weeks of injury, a significant proportion (14-42%) will develop persistent ongoing pain up to two years post injury (Sterling 2004). The processes that may underlie whiplash-associated disorders (WAD), the heterogeneous of pain and also the maintenance of symptoms in those who do not recover are not well understood. Chronic WAD displayed mostly reduced ranges of motion (ROM) and larger head repositioning error (Feipel et al. 1999), as well as altered cervical motion patterns during active movement compared to asymptomatic subjects (Feipel et al. 1999, Woodhouse et al. 2010). Recent data has also demonstrated structural and neuromechanical muscle changes in patients with chronic WAD, which could be associated, in some way, with the development of chronic pain following whiplash injury. Indeed, Elliot et al. (2006) found muscle fatty infiltrates on magnetic resonance imaging in the neck extensor muscles of patients with chronic WAD. In addition, Falla et al. (2004) have shown neuromuscular alteration of the superficial neck muscles under low load and delayed onset during shoulder flexion. So, it seems that neuromuscular evaluation combined with classical mechanical tests could be relevant prognostic indicators for WAD and transition to chronicity. Therefore, this study was designed to improve kinematic, proprioceptive, postural response outcomes and associated muscle efficiency of the head neck segment in WAD at two stages of recovery compared to healthy control. Eight patients (age 20-50 years) with acute WAD were followed Poster Sessions Full Abstracts and assessed from within 8-to-15 days of injury and 6 months post injury. They were recruited via local hospital emergency departments as they all complaining about neck pain in the first 48 hours after the collision, and classified as grade I or II according to the Quebec Task Force (Sterling 2006). Eight healthy subjects, physically active, with no concern of specific neck training or history of whiplash injury or neck/back pain, were also recruited. Full body kinematics was recorded with an active 3D motion analysis system (Coda-motion). Surface EMG (Delsys) of sternocleidomastoid, paraspinal, anterior deltoid and trapezius muscles was assessed bilaterally. Cervical principal and coupled movements as well as muscle EMG patterns were measured in flexion/extension, axial rotation and side bending, as well as repositioning in neutral position after axial rotation and extension. Furthermore, frequency analyses of the head during simple trajectory tasks and jerk indexes were quantified. Results are being processed; differences are expected in particular for muscles strategies between each group. Elliott et al, Spine, 31:847-855, 2006 Falla et al, Man Ther, 9:125-33, 2004 Feipel et al, Int Orthop, 23:205-9, 1999 Sterling, Man Ther, 9:60-70, 2004 Woodhouse et al, Exp Brain Res, 201:261-70, 2010 2-B-45 Why does picture naming take longer than word reading? Contribution of motor processes Nicole Malfait1, Stéphanie Ries2, Thierry Legou1, Boris Burle1, F.Xavier Alario1 1 CNRS & Aix-Marseille Université, 2University of California at Berkeley Since the 19th Century, it has been known that response latencies are longer for naming pictures than for reading words. While several interpretations have been proposed, a common general assumption is that this difference stems from cognitive word-selection processes and not from motor articulatory processes. Here we show that, contrary to this widely held view, articulatory processes are also affected by the task performed. To demonstrate this we used a procedure that has not been used before in research on language processing: response-latency fractionating. Along with vocal onsets, we recorded the electromyographic (EMG) activity of facial muscles while participants named pictures or read words aloud. On the bases of these measures, we were able to fractionate the verbal response latencies into two types of time intervals: pre-motor times (from stimulus presentation to EMG onset), mostly reflecting cognitive processes, and motor times (from EMG onset to vocal onset), related to motor execution processes. We show that pre-motor and motor times are both longer in picture naming than in reading, although articulation is already initiated in the latter measure. Future studies based on this new approach should bring valuable clues for a better understanding of the relation between cognitive and motor processes involved in speech production. 2-B-46 Training at different movement speeds: Transfer to different speeds or directions of movement Ulrike Hammerbeck1, Nada Yousif2, Joern Diedrichsen2, John C Rothwell1 1 Institute of Neurology, UCL, 2Institute of Cognitive Neuroscience, UCL Introduction: Rehabilitation after stroke focusses on relearning movement skills and its transfer to related tasks. In a clinical setting this usually involves intensive, task-specific practice, which is aimed at improving the accuracy of movement. Speed of movement is rarely emphasised in rehabilitation even though there is a well-recognised trade-off between movement accuracy and speed. In this study we investigated in healthy adults the effect of training at either fast or slow movement speed, on i) endpoint accuracy at the trained speed, ii) transfer characteristics to different movement speeds and iii) transfer in space to an untrained direction of movement. Methods: Eighteen healthy adults (mean age: 30.4 /-8.30 SD years, 7 males) attended for 5 consecutive days performing horizontal arm reaching movements in a robotic manipulandum. The starting point of the reach was ~15cm anterior to the sternum in the midline; there were 2 possible targets 25cm from the start. One target was immediately straight ahead (0 deg) the other was at 45 deg clockwise. Points from 1-5 were given for landing within circles of decreasing (5 to 1 cm) diameter centred on the target. Visual feedback of the cursor location was removed immediately following the onset of movement and was restored at termination. On day 1 and day 5 we plotted the speed-accuracy trade-off (SAT) by establishing endpoint accuracy for 4 enforced movement times (MT: 300, 500, 700, 900ms) at 0 and 45 degrees. On days 2, 3 and 4, participants were randomly assigned to either the fast (300ms) or slow (900ms) training group during which they performed 630 reaches per day attempting to improve their endpoint accuracy to the 0 deg target. Results: After training both groups showed improvements in accuracy for most speed conditions. The 300ms group showed the largest gains in endpoint accuracy for the speed that they trained on. When the error was partitioned into perpendicular (directional) error and parallel (distance) error both groups improved in their perpendicular accuracy mostly at the trained MT and this transferred to all MTs. The parallel accuracy also demonstrated training specific improvements but the fast group additionally showed a significantly greater improvement at 300ms (p=0.005 and post-hoc t-test p=0.034) which accounted for the effects on overall accuracy. Further analyses suggested that much of this improvement in parallel error in the fast training group was due to reduced trial-to-trial variability in the maximal movement speed. The accuracy gains, due to this reduction in speed variability, generalized well to the 45 deg target, while the decrease in the perpendicular component of the endpoint error was more specific to the trained training direction. Conclusion: Training at different movement speeds leads to differential improvement in different subcomponents of the task, which only partially generalizes to new movement speeds. Training at fast training shows better transfer characteristics than slow training, to new movement direction. 2-B-47 Role of the rostral medial prefrontal cortex during the associative learning in behaving rabbits Rocio Leal Campanario1, José María Delgado García1, Agnès Gruart1 1 Pablo de Olavide University We have studied the role of the rostral medial prefrontal cortex (mPFC) in the classical conditioning of eyelid responses in alert behaving rabbits. The rostral mPFC was identified by its afferent projections from the medial half of the thalamic medio-dorsal nuclear complex, and by the firing rate synchronization of mPFC neurons evoked by the stimulation of this thalamic nucleus. Classical conditioning consisted of a delay paradigm using a 370-ms tone as the conditioned stimulus (CS) and a 100-ms air puff directed to the left cornea as the unconditioned stimulus (US). During classical eyeblink conditioning sessions, the firing rate of recorded single unit activity of mPFC neurons increases during the CS-US intervals in simultaneity with the presence 59 Poster Sessions Full Abstracts of CRs. Electrical train stimulation of the contralateral rostral mPFC produced a significant inhibition of air puff-evoked blinks. The same train stimulation of the rostral mPFC presented at the CS-US interval for 10 successive conditioning sessions significantly reduced the generation of conditioned responses (CRs) compared with values reached by control animals. Interestingly, the percentage of CRs reached almost control values when train stimulation of the rostral mPFC was removed from the 5th conditioning session on. The electrical stimulation of the rostral mPFC in well-conditioned animals decreased the percentage of CRs. The stimulation of the rostral mPFC also modified the kinematics (latency, amplitude, and velocity) of evoked CRs. In contrast, lidocaine injections in the mPFC during the 2nd conditioning session increased the expected rate of CRs, and modify the kinematics of both reflex and CRs. Thus, local administration of lidocaine produced opposite effects to that evoked by mPFC stimulation. In conclussion, the rostral mPFC seems to be a potent inhibitor of reflex and learned motor responses. In addition, these results seem to point out the need of the right activation of the mPFC to execute the acquired motor response in an appropriate and timed way. 2-B-48 Anosmin-1-over-expression on in vivo hippocampal long-term potentiation and postnatal neurogenesis V. Murcia-Belmonte1, D. García-González1, P.F. Esteban1, A. Gruart2, J.M. Delgado-García2, F. de Castro1 1 Grupo de Neurobiología del Desarrollo-GNDe, Hospital Nacional de Parapléjicos, 2Division of Neurosciences, Pablo de Olavide University Anosmin-1 is an extracellular matrix glycoprotein encoded by the KAL1 gene, which is responsible for the X-linked form of Kallmann syndrome in humans. During CNS development, anosmin-1 participates in the migration of immortalised GnRH neurons, neuronal precursors, as well as of oligodendrocyte precursors. It also has a role in axon guidance, neurite outgrowth and the formation of axon collaterals from neurons of the olfactory system and proyection neurons of the cerebellum. Although to date, the mechanism of action of anosmin-1 it is not completely understood, it has been proposed that anosmin-1 could regulate the signalling of FGF2/FGFR1 and other receptors and ECM molecules. We have generated a transgenic mouse line that over-expresses anosmin-1 in which the histological examination of adult did not reveal seeming neuroanatomical alterations compared to control mice. So far, we have studied the effect of anosmin-1 overexpression in adult neurogenesis and the distribution of several neuronal markers in the hippocampus: although no significant differences in BrdU incorporation were observed, homozigous anosmin-1-over-expressing mice showed a significant decrease in the number of doublecortin+-neurons in the hippocampal dentate gyrus, and significantly more parvabalmin+-neurons in the CA3 layer. We monitored the physiological properties of the CA3-CA1 synapse in adult mice in vivo under specific experimental conditions, which included input/output curves, paired-pulse facilitation, and electrically-evoked LTP. Field excitatory postsynaptic potential (fEPSPs) evoked at the CA1 area by stimulation of Schaffer collaterals in anosmin-1 transgenic mice for input/output curves and the paired-pulse test presented values similar to those reached by controls. Induction of long-term potentiation (LTP) in alert-behaving mice showed that anosmin-1 over-expression prevented evoking an LTP response. Altogether, our data suggest that anosmin-1 is involved in synaptic plasticity in the adult mice. 60 This research was supported by grants from the Spanish Ministerio de Economía y Competitividad-MINECO (SAF2009-07842), Gobierno de Castilla-La Mancha (PI2009/29; MOV-2007-JI/19 and MOV-2011-JI/11) Spain, and ‘Fundación Eugenio Rodríguez Pascual’ to FdC and by MINECO (BFU2008-00899, BFU2008-03390) and Junta de Andalucía (BIO-122, CVI-02487 and P07-CVI-02686 ) grants to JMDG and AGM. VMB and DGG are hired currently under SAF2009-07842, and FdCS and PFE by SESCAM. D - Disorders of Motor Control 2-D-81 Clinical assessment tool development: Measuring cognitive-motor integration in healthy aging and early Alzheimer's disease Kara Hawkins1, Jeya Thayaparan2, Adriana Bida3, Lauren E Sergio1 1 York University, 2York Central Hospital, 3Southlake Regional Health Centre The central hypothesis guiding our research is that different types of visuomotor compatibility are processed in separate, but overlapping, parietofrontal networks, and that these separate networks are affected differently in healthy aging versus disease. Generally when reaching for an object in the environment the visual stimulus and its required motor action are in alignment. However, the evolution of the capacity for tool-use in primates has resulted in situations where the correspondence between vision and action is not direct. A common example is the use of a computer mouse to move a cursor on a monitor, which involves a decoupling, or dissociation, between the spatial location of the visual target and the spatial location of the movement goal. Such learned sensory- and cognitive-motor associations underlie much of our everyday activities (including driving), yet the basic cortical mechanisms responsible for these behaviours remain unknown. Following damage to the cerebral cortex (e.g. neurodegeneration, stroke, TBI), these complex visuomotor transformations may become impaired, and the pattern of impairment may provide insight into the underlying neural mechanisms. While the ability to directly interact with objects does not appear to be impaired in early Alzheimer's disease (AD) relative to healthy aging, performance decrements are observed when early AD patients perform motor control tasks under conditions in which direct visual feedback is not provided¹. We propose that measuring visuomotor integration under conditions that place demands on visualspatial and cognitive-motor processing may provide an effective behavioural means for the early detection of underlying Alzheimer's-type neuropathology. Currently, we are evaluating the validity, practicality, and, over time, predictability of using such a measure in clinical populations. To this end, patients clinically suspected of amnestic mild cognitive impairment (aMCI) or early AD are tested on six visuomotor transformation tasks: vertical (direct visual feedback), vertical memory, vertical rotated cursor feedback, horizontal, horizontal memory, and horizontal rotated cursor feedback, using a dual touch-screen tablet. Preliminary results have revealed that aMCI patients and, unexpectedly, participants with a maternal family history of AD exhibit fragmented velocity profiles and trajectory deviations during the horizontal tasks, resulting in larger endpoint errors, path lengths, and total movement times Poster Sessions Full Abstracts when compared with healthy controls. aMCI patients also produced more 'direction reversal' and 'leave too early' errors in conditions with rotated cursor feedback and memory delays, respectively. These impairments may reflect early neuropathology disrupting the intricate reciprocal communication between parietal and frontal brain areas required to successfully prepare, execute, and update complex reaching behaviours. Recent studies using diffusion tensor imaging (DTI) in MCI and AD have documented disruption to the white matter tracts forming these parietal-frontal connections², as well as projections from hippocampal regions to parietal association areas³. Future DTI work in our lab will investigate the neural correlates of observed decrements in performance on our cognitive-motor integration tasks in preclinical AD populations. 1. Ghilardi et al. (2000) Brain Res 876: 112-123, 2. Bosch et al. (2012) Neurobiol 3. Villain et al. (2008) J Neurosci 28: 6174-6181 2-D-82 Progressive resistance exercise improves bradykinesia and muscle activation patterns in Parkinson's disease Fabian David1, Julie A Robichaud1, Sue E Leurgans2, David E Vaillancourt3, Cynthia Poon1, Wendy M Kohrt4, Cynthia L Comella2, Daniel M Corcos1 1 Universtiy of Illinois at Chicago, 2Rush University Medical Center, University of Florida, 4University of Colorado School of Medicine 3 Reduced movement speed and impaired EMG activation patterns are consistent findings in patients with Parkinson's disease (PD). Previously, we demonstrated that antiparkinsonian medication and deep brain stimulation improve elbow movement speed and EMG activation patterns. In this poster we report on the efficacy of progressive resistance exercise (PRE) to improve elbow movement speed and EMG activation patterns in PD. We tested the hypothesis that engaging in 24 months of PRE in PD will provide significant improvement in elbow movement speed and muscle activation patterns when compared to Fitness Counts (FC), a non-progressive exercise program recommended by the National Parkinson's Disease Foundation. The basis for this hypothesis is that a progressive/dynamic exercise intervention would be better than a non-progressive/static exercise intervention. We conducted a randomized controlled trial comparing the effect of PRE with FC on elbow movement speed and muscle activation patterns. Patients with PD were matched on sex and off-medication UPDRS-III score and randomized to the PRE or FC group. Patients were assessed by raters who were blinded to group assignment. Both exercise interventions were supervised and patients exercised between 60 to 90 minutes, 2 days/week for 24 months. The 24 patients in the PRE group performed a progressive weight lifting program. The 24 patients in the FC group participated in a non-progressive flexibility, balance and strengthening program. At baseline, 6, 12, 18, and 24 months of the intervention, elbow kinematics and EMG activity were recorded while non-medicated patients performed point-to-point movements at maximal speed on a light-weight instrumented manipulandum. Fifty-eight percent of the subjects were male, average age was 58.9 years, and average disease duration was 6.5 years. Forty-eight patients completed the 6-month evaluation and 38 patients completed the 24-month evaluations. The group by time interaction was significant for the following variables: Elbow peak velocity (p = 0.02), magnitude of the first agonist EMG normalized to burst duration (p = 0.013), and number of agonist bursts prior to peak velocity (p = 0.003). Post-hoc analysis at the study end-point of 24 months revealed the following: First, the PRE group increased peak velocity by 30o/s more than the FC group (p < 0.0001); second, the PRE group increased the magnitude of the first agonist EMG normalized to burst duration more than the FC group (p = 0.009); and third, the PRE group decreased the number of agonist bursts prior to peak velocity more than the FC group (p = 0.001). In conclusion, PRE brought about significant gains in upper limb movement velocity and therefore had a significant benefit on bradykinesia. In addition, PRE significantly increased the magnitude of the first agonist EMG normalized to burst duration and reduced the number of agonist bursts. These findings indicate that PRE may modify the cortical output to skeletal muscle in PD. 2-D-83 Nonlinear summation of evoked forces and EMG using intraspinal microstimulation trains in the macaque monkey Jonas Zimmermann1, Andrew Jackson1 1 Newcastle University Cervical intraspinal microstimulation (cISMS) is a potential approach for restoring upper-limb function in patients paralysed by stroke or spinal cord injury. In a recent study, we controlled functional arm and hand movements by delivering cISMS trains to individual electrodes in nonhuman primates. Here we investigate the effect of delivering stimulus trains at two sites simultaneously. We were interested in the extent to which spatio-temporal interactions mediated by intraspinal circuits would produce a motor output that differed from the linear combination of individual cISMS responses. Four female macaque monkeys were terminally anesthetized. Following a cervical laminectomy, we inserted tungsten microwires (50 μm diameter) and floating microelectrode arrays (MicroProbes) to depths of 3-5 mm into the cervical enlargement. Pairs of electrodes were stimulated with interleaved trains (0.5 s long, 50 Hz per channel) delivered at different relative phase. Stimulation currents were set to evoke a visible motor response and were typically in the range of 20-200 μA. We recorded electromyogram (EMG) responses from 12-16 muscles of the arm and hand, and isometric force trajectories with a 6axis force/torque transducer attached to the hand. We collected data from 68 pairs of electrodes. We compiled average force trajectories over the last 250 ms of the stimulation period. EMG recordings were rectified and integrated over the whole stimulation period after baseline removal. We compared forces and EMG generated by interleaved trains against a prediction generated under the null hypothesis of linear summation of the responses to stimulation of individual electrodes. 27 (40%) pairs showed no supra-linear interaction in any recorded muscle. For the remaining pairs, supra-linear summation was seen in 6 muscles on average. Sub-linear summation was found less frequently: 31 (45%) pairs showed no sub-linear effects, while the remaining pairs showed sub-linear effects in 3 muscles on average. Non-linear interactions often occurred only for particular relative phase shifts between stimulus trains. The proportion of muscles showing supra-linear summation was largest for 0, 2.5, and 5 ms phase shifts (19%, 16%, and 14% respectively). Sub-linear summation was most prevalent for opposite phase shifts (-5, -10, -2.5 ms; 9%, 8%, and 8%, respectively). Finally, we investigated whether these non-linear muscle responses had an effect on the evoked force trajectories. In 15 (22%) pairs there was a significant change of direction for at least one phase shift, 14 (20%) showed changes in direction and magnitude and 6 (10%) showed changes in force magnitude only. The remaining 33 (50%) of pairs did 61 Poster Sessions Full Abstracts not show any deviation from the force trajectories predicted by linear summation. In summary, spatio-temporal interactions between the neural elements activated by cISMS produce a range of supra- and sublinear motor responses. This is inconsistent with the notion of independent spinal cord modules activated by individual electrodes. cISMS control strategies will need to reflect the distributed nature of stimulus-evoked activity, but careful timing of stimuli delivered at multiple sites may extend the repertoire of possible upper-limb movements that can be restored by spinal cord stimulation. 2-D-84 Sham-controlled study of transcranial direct current stimulation (TDCS) for dystonia in children Scott Young1, Matteo Bertucco1, Terence D Sanger1 1 University of Southern California Childhood dystonia is a movement disorder in which excess muscle activity interferes with voluntary movement. Most treatments have limited success, so there is a need to investigate alternate non-invasive approaches for the control of dystonia in children. Research has shown that dystonia is associated with increased excitability of the motor cortex. This suggests the possibility of reducing dystonic symptoms through a reduction in cortical excitability. In a previous study, our laboratory found evidence that cathodal transcranial direct current stimulation (TDCS) might reduce overflow muscle activity in a subset of participants. We have completed the present sham-controlled study to verify those earlier findings. Participants were stimulated with sham or cathodal TDCS of primary motor cortex. To determine the effects of stimulation, we compared measures of muscle control and stretch reflex prior to stimulation with the same measures following stimulation. We investigated control of intrinsic hand muscles by measuring tracking error and overflow in an EMG tracking task. We measured stretch reflex activity in response to a mechanical perturbation of the first dorsal interosseous muscle. We tested 15 children (ages 4 to 18) with various causes of dystonia. Initial results from 8 participants have shown a significant decrease in overflow activity, but no significant difference between sham and real stimulation. There was no significant effect of stimulation on tracking error. We will present further analysis of these results as well as analysis of the effects of TDCS on partipants' stretch reflex. This is one of the first studies on TDCS for children with dystonia. Further studies will focus on increasing the effect size through stimulation on multiple consecutive days. While TDCS still appears to hold promise for reduction of dystonic symptoms in some cases, the findings from this controlled study do not support a significant effect. 2-D-85 Vibro-tactile biofeedback for neuromuscular rehabilitation in children with dystonia Francesca Lunardini1, Serena Maggioni1, Claudia Casellato1, Matteo Bertucco2, Alessandra Laura Giulia Pedrocchi1, Terence D Sanger2 1 Politecnico di Milano, 2University of Southern California Dystonia is a movement disorder in which involuntary sustained or intermittent muscle contractions cause twisting and repetitive movements, abnormal postures or both. Recently, some studies support the possibility that some symptoms of dystonia may result from a lack of sufficient sensory feedback about motor actions. Biofeedback is a method that could improve patients' movement performances by providing additional sensory information about a particular physiological process. In this study dystonic children receive vibro-tactile biofeedback proportional to the activation of a target muscle, to test whether an augmented sensory feedback 62 could allow patients to be more aware of their muscular activation and thus to more properly control the muscle contraction. To investigate the effectiveness of this technique, dystonic and healthy children are asked to perform an eight-figure writing and a back and forth tracking task using a stylus pen. They are required to do the same task first without and then with biofeedback. During movement execution kinematic data are recorded through a magnetic motion-capture system by placing eight sensors on the bone landmarks of the moving arm. In addition, the electrical muscle activity is recorded from eight upper-limb muscles. The 2-D coordinates of the stylus pen tip on a touch-screen tablet are used to measure the end-effector kinematics. For the session with the biofeedback a vibrating motor is attached to a target muscle, assessed individually for each patient. This integrated set-up has been developed and tested. The data analysis aims to quantify the movement behavior, outlining any differences within-subject (with and without biofeedback) and between-groups (healthy and dystonic). Kinematics and EMGs are processed both in time and in frequency domains. Preliminary results on healthy subjects have shown repeatable frequency aspects in joint kinematics (the vertical and the horizontal frequency components are in an exact ratio of two). These rhythmic properties allow for a kinematic-EMG coupled analysis, which associates each muscle activity to one or both the kinematic components, thus defining functional motor synergies. The results of this study will be useful for testing the effects of short-term vibro-tactile biofeedback on the kinematics and muscular parameters. A promising outcome could be the starting point for a prolonged use of this technique during daily-life activities. 2-D-86 Kinematics analysis of constrained reaching movements in children with dystonia Matteo Bertucco1, An Chu1, Terence D Sanger1 1 University of Southern California Childhood dystonia is defined as a movement disorder in which involuntary sustained or intermittent muscle contractions cause twisting and repetitive movements, abnormal postures, or both, where bradykinesia and hyperkinesia jointly or separately occur. The clinical consequences of dystonia often lead to severe communication impairments to interact with the outside world, and the cognitive and social development may, therefore, be secondarily affected. Nowadays, it is a common practice that these children communicate through the use of assistance devices. These communication devices are characterized by physically constrained features such as the number, size and location of the buttons on the screen and rarely taken in consideration the perceived cost of hitting the unwanted button. Thus, as these children become dependent upon their sensory-motor abilities to interact with touch screen devices, through deficits that may hamper the skilled use of their upper limbs, their social interaction abilities may be further significantly limited. The aim of the study is to quantify the influence of bradikinesia and hyperkinesia on assisted communication devices. Healthy and dystonic children are asked to point at a touchscreen with different targets widths and spacing between the correct and erroneous target. The kinematic data are recorded by using a magnetic tracking device and eight sensors are attached at the bone landmarks of the reaching arm joints. As we expected, the preliminary data have shown significantly slower movements, higher variability and lower optimal trade-off between speed and accuracy of the pointing movement in children with dystonia. In addition we Poster Sessions Full Abstracts will discuss the results of the frequency domain and principal components analysis of the movement trajectories to distinguish the motor synergies that characterize children with dystonia. The results will be used to optimize the choices made in device features and user interface design in order to maximize the augmented communication rate with assisted devices based on specific motor impairments in childhood dystonia. 2-D-87 Development of a new brain ischemia model to induce motor deficit in the non-human primate: Anterior choroidal artery occlusion 1 Sandra Milena Puentes Martinez , Kazuhiko Seki 1 1 National Institute of Neurosciences Non-human primate models of stroke can offer to brain ischemia research several advantages in comparison with less evolved animals models because of the similar ergonomics and hand dexterity which put them closer to the human condition. In the marmoset, a model using the occlusion of the middle cerebral artery (MCAO) is available. Although this procedure can induce motor impairment, the compromised brain volume is very wide affecting additional brain functions hampering the evaluation of the pure motor damage. In contrast, lacunar infarctions affecting the internal capsule are generated often by the obstruction of a single artery: the anterior choroidal artery (AChA); this lesion induces in most cases motor dysfunctions without cognitive impairment in human patients (Likitjaroen et al. J Neurol. 2012). The purpose of our investigation is to develop a brain ischemia model for marmosets by the direct occlusion and transection of the proximal AChA, and to compare the anatomicaly compromised zones and the neurological outcome with the already developed MCAO model. Adult marmosets were distributed in 3 groups to underwent MCAO (n=2), anterior choroidal artery occlusion AChAO (n=2) or sham operation (n=2). Using a frontal approach, the first portion of the MCA or the AChA were transected. After surgery animals were hand-fed and nursed until required. From day 1 to 10 days after surgery, animals were assessed by the Overt abnormal signs scale which is used to assess the neurological outcome of marmosets. We evidenced that the recovery of animals after AChAO was faster than the MCAO; moreover the neurological score (Virley et al. JCBFM. 2004) which evaluate the motor statement of upper and lower limbs of marmosets showed an important neurological deficit for both groups after surgery, which was recovered briefly, rising similar scores at day 10th. In order to evaluate the anatomically compromised area, MRI analysis was performed at day 4th after surgery. For MCAO group, we found a wide compromised area including the cortex, basal ganglia and part of the internal capsule. A complete transection of the left MCA was confirmed by the AngioMR. For the AChAO group, we found a lesion compromising the internal capsule and partially the basal ganglia. The AngioMR showed that the MCA and the internal carotid artery were intact. Animals were sacrificed at day 11 and histology performed. Nuclear markers (Nissl) and Myelin staining were performed in serial sets. For MCAO a wide area compromising the cortex, basal ganglia and internal capsule was found, with diffuse infiltrating cells on the affected parenchyma. Myelin staining also showed degeneration of the internal capsule. The AChAO showed a focal injury corresponding with the MRI findings, sectioning completely the internal capsule, with dense cell infiltration and compromising briefly the basal ganglia. In conclusion, we succeed to develop an effective technique to induce a lacunar infarction in the marmoset internal capsule. Comparing both procedures, the AChAO becomes a more accurate technique to induce motor deficit since it transected the internal capsule without damage broadly the surrounding structures. Detailed behavioral assessment is required to evaluate the differences in the motor function and cognition between the two techniques. This technique also will be used in the macaque monkey to evaluate in detail the hand dexterity impairment after internal capsule infarction. 2-D-88 Spasticity emerges when abnormal firing thresholds are introduced into the spine neuronal network emulated on hardware C. Minos Niu1, Sirish K Nandyala1, Won Joon Sohn1, Terence D Sanger1 1 University of Southern California Our overarching goal is to reveal the organizing mechanisms underlying movement disorders believed to cause hypertonia in childhood. In this study we focus on the disorder of spasticity, defined in clinic as increases in joint resistance to externally imposed movement with a velocitydependent threshold. This characteristic suggests that spasticity is caused by a low firing threshold of the motoneurons that are synaptically connected to the dynamic spindle fibers (primary afferent, group Ia). As such, it makes these motoneurons more likely to fire and therefore provide more resistance due to imposed joint velocity. In order to emulate this possibility, we previously developed a scalable, general-purpose neural emulation platform by leveraging the recently available Field Programmable Gate Array (FPGA) technology. The platform allowed us to emulate -- in 365x real-time -- a motor nervous system consisting of 1,024 motoneurons, 1,024 sensory afferent neurons (both using Izhikevich models), proprioceptors (Loeb's model) and Hilltype muscles. Here in this study we indiscriminately reduced the firing threshold of the motoneuron pool innervated by Ia afferent spindle fibers. We will demonstrate that a symptom compatible with the above definition of spasticity will naturally and robustly emerge. Our next goal is to investigate whether the severity of spasticity in individual patients can be differentiated solely based on a threshold mechanism. This will allow us to investigate changes in spasticity over time during development in children. Our results are consistent with current models of the development of spasticity and show that these models are sufficient to yield symptoms consistent with clinical findings. 2-D-89 Kinematic investigations of reaching slowness in hemiparetic stroke patients Agnes Roby-Brami1, Laurence Mandon2, Johanna V Robertson2, Raphael Zory2, Djamel Bensmail2 1 University Pierre et Marie Curie, CNRS, 2Hôpital Raymond Poincaré It is well-known that reaching movements made by hemiparetic stroke patients are slow and irregular. However, the relationship between the impairments following stroke (weakness, spasticity, disruption of coordination) and slowness of movement remains poorly understood. To investigate the factors limiting velocity in hemiparetic patients, we recorded aiming movements directed to two targets (at respectively 60% and 90% of anatomical arm length) in 6 hemiparetic patients and examined the ability of the patients to increase aiming velocity under the instruction to move as fast as possible. Two experimental conditions were compared: unconstrained condition and trunk blocked condition. The reaching movements were recorded (MotionAnalysis) and the following kinematic variables measured: amplitude and smoothness of the hand velocity profile, amplitude and velocity of elbow extension and amplitude of acromion displacement. Force production was recorded using an isostatic dynamometer (ConTrex). The isokinetic dynamometer was also used to impose passive 63 Poster Sessions Full Abstracts elbow extensions at a controlled speed thanks (6°/s and 30180°/s by 30°/s steps) and an index of spasticity was computed as the slope of the maximum torque versus angle plot. The results confirmed the reduced aiming velocity of hemiparetic patients when they point at their preferred velocity. In addition, we demonstrated that they were able to increase aiming velocity when instructed to do so. The effect of the instruction was independently added to the effect of target distance. Elbow extension velocity was linearly related to aiming velocity. The amount of elbow extension increased with target distance but decreased when the instruction was to move faster. The displacement of the acromion increased with target distance, but only moderately with the instruction to move faster. The trunk restraint limited acromion movement as expected and increased elbow extension, in some patients trunk restraint induced a limitation of the amplitude of the hand movement. The slope of the relationship between hand velocity and elbow rotation velocity was increased in the trunk blocked condition. Preliminary correlation analysis showed that the maximum hand velocity obtained for each patient (in the condition "fast condition/far target/trunk blocked") was inversely correlated with the spasticity index. The amount of acromion displacement in the trunk free condition was inversely correlated with maximum flexion strength. Maximum elbow extension velocity was correlated with maximum concentric extension strength. In conclusion, these results show that the patients are able to voluntarily increase movement velocity, at the risk of reducing elbow extension. This underlines the importance of limiting compensations through trunk restraint in these conditions. It is not possible in this preliminary study to determine if slowness is related to flexor spasticity and/or to weakness. The correlations should be taken with care because of the limited number of patients included. Further studies are planned in a larger number of patients and following Botulinum Toxin therapy. 2-D-90 Quantitative assessment of driving performance in Parkinsons disease (PD): Deficient coordination of gaze-hand-feet-control with a deactivated sub-thalamic nucleus stimulator Wolfgang H. Zangemeister1, Lea Maintz1, Carsten Buhmann1 accident- errors. According to the severity code of the errors, they were graded from 1=small to 4=critical. The error amount was generated from the quantity and the quality of the errors. Compared to approximately normal driving (Average error (AE): 10.4 -7), PD Patients with STN On drove worse (AE: 20.9 -6); however, PD Patients with STN Off, medication ON demonstrated a lot more traffic errors (AE: 24,6 -5); PD Patients in a total Off-condition (STN Off, med. OFF) showed the highest number of traffic errors (AE: 27,1 -8). Comparing the driving time, PD Patients with STN stimulator ON needed on average 258 sec to accomplish the specified driving route. This can be considered the upper limit of the normal value (Average: 231 -30). PD Patients in a total Offcondition needed a mean time of 278 sec to accomplish the route.- Integration of steering movements over total time of driving showed a three times reduced power in the PD´s with STN OFF [off=2.56, on=7.56]. A similar effect was obtained in the Lomb periodograms, and also in Wavelet analyses of steering movements. They demonstrated that driving frequency power was similar for PD ON and normal subjects with frequency peaks around 0.03, 0.05 and 0.09 . For PD with STN OFF in comparison, frequency power was highly significantly lowered with frequency peaks around 0.0025, 0.01 and 0.025.- The basic frequencies of the parametric [three sine wave] reconstruction of PD´s in STN OFF condition were around 0.2 , 0.4 and 0.9 Hz, whereas normal subjects and PD´s in STN ON condition showed 0.3, 0.7 and 1.4 Hz respectively. Furthermore, Gaze and steering movements were closely correlated. From a clinical point of view these data demonstrate the loss of fine control of gaze-arm-foot coordination in the STN OFF condition as well as the special role of the narrowing of "attention" as a single, most important part of the disease progress. 2-D-91 Decreased saccade velocity in spinocerebellar ataxia 6 1 John Anderson1, Peka C Savayan2 Driving a car is an essential everyday coordination task that may be severely limited in idiopathic parkinson´s disease (PD) with narrowed attention, prolonged sensory-motor latencies, tremor, rigidity, slowness and hypometria of the patients´ movements. During smooth pursuit vision the target is fixed on to the fovea as the eyes track the slowly moving target up to velocities of 30°/sec, i.e. dynamic as compared to static fixation. 1 Universität Hamburg We recorded and analyzed driving performance of 20 PD patients´ (mean age 63.6 , 4 females) and 20 age matched healthy subjects using an infrared system (Gazetracker) that allowed free head-eye movements within a driving simulator recording, as well as steering, indicator and accelerator/ brake signals. PD symptoms were graded according to: UPDRS pt.1-4, Hoehn & Yahr, MMST and Demtect. Average duration of PD symptoms was 5 years (stdev. - 0.8y.). All patients were on dopaminergic medication and were treated with an implanted STN stimulator (sub-thalamic nucleus stimulator). After some basic oculomotor checks, and 5 minutes of practicing with the simulator driving system, patients had to drive through an unknown realistic course, which lasted about 5 minutes (Bessier Software: 3d driving school), with STN stimulation On, after a 30 min. pause with STN stim. Off, and after another 30 min. rest, under high L-Dopa medication. Typical driving errors were Velocity-, gap selection-, lane keeping, brake/accelerator-, indicator-, traffic sign-, near accident/ 64 University of Minnesota, 2Minneapolis VA Health Care System Spinocerebellar ataxia 6 (SCA6) is a neurodegenerative disease with clinical manifestations indicating cerebellar dysfunction. It is characterized by progressive limb and gait ataxia, dysarthria, and nystagmus along with cerebellar atrophy. A decrease in saccade velocity has been described in some SCA6 patients who are moderately to severely affected by the disease and also in some presymptomatic subjects (Chirstova et al., Arch Neurol 2008; 4:530-536), although the decrease is not to the extent that occurs in other SCA subtypes, e.g., SCA2 (Federighi et al., Brain 2011; 134:879-891). Although decreases in saccade velocity are well documented in patients with brainstem lesions (Ramat et al., Brain 2007; 130:10-35.), velocity changes have been reported in patients with signs of cerebellar dysfunction (Kumar et al., Ann N Y Acad Sci 2005; 1039:404-416; Zivotofsky et al., Brain Res 2006; 1093:135-140), in animal studies with inactivation of the fastigial nucleus (Goffart et al., J Neurophysiol 2004; 92(6):3351-3367), and in modeling studies of cerebellar superior colliculus - brainstem reticular formation interactions (Pelisson et al., Prog Brain Res 2003; 142:6989). These studies indicate that there can be a change in the velocity profile or in the timing of the acceleration and deceleration phases of saccades. The aim of this study was Poster Sessions Full Abstracts to identify and characterize changes in the saccade velocity profile and time course in SCA6 in order to gain further insight into the pathophysiology of SCA6. Data were collected from seven normal control subjects, four presymptomatic subjects, and five ataxic patients. The horizontal and vertical components of eye movements were recorded monocularly using the magnetic search coil technique. The saccade target was displaced 5, 10, 20, and 39 degrees from one side of the center in the horizontal and vertical directions. Matlab programs were used to digitally filter the sampled data, differentiate the eye position signal, calculate the amplitude, velocity, duration, and time of peak velocity for saccades, and calculate confidence ellipses for two-dimensional plots. The results showed that there were changes in the velocity profile in those cases were the peak velocity was less than normal. This could be due to abnormal signals from the cerebellum to the burst/ominpause network in the brainstem in SCA6. 2-D-92 Developmental dyspraxia in children with motor disorders Stefanie Bodison1, Terence Sanger1 1 University of Southern California Developmental dyspraxia is a childhood disorder that affects the child's ability to learn complex motor actions. Developmental dyspraxia is reported to exist in approximately 6% of the population and negatively impacts the child's attainment of gross motor skills, fine motor skills, self-care skills and the play skills needed to effectively participate in age-appropriate activities with peers. While studies have suggested that the underlying mechanisms associated with developmental dyspraxia may be the result of problems in sensory processing, motor planning or limb kinetic deficiencies, few if any have compared these underlying mechanisms across diagnostic groups. Recent research has suggested that children with Autism Spectrum Disorder (ASD), Developmental Coordination Disorder (DCD) and Dystonia may have some level of delopmental dyspraxia, but it was suspected by this research group that the underlying mechanisms of the dyspraxia may vary among the three diagnostic groups. This research study explored whether a newly developed assessment of developmental dyspraxia (the Test of Hand Gestures) could provide insight into the varying underlying mechanisms of dyspraxia and if there are any group differences in developmental dyspraxia among children ages 2-21 years with ASD, DCD and Dystonia. The results to be presented include normative data on the Test of Hand Gestures and preliminary data on the Test of Hand Gestures across the three diagnostic groups described. F - Fundamentals of Motor Control 2-F-28 Investigating motor learning with the ETH Pattus - a robotic approach to studying the neural control of forelimb movements in rodents Bogdan Vigaru1, Olivier Lambercy1, Maximilian Schubring-Giese2, Jonas Hosp2, Melanie Schneider1, Andreas Luft2 , Roger Gassert1 1 ETH Zurich, 2University of Zurich Building on the recent insights robotic manipulanda have given into the neural mechanisms underlying sensorimotor learning and control in human and non-human primates, we developed ETH Pattus, a robotic approach to investigating planar reaching and pronosupination movements in rats. These movements correspond to the widely used training paradigm of grasping a food pellet and moving it to the mouth. The compact, highly transparent three-degree-of-freedom manipulandum can render forces up to 2N to guide or perturb rat forelimb movements, allows to automate the time-consuming training periods, and offers the possibility to quantitatively assess endpoint kinematics and kinetics of rat movements. Preliminary experiments with 10 healthy rats provided evidence for the ability of animals to learn how to touch and interact with the robotic manipulandum by performing a 10mm pulling movement in a null field as well as a guiding and a perturbing force field. Kinematic and kinetic endpoint data collected during interaction suggest that rats modify their behaviour in response to the force fields applied during pulling movements. Training in a haptic tunnel resulted in a significant reduction of the integrated straight line error, which persisted after force field removal. An inter-session decrease in interaction force within the haptic tunnel force field further suggests that rats progressively produced trajectories that were closer to the straight line. Similar to human studies, velocity profiles in rat movements exhibited bell-shaped profiles, which could indicate comparable motor control and optimization strategies. These promising results open up new research avenues for future investigations of motor learning stages in healthy animals as well as in stroke models, where measurements of endpoint kinematics and kinetics could reveal the changes in movement patterns between pre- and post-lesion conditions. 2-F-29 Mental rotation of hand movements in congenitally blind subjects Maitê Mello Russo1, Luis Aureliano Imbiriba2, Laura Alice Santos de Oliveira1, Erika de Carvalho Rodrigues3, Claudia Domingues Vargas1 1 IBCCF/UFR, 2EEFD/UFRJ, 3UNISUAM It has been suggested that mental simulation of an action corresponds to any mental state involving action contents where brain activity seems to mimic or simulate the same aspects of movements, except for the absence of any overt motor behavior. While mentally simulating one's own movement induces the use of primarily motor resources, about the motor imagery of another person's movement activates visual rather than motor processes. Although blindness has been shown to affect visual representation in the brain, much less is known about how higher order action representations are affected by complete visual loss. How does early blindness affect perspective taking? Are blind subjects able to represent spatial visuomotor actions performed by another person? Ten congenitally blind (mean age 25.8 (standard deviation ± 4.1) years) and nineteen sighted adults (mean age 23.6 (± 2.2) years), all righthanded, were instructed to perform a laterality judgment which induced the implicit simulation of their own hand posture (first-person perspective - 1P) or of another person's hand (third-person perspective - 3P) located in front of their face. They were asked to respond as quickly as possible queries about the location of a single finger on the imagined hand after the end of the instruction (Sirigu and Duhamel, 2001). Each instruction was presented through headphones and the subject's verbal response was digitally recorded for offline analysis. Response time (RT), the interval between the end of the instruction and the subject's verbal response, and error rates were calculated for each perspective. Twoway repeated measures ANOVA was employed to compare the RT and the number of errors, with imagery perspective (1P and 3P) as a within-subject factor and group (blind or sighted) as a between-subjects factor. RT of wrong 65 Poster Sessions Full Abstracts responses was not included in the analysis. Statistically significant differences were found for RT and number of errors between groups (F(1, 27)=6.72; p=0.015 and F(1, 27)=9.64; p=0.004, respectively) and perspectives (F(1, 27)=29.69; p=0.00001 and F(1, 27)=6.89; p=0.014, respectively). In all cases, higher values were observed for the blind group and the 3P. No significant interaction was found for any parameter. Taken together, these results suggest that congenitally blind subjects keep the capacity to perform spatial visuomotor imagery of the hand, although presenting a worse performance when compared to sighted subjects. 2-F-30 Muscle specific modulation of corticospinal excitability depends on valence of the object to be grasped Anaelli Aparecida Nogueira-Campos1, Laura Alice Santos de Oliveira1, Valeria Della-Maggiore1, Paula de Oliveira Esteves1, Érika de Carvalho Rodrigues2, Cláudia Domingues Vargas1 1 Instituto de Biofísica Carlos Chagas Filho / UFRJ, 2UNISUAM Daily in life humans are faced with situations where they need to interact with emotional objects. Can the valence of objects influence the brain activity preceding their grasping? The aim of this study was to examine the corticospinal excitability during motor preparation to grasp emotionally-laden objects. Ten participants were instructed to perform two conditions: to grasp (action block) or just to observe (no-action-block) objects with different valences (pleasant, unpleasant and neutral). Four blocks (2 action and 2 no-action) containing 42 objects each were randomly presented. Objects used were balanced in weight and placed inside transparent cups to prompt a similar grip among trials. Participants sat on a comfortable chair in front of a table where the emotionally loaded objects were presented on a sliding slab by an experimenter sitting behind a black curtain. At the beginning of each trial the left arm of the participant laid with the palm facing down over the table (initial position). Trials began with the object presentation. Then, a go signal was turned on 3s later indicating that the participant should grasp the object. Transcranial magnetic stimulation (TMS) was applied over their primary motor cortex randomly at 500 or 250ms before the go signal. Motor evoked potentials (MEP) were measured by recording the electromyographic signal from first dorsalis interosseous (FDI) and abductor digiti minimi (ADM) muscles. MEP amplitude for pleasant and unpleasant trials was normalized relative to neutral trials, creating an unpleasant/neutral and pleasant/neutral ratio. Tree-way ANOVA revealed a condition X valence X muscle interaction (p<0.05). A 2-way ANOVA was applied to find out what muscle was driving this interaction. We found a significant condition X valence interaction (p<0.05) for FDI, which is more directly enrolled in the grasping movement. The MEP amplitude was larger for unpleasant/neutral compared to the pleasant/neutral conditions. There was no significant effect for ADM and no valence effect in the no-action block (p>0.05). In conclusion, we showed here that the preparatory activity preceding the grasping of an object, as measured by corticospinal excitability, is affected by its emotional value. More specifically, our findings show a muscle specific modulation of corticospinal excitability depending on the valence of the object to be grasped during motor preparation. 2-F-31 Directional tuning of arm muscle activation in isometric force generation and its prediction by flexible and synergistic models Daniele Borzelli1, Andrea D'Avella1, Reinhard Gentner1, Timothy Edmunds2, Dinesh K Pai2 1 2 Santa Lucia Foundation, University of British Columbia 66 A long standing question in neuroscience is how the CNS coordinates the large number of degrees-of-freedom of the musculoskeletal system to achieve a variety of goals. One possibility is that CNS optimizes task specific muscle activations according to some performance criterion, such as effort or accuracy, without imposing any constraint on the muscle patterns, i.e. flexibly. Alternatively, to overcome the complexity inherent in a system with many non-linear, compliant actuators acting on multiple skeletal segments, the CNS might reduce the number of degrees-of-freedom it has to control directly by combining a small number of muscle synergies. Evidence for muscle synergies comes from the observation of low-dimensionality in the muscle patterns. However, whether muscle synergies are a simplifying control strategy actually employed by the CNS or they only represent a parsimonious description of the regularities in the motor output generated by a nonsynergistic controller and due to specific task constraints is an open and debated issue. A recent study (Kutch et al. 2008) has argued against muscle synergies in the generation of planar isometric forces on the basis of the comparison of the directional dependence of the covariance of the force fluctuations due to signal-dependent noise observed experimentally with the dependence predicted by either a synergistic model of muscle activation or a model assuming flexible activation of individual muscles. Here we compared the directional tuning of the activation of several muscles acting on the shoulder and elbow joints with those predicted by flexible and synergistic models. We recorded EMGs and forces while five subjects generated planar isometric target forces in 16 directions and 3 different magnitude levels (0.1, 0.2, and 0.3 of their mean maximum voluntary force). We estimated the end-point isometric force generated by each muscle by multiple linear regression of the recorded forces and EMGs. We identified muscle synergies underlying the generation of isometric hand forces using a non-negative matrix decomposition algorithm. Finally, for each target, we predicted muscle activations assuming minimization of the sum of square of the either muscle or synergy activations. We found that the synergistic model predicted the experimental directional tuning of muscle activation better than the flexible model. For all subjects, the mean squared prediction error was lower with the synergistic model. This result supports the hypothesis that the CNS employs combinations of muscle synergies to efficiently, even if not optimally, select the muscle activity patterns required to achieve a goal. However, further work is required to validate the specific approximations and assumptions introduced in the models. In particular, the result depends on the estimation of subject-specific EMG-toForce matrices which was performed with multiple linear regression. Such estimation could be improved by constraining the regression with a priori information on the musculoskeletal geometry and physiological characteristics of the muscles. 2-F-32 Context dependent changes in cue responses prior to movement in primate ventral premotor cortex Lachlan L. Franquemont1, Carlos E Vargas-Irwin1, Michael J Black2, John P Donoghue1 1 Brown University, 2Max Plank Institute for Intelligent Systems Neurons in primate ventral premotor cortex (PMv) are preferentially active during specific types of grasping movements. These neurons may also fire selectively in response to the visual presentation of specific objects. The Poster Sessions Full Abstracts visually evoked responses of PMv neurons have been interpreted as contributing to the transformation of the visual attributes of an object into a motor plan suitable for grasping it. This transformation need not represent a one-to-one mapping, since it is possible to grasp a given object in different ways. We examined the interaction between object and grip-related neural activity with a grasping task where each object could be grasped with multiple grip types. We compared two experimental conditions: one where the grip type is specified prior to the visual presentation of the object (G-O), and one where an object is presented before specifying the grip type (O-G). We also examined an additional control condition similar to O-G that included a delay period without any stimuli. This 'memory' period was followed by a second presentation of the object, allowing us to compare the object-evoked response in two different contexts (with and without a previously cued grip strategy). Neural activity was recorded in a macaque monkey (Macaca Mulatta) using a 96-microelectrode array chronically implanted in PMv on the exposed cortex behind the arcuate genu. About 30 well-isolated single units (with SNR>1.5) were recorded in each session. For each session the monkey alternated between performing blocks of the O-G task and either the G-O or memory task. Note that all tasks required the same motor responses (lifting one of two objects from the same location using the prescribed grip). We found that neural activity evoked by the presentation of the target object varied according to whether it was presented before (O-G condition) or after the grip cue (G-O condition). On average 68.4% of all selective neurons (30.0% of all well isolated units) displayed different firing rates depending on the epoch of object presentation (Kruskal-Wallis test, p<0.01). Similarly, neural activity following the grip cue was different depending on the temporal order of the cues, with an average of 63.6% of selective neurons (27.2% of all well isolated units) displaying statistically different responses. Of all well isolated units that displayed selectivity for grip or object type in, or between, epochs, 97.5% displayed context dependent selectivity. An SVM classifier utilizing the neural population was used to classify epoch as either a grip cue and object presentation. It correctly classified 73.6+/-0.24% of object epochs, and 75.7+/-0.24% of grip epochs (95% confidence boundary at 60.2%) using the first second of neural activity after the object presentation or grip cue. The two object presentations in the memory condition (before and after the grip instruction) also elicited different responses. On average, 85.0% of all selective cells (45.9% of well isolated) neurons responded differently during the two object presentations. These findings show that object-evoked responses in PMv can be influenced by ongoing motor plans for grasping. This suggest that PMv represents an evolving combination of object and grip related information rather than a strict mapping from one to the other. 2-F-49 Reach and grasp encoding in macaque parietal area AIP Hans Scherberger1, Sebastian J Lehmann1 1 German Primate Center The anterior intraparietal cortex (AIP) in the macaque brain is strongly involved in the visuo-motor transformation of hand grasping actions. However, the influence of target location during these reach-to-grasp movements have not been analyzed systematically. Here, we varied the eye and target position in space to study the influence of these signals on the grasp encoding in AIP. Macaques monkeys performed a delayed reachto-grasp task where they grasped a target with one of two grip types while the target and the eye fixation position were systematically varied (total of 26 task conditions; randomly interleaved). Eye position was monitored with an optical eye tracker. Grasp trials consisted of four epochs (fixation, cue, memory, and movement). We recorded 353 single units in AIP (207 in monkey P, 146 in monkey S). Of those, 39.9% (n=141) were highly significantly tuned for grip-type and 71.1% (n=142) for the target position in space in at least one of the epochs 'cue', 'planning' or 'movement' (2-wayANOVA, p<0.01). Grasp-related activity increased towards movement execution, whereas reach-related information was rather constant throughout the task. A substantial number of cells encoded reaching information in retinotopic coordinates, i.e. relative to the gaze position. Furthermore, we simulated the decoding of grasp-type and position from this dataset separately for each epoch using a maximum likelihood approach. Decoding performance was 78% (81% for the second animal) for grasp-type in the cue epoch, 78% (87%) in the planning, and 98% (99%) in the movement epoch, whereas for the different spatial conditions performance was at 61% (59%) for fixation, 91% (83%) for cue, 92% (81%) for planning, and 85% (76%) for the movement epoch (chance level 7.7%). These findings indicate that AIP encodes reach-related information in spatial and retinotopic coordinates in addition to the grasp type. 2-F-50 Role of motor cortex in skill learning depends on learning strategy Risa Kawai1, Tim Markman1, Bence P Ölveczky1 1 Harvard University Much of our behavioral repertoire, whether it is hitting a tennis serve or playing the piano, requires learning and executing complex motor sequences with precision. Motor cortex (MC) is thought to be integral for learning such motor skills, but its specific role remains to be understood. Studies in both rodents and primates have shown some behaviors to be severely affected by MC lesions, while others are largely spared. Here we propose that the role of MC depends on how the motor skill is first acquired. We distinguish two fundamentally different learning processes: (i) trial-and-error learning in which the animal relies on differential reinforcement of internally generated exploratory actions for shaping motor output, and (ii) instructive learning, in which external sensory cues inform the the timing and quality of the behavior. To test the involvement of MC, we manipulated its function by way of lesions and temporary inactivations in rats trained to press a lever with their forepaw in a precise temporal sequence. To explore the dependence of learning strategy, the same behavior was trained either through trial-and-error (i) or with external cues instructing the tap times (ii). In both cases, animals developed highly precise and stereotyped motor behaviors, as measured by the intertap intervals and paw trajectories extracted from high speed movies. Preliminary results show that rats learning the task through trial-and-error were unaffected by MC lesions when tested a week after lesions, while animals learning through instruction showed no memory of the acquired behavior post lesion. Irrespective of which learning strategy was used, reversible pharmacological inactivations of MC severely disrupted the acquired lever press sequence. However, the pharmacological dose required to induce an effect in rats that had learned through instruction was more than an order magnitude smaller than that required for rats trained through trial-and-error. These results suggest that motor sequences acquired through trial-and-error learning are generated by sub-cortical circuits that are only transiently affected by removal of MC, while motor sequences learned with instructive cues are MC dependent. Furthermore, bilateral MC lesions in naive animals did not significant impair 67 Poster Sessions Full Abstracts learning in the trial-and-error paradigm, but showed severe deficits in the instructive learning paradigm. These results suggest that the function of MC in the acquisition and production of motor skills depends on the mode of learning. 2-F-51 The influence of movement speed on variances of target tracking arm movements using a computer mouse Jozsef Laczko1, Bence Borbely2, Gabor Fazekas3, Jozsef Takacs2 2-F-52 How octopuses coordinate their eight flexible arms while crawling Guy Levy1, Tamar Flash2, Benny Hochner1 1 Hebrew University of Jerusalem, 2Weizmann Institute of Science Crawling is a typical way of locomotion of octopuses on the seabed or along rocks in shallow waters or outside of the water. 1 Pazmany Peter Catholic University and Semmelweis University, Pazmany Peter Catholic University, 3National Institute for Medical Rehabilitation 2 We study a visuo-motor task performed by 10 healthy individuals. The subject sat on a chair in front of a computer desk and faced a monitor and saw a moving target on the screen. The instruction was to follow the moving target with the mouse pointer as accurately as possible. The motion of the mouse pointer was controlled by hand movements of the participants using a specific mouse device on a digitizer tablet. There were 2 path and 2 speed conditions for the moving target: circle- and square-shaped paths and normal and fast speeds based on a speed calibration. Subjects performed 10 trials under each condition while coordinates of the arm's anatomical points were recorded using an ultrasound based movement analyzer (ZEBRIS). Arm configuration was defined by 7 rotational angles: 3 in the shoulder (shoulder frontal, shoulder sagittal and upper arm rotation), 2 in the elbow (elbow flexion and lower arm rotation) and 2 in the wrist (wrist elevation and wrist azimuth). Tracking precision, hand position- and arm configuration- variances were computed for each condition. Moving on square path the increased speed induces significantly less accurate pursuing of the target than in the case of circle path. Neither the shape nor the speed brings significant differences in hand position variances, although hand position variance is affected more by the movement speed than by the shape of the path. Higher speed led to increased hand position variances. For higher speed the variance of arm configurations increased at a smaller rate than hand position variances. The structure of arm configuration variances was investigated using the uncontrolled manifold (UCM) approach to discern why arm configuration variances changed relatively less while hand position variances increased with speed. We assume that the structure of arm configuration variances has changed with movement speed in such way that compensated (within the UCM) variance that did not affect mean hand position variance decreased while uncompensated (outside the UCM) variance increased. Thus movement speed affected total arm configuration variance at a smaller rate in comparison to hand position variance since the latter does not depend on the compensated component of arm configuration variance and the drop of this component is not reflected in hand position variance. Consequently hand stability weakened at larger degree than arm configuration stability when the target moved faster. The increment of the uncompensated component of arm configuration variance as a consequence of increased speed was smaller for circle than for square tracking. This may explain the more accurate tracking when the target moved on circle path. We conclude that the subjects rather paid attention to control whole arm movements while watched the target on the screen and moved the mouse on the tablet. For this kind of visou-motor coordination - that is quite common in using computers - the participants tried to reduce mainly arm configuration changes instead of focusing on hand position stability as movement speed increased. 68 The first stage of this study show that octopuses use their arms while crawling for pushing the body by alongation with rhythmical stereotypical steps composed of anchoring several neighboring suckers to the substrate followed by arm elongation to push the body in the direction opposite to the elongating arm. The second stage of the study show how the octopus coordinates its arms when crawling. Mature octopuses were videotaped from underneath while crawling in shallow waters and chosen sections of interest were stored as single images. Arms that participated in pushing and additional points of interest were labeled manually on consecutive images for the kinematic analysis. The data was used to test our hypothesis suggesting that the crawling direction is a simple vectorial combination of the pushing arms and changing the direction of crawling is done simply by activating a suitable set of arms rather than by rotating the body. Following the octopus morphology, the arms are described as spreading around the body symmetrically with a 45º between each adjacent couple of arms. We assume that each arm has a fixed direction of pushing by elongation relative to the body. Using this assumption together with the information regarding the identity of the pushing arms at each moment, we were able to reconstruct, in a simple toy model, the direction of crawling using only the simple vectorial combination of the participating arms. We confirm our hypothesis by showing an excellent match between the reconstructed and the actual crawling path. We suggest that as seen in other octopus movements, control simplification is achieved by using simple stereotypical movements generated at the level of the peripheral neuromuscular system of the arm (rhythmical suckers adherence and length changing) while the participating arms are coordinated by commands from the central brain. Supported by the European Commission 7th Framework (FP7), Project OCTOPUS 2-F-53 Functional characterization of the cholinergic motor innervation in the special neuromuscular system of the octopus arm Nir Nesher1, Naomi Feinstein1, Lili Englister1, Finkel Eran1, Benny Hochner1 1 Hebrew University of Jerusalem The octopus arm is an outstanding example of an efficient skeleton-free motor system. The arm is composed almost entirely of muscle cells, whose constant volume constraint creates a muscular-hydrostat. Previously we showed that the obliquely striated muscle cells are small (1ý~0.01mm), isopotential, lack Na but have a fast Ca 2 spike and appear to be innervated by three excitatory cholinergic synaptic inputs (Matzner et al 2000, Rokni and Hochner 2002). Here we ask if these unique properties involve special neuromuscular junction. Using rhodamine conjugated alphabungarotoxin ( ¿-BGT) to label enzymatically dissociated Poster Sessions Full Abstracts muscle cells and transverse arm sections. ¿-BGT labeling reveal a loclized bulk of AchR were localized, eye-shaped, at the center of the cell; close to the nucleus. The labeled area coincide with membrane invaginations which were clearly visible also in transverse sections. We next characterized the physiological and spatial properties of AChRs. 10 mM ACh was pressure injected through a micropipette at specific locations. In accordance with the ¿-BGT labeling, the strongest and fastest depolarization responses were obtained at the cell center. In contrast to other muscular AChR, these responses were not desensitized. Similar to nerve evoked EPSP in-vivo, the ACh induced current/voltage relationship indicated a linear relationship with a reversal potential close to zero membrane potential. These results confirmed that ACh is the neuromuscular transmitter in the octopus arm and the results fit our previous deduction, based on the electrotonic compactness of the muscle cells, that a single neuromuscular junction is sufficient to control the membrane potential of the muscle cell, with no need for the poly-terminal innervation common in other invertebrates. Anastasia Sylaidi1, Aldo Faisal1 Support: European Commission 7th Framework (FP7), Project OCTOPUS and Smith Family Laboratory the Hebrew university Here we employ an empirical approach to examine three possible hierarchical architectures of human motor learning in object manipulation tasks: (a) The first suggests that task dynamics are principally learned and represented in an intrinsic joint-based frame of reference. Based on this model, task dynamics are learned as a single motor representation for all possible joint configurations but for a single object position. Separate task representations are thus used for different object positions. Conversely, (b) the second scheme predicts that learning is realised with regard to an extrinsic object-based frame of reference, whereas (c) the third scheme points to the independent and equally weighted representation of learned task dynamics both in an intrinsic and extrinsic frame of reference. In the last case separate task representations are used for each different joint- or object configuration. 2-F-54 When emulation becomes reciprocity Luisa Sartori1 1 Università degli studi di Padova It is well known that perceiving another's body movements activates corresponding motor representations in an observer's brain. It is nevertheless true that in many situations simply imitating another's actions would not be an effective or appropriate response, as successful interaction often requires complementary rather than emulative movements. At what point, then, does the automatic tendency to 'mirror' become the inclination to carry out an appropriate, complementary action? In the present study, single-pulse transcranial magnetic stimulation (TMS) was used to evaluate the shift from emulation to reciprocity in observers' corticospinal activity. The effects of single-pulse TMS on the muscle-specificity of motor evoked potentials (MEP) during action observation were measured in participants at time intervals matched to specific kinematic events taking place during a model's movements. MEP were recorded from the abductor digiti minimi (ADM) and the first dorsal interosseus (FDI) muscles of the right hand while participants observed video-clips portraying a sequence of movements which, in some cases, elicited a spontaneous complementary response. We found that observation of such double step action characterized by a final complementary request engendered a shift from simulation to reciprocity in the participants' corticospinal activity. When an observed action calls for a non-identical complementary action, an interplay between the automatic tendency to resonate with what is observed and to implicitly prepare for the complementary action do emerge. A variation in motor evoked potentials (MEP) was noted early, even before the complementary request became explicit. Control conditions in which participants observed the same action sequence but in a context not implying a complementary request were included. As corroborated by kinematic analysis, our findings demonstrate that observers are attuned to advance motor information implicit in observed movements and use it to anticipate a future course of action and to prepare for an appropriate, complementary gesture. Implicit complementary requests might have the ability to prime nonidentical responses. Modulation of corticospinal excitability appears to be a reliable, indirect measure of the automatic human tendency to interact, regardless of whether there will be an effective interaction. 2-F-55 Hierarchical representations of object manipulation tasks 1 Imperial College London Motor tasks are encoded by the brain at a symbolic level (Badre, D., 2008), but are ultimately executed at the level of muscle activations. This illustrates that the central nervous system (CNS) has to solve a problem of hierarchical motor control. In this context motor control problems can be defined with regard to an intrinsic frame of reference based on the body's actuators and sensors (joints and muscles) and/or an extrinsic frame of reference related to task context, environmental settings and object properties. These two frames of reference have been indirectly suggested or explicitly examined in the past (Shadmehr, R., Mussa-Ivaldi, F., 1994; Ahmed, A., 2008; Ingram, J. 2010). However, it remains unclear how motor coordination is linked to one or the other depending on task conditions and whether the frames can be explained as parts of a hierarchical structure underlying motor learning. In order to test the three representation systems as candidate mechanisms of motor control we used a 3D virtual reality setup with body motion tracking. Human subjects were asked to match a rotational task to a given accuracy while holding a half full 1 L bottle of water at varying orientations relative to the hand. In contrast to previous studies, which primarily made use of tools with no internal degrees of freedom or static dynamics (Shadmehr, R., Mussa-Ivaldi, F., 1994), we selected naturalistic object dynamics to facilitate motor tasks encountered in real-life conditions. We measured subjects' performance in terms of the pivot point displacement and examined how learning is generalised to multiple test orientations after exposure to a single training orientation of the object. An inverse experimental protocol was subsequently implemented, in which subjects were asked to perform object rotations while the object's orientation remained constant relative to the hand, whereas joint coordinates varied across different experimental sessions. Our results revealed a significant difference between pivot point displacement in training and testing phases for both experiments (Wilcoxon test, p<0.0125). This difference demonstrates a poor generalization capability of learned task dynamics to novel task contexts. It thus provides supporting evidence for our third scheme of hierarchical motor control in which different task representations are employed for different joint and object configurations. As such, our work introduces a novel, inclusive formalisation for capturing learning during the interaction with objects of complex internal dynamics. 69 Poster Sessions Full Abstracts 2-F-56 Expectation about movement error and its influence on real-time reach control Darian Cheng1, Brendan Cameron1, Gordon Binsted1 1 University of British Columbia When error arises during an unfolding movement, our sensorimotor system rapidly corrects for it. These online corrections generally occur without our awareness, and they are largely resistant to our attempts to override them, though there is evidence that they can be partially suppressed under the right conditions (Cameron et al. 2009; Streimer et al. 2010; McIntosh et al. 2010). In the present study, we further examined the limits on the 'automaticity' of online corrections. Specifically, we tested whether expectation about upcoming movement error could be used to enhance online responding. Participants made targeted movements with a stylus on a graphics tablet. They were given a pre-cue at the start of each trial that indicated whether the target was likely to jump on that trial (80% probability) or unlikely to jump on that trial (20% probability). The target jump (up or down) was orthogonal to the primary reach vector, and no information was provided by the pre-cue about the direction of the jump. When a jump occurred, it was coincident with the onset of the movement. In one condition (Pro-point), participants were instructed to correct their movement to the target's jump location. In the other condition (Anti-point), participants were instructed to correct their movement to the location opposite the jump location. In the Propoint condition we examined the latency of the online correction to the target, while in the Anti-point condition we examined the latencies of both the initial (unintended) correction towards the target and the secondary (intended) correction away from the target. In the Pro-point condition we did not find clear evidence of a faster correction when participants were expecting a target jump. In the Anti-point condition, however, we observed 1) a slower and smaller initial correction when participants were expecting a jump and 2) a faster secondary correction when participants were expecting a jump. That is, the anticipation of a pending jump appeared to allow some suppression of the automatic response to the target in addition to a speeding of the voluntary response. 2-F-57 Thalamo-cortical network in locomotion Irina Beloozerova1, Vladimir Marlinski1, Mikhail G Sirota1 1 Barrow Neurological Institute Locomotion is one of the most essential and frequently used behaviors. The neural mechanism that determines the order of muscular contractions and the coordination of limb movements during locomotion resides in the spinal cord. The spinal mechanism, however, lacks the distant information about the outside world, in which locomotion takes place and thus has to be adapted to, and lacks information about the purpose of locomotion. The motor centers of the brain adapt locomotion to the peculiarities of the environment and to the needs of a subject. The motor thalamo-cortical network plays a central role in this adaptation. The ventro-lateral thalamus (VL), a part of "motor thalamus", receives locomotion-related information from the spinal cord both directly and indirectly via the cerebellum. It receives information about the outside world from the cerebellum and also likely from the cortex. The VL merges these two streams of information and conveys the resulting signal for locomotion adaptation up to the motor cortex (MC) for delivery to the spinal cord. This signal from the VL is, however, gated by the reticular nucleus of thalamus (RE), a layer of inhibitory neurons on the rostro-lateral border of the thalamus. The RE is under direct influence from the MC. 70 In this study in freely walking cats, we analyzed single neuron activity in the VL, RE, and MC during two locomotion tasks. One of them was a walk on a flat surface, a task that can be accomplished by the spinal cord alone, while another was stepping on cross-pieces of a horizontal ladder, a task for which vision is essential and which thus requires the activity of thalamo-cortical network in order to be successful. Based on this analysis, we defined five modes of integration of locomotor-based and vision-based information in the VL; we uncovered distinction in the thalamic and cortical controls over the shoulder, elbow, and wrist; and revealed striking differences in signals that are channeled through different cortical efferent systems during locomotion. Using this information we propose a novel model of how thalamocortical network contributes to visual guidance of locomotion. 2-F-58 Limb movement amplitude systematically influences temporal order judgments Robert Hermosillo1, Paul van Donkelaar1 1 University of British Columbia – Okanagan When generating limb movements, motor planning signals affect the perception of somatosensory signals to allow for differentiation between self- and externally-imposed cutaneous sensations. In the present study, we examine whether limb movements can systematically influence temporal order judgments based on the relative position of the limbs and amplitude of the movement. We have previously shown that planning to cross the arms increases error rates for temporal order judgments (TOJs) of cutaneous stimuli delivered to the tip of each index finger. To further investigate how movement planning influences TOJ decisions, we performed an experiment in which participants moved their limbs symmetrically or asymmetrically. In particular, we investigated whether or not the amplitude of the movement affected the accuracy in the TOJ task. Results showed that when the hand generating the larger amplitude movements was stimulated first, error rates in the TOJ task were larger relative to when the hand generating the shorter amplitude movements was stimulated first. By contrast, we observed no difference between error rates for either hand during symmetrical movements. Taken together, this implies that some aspect of planning movements which vary in amplitude interacts with the decision-making processing associated with judgments of temporal order. 2-F-59 Properties of force fields in the primate arm induced by intraspinal microstimulation Hiroaki Yaguchi1, David P Kowalski2, Tomohiko Takei1, Kazuhiko Seki1 1 National Institute of Neuroscience, 2Drexel University Isometric force fields output by the limb have been evaluated in the frog, rat, and cat, and the concept of the motor primitive has been postulated as the underlying fundamental mechanism in these systems. However, the force field generated by intraspinal microstimulation of the cervical cord has yet to be studied in the monkey, and as such it is not known if the primate spinal cord still follows the same principle for movement control. Toward this end, two macaque monkeys were chronically implanted at the C5-7 level with floating microelectrode arrays, each consisting of 12 electrode shanks arranged in a three by four grid with an intershank spacing of 2.5mm and a uniform penetration depth of 3mm (Monkey U) or 4mm (Monkey E). These arrays were specifically chosen for their usability as a Poster Sessions Full Abstracts chronically implanted device, and have previously been used as a stimulating electrode array in acute experiments for individual muscle recruitment. EMG recording electrodes were implanted in the left shoulder (N=3) and arm muscles (N=5-6) . Under atropine-medetomidine (Dormitor®) anesthesia, the monkey was placed in a supine position and the arm moved throughout its normal range of motion lateral to the body on a 4 or 8cm grid. A 200µs bipolar stimulation train was applied at 40Hz for 500ms to either one or two electrode sites. Different force fields were evoked which, in some cases, contained an equilibrium point or line where the force output was zero with restorative forces being generated at other positions, while others were uniformly directed as would be the case for direct recruitment of motor pools or passing motor fibers. Further, each stimulation site evoked activity in particular sets of muscles, or synergies, which were in some cases dependent on the configuration of the arm. Simultaneously stimulating two sites mainly led to a winner-takeall or non-linear response at an electrode depth corresponding to Lamina VII (3mm from cord surface), while an electrode depth corresponding to Lamina IX or the anterior funiculus (4mm) lead to a linear summation in most cases. In some cases, stimulation at one site inhibited the ability of the second site to evoke movement even after stimulation of the first site had ceased, evidence that a long-term pattern of interneuronal activity was being evoked. We also found that the finger or arm movement was evoked for more than five months, and evoked force fields were stable over that time on all but one channel. These results suggest the feasibility of long-term stimulation of the primate spinal cord through implanted microelectrode array for restoring upper arm movement in a patient with spinal cord injury. In conclusion, although the level of dexterity in the primate upper limb is defined mainly by descending control from the brain, the underlying motor primitives are still present and can be recruited in a similar manner to what has been previously shown in lower animals. 2-F-60 Evidence for distinct posture and movement states at the neural population level Nicholas Sachs1, Christian Ethier1, Rachel M Cassidy1, David P Bontrager1, Zachary A Wright1, Lee E Miller1 1 Northwestern University One of the fundamental questions in motor behavior is whether the contrasting goals of limb posture and movement are realized though distinct specialized processes or are simply the result of a single robust control system. Single unit recording and human psychophysics studies have addressed this question, but there appears to be a lack of analysis at the neural population level to support either theory. We analyzed ensemble neural activity simultaneously recorded from tens of neurons in the primary motor cortex of monkeys during sequences of point-to-point reaches separated by stable hold periods. Attempts to model the relationship between neural discharge and limb endpoint kinematics with a linear filter or Wiener cascade nonlinearity across the entire dataset were moderately successful, however we discovered two sub-regions within which the relationship between neural discharge and limb endpoint velocity was very nearly locally linear. These two sub-regions were distinguishable by limb endpoint velocity, comprising lower velocities associated with limb stabilization and higher velocities associated with movements between targets, respectively. These findings support the existence of distinct posture and movement states at the neural population level. Furthermore, we could identify these states accurately using a linear classifier trained on neural firing rates, where ground truth posture and movement classes were distinguished based on a threshold applied to limb endpoint speed. Investigation of individual firing rates within the neural population indicated that some neurons had a tendency to increase their mean firing rates in the movement state relative to those in the posture state, while others demonstrated the opposite effect. This suggests the presence of different classes of neurons that may be consistent with the description of M-class and S∆ neurons presented by Humphrey and Reed (1983). Algorithms for brain-machine interfaces (BMIs) typically have not distinguished between postural and movement intents when decoding neural activity. In order to test the potential role of movement state detection in BMI applications we devised a dual-state control paradigm that calculated cursor velocity with a decoder based on a mixture of linear filters that were trained exclusively on neural data during either posture or movement periods from the reaching experiment described above. During online testing the mixture weights assigned to the movement and posture filters were calculated according to the following equations: w(movement)=1/(1 e^(-4(d-2.5))) w(posture)=1-w(movement) where 'd' represented the distance (in cm) between the center of the cursor and center of the target. This was based on the assumption that the subject would attempt to stabilize the cursor near the target, but would want to move the cursor more freely when moving toward new targets, potentially engaging its motor cortical neurons with patterns similar to those present in natural posture and movement states. We compared the performance of monkeys using this dual-state decoder to control cursor movement with the use of a single linear filter. Monkeys consistently performed better with the dual-state decoding method than with the linear filter, suggesting that the use of algorithms that respect differences between posture and movement states in the neural space, consistent with those observed during normal behavior, may be beneficial when designing BMIs. 2-F-61 Dissociating anticipatory control of digit positions and force in the primary motor cortex Marco Davare1, Qiushi Fu2, Jason Choi2, Marco Santello2 1 Institute of Neurology, 2Arizona State University When manipulating objects, visual cues and past experience enable us to prepare an accurate hand shaping and to adjust fingertip forces according to the object intrinsic properties. How anticipatory control of digit positions and force is processed in the primary motor cortex (M1) is still debated. In a total of eight subjects, we used transcranial magnetic stimulation (TMS) to quantify changes in corticospinal excitability (CSE) during tasks involving different hand shaping and force control. The object consisted of an inverted T-shaped manipulandum with force/torque sensors to measure digit placement and force development. In the first task, subjects had to place their thumb and index finger on pre-defined points on the object and exert a force equal to 10% of their maximum voluntary contraction (MVC, 'place+squeeze' condition) or simply place their fingers on the same points on the object without exerting force ('place'). In control conditions, we also asked subjects to perform the task in a reversed order ('squeeze+place') or to simply squeeze the object using a 10% MVC force level ('squeeze'). In the second task, subjects had to grip and lift the object using a precision grip at constrained or self-chosen digit locations. The object center of mass was shifted to the right so that to minimize object roll during lift, subjects had to anticipate precisely either digit forces or digit forces and positioning on the object, respectively. TMS was delivered during movement preparation (the TMS click was the 'go' signal) and motor evoked potentials (MEP) where measured in the first dorsal interosseus and abductor pollicis brevis muscles. We found 71 Poster Sessions Full Abstracts that the peak-to-peak MEP amplitudes were larger in the 'place' compared to the 'place+squeeze' condition (on average 46 and 27% increase in the FDI and APB, respectively). MEP amplitudes were also larger the 'squeeze+place' compared to the 'squeeze' condition (41 and 36% increase in the FDI and APB, respectively). The fact that a significant difference in CSE was found between 'place' vs. 'place+squeeze' conditions indicates the involvement of different neural resources even though the first event (digit positioning) was identical in the two tasks. Moreover, the increase in CSE was not specific to digit placement being the first event per se as 'squeeze+place' also elicited larger MEPs than 'squeeze'. Together, these results indicate that cortical excitability during grasp planning is not merely related to adding more elements to a task sequence as one-event task elicited larger MEPs than a two-event task. Finally, we found larger MEPs (16 and 21% increase in the FDI and APB, respectively) using self-chosen rather than constrained digit locations, which suggests that planning the coordination of digit placement and forces required for unconstrained grasping and manipulation relies on different neural mechanisms than force planning associated with constrained grasping. Our preliminary results suggest that the corticospinal system attributes different neural resources to the anticipatory control of finger positioning and force scaling. It is possible that the visuomotor circuits related to the control of hand shaping and digit contact distribution have a larger gain than the sensorimotor pathways related to the anticipatory control of force. 2-F-62 Development of spatial and temporal bimanual coordination during childhood Betteco de Boer1, Lieke Peper1, Peter J Beek1 1 VU University The effects of development on bimanual spatial and temporal coordination were examined by comparing task performance among four age groups: 6/7 years, 10/11 years, 14/15 years, and young adults. During development across these ages, myelination of the corpus callosum may induce changes in interlimb coupling, along with many other relevant changes in neuronal sites and networks. Spatial coupling was assessed using a bimanual line-circle drawing task. The effect of coupling was assessed by comparing performance in drawing the two different shapes simultaneously (i.e., line-circle) to bimanual drawing of the same shapes (i.e., line-line and circle-circle) and unimanual drawing of a single shape (i.e., line or circle). Temporal coupling was assessed by studying the stabilizing contributions of interlimb interactions related to movement planning, error correction, and reflex interactions. For this purpose, participants performed rhythmic flexion-extension movements of the wrist in a horizontal plane. By comparing several tasks in which these interactions related to planning, correction, and reflexes are involved to a different extent, the stabilizing contribution of each of these interactions to the temporal coordination pattern was assessed. Temporal coupling strength and coordinative stability increased with age, at similar rates for in-phase and antiphase coordination. Age-related changes in underlying interlimb interactions differed for the three interaction sources: the relative contributions of planning and reflex interactions to the achieved stability did not change with age, whereas interactions involving error corrections improved. Spatial drawing performance increased with age, as evidenced by improved circularity, decreased variability, and improved smoothness. With increasing age, participants were better in drawing two different shapes simultaneously, as evidenced by a smaller deterioration in the circularity index of circle drawing for adults than children when drawing line and circle together relative to drawing two circles or one circle. 72 Thus, temporal coupling changes were observed gradually across age 6 to adulthood, whereas spatial coupling changes occurred mainly after age 14/15. This difference in the development of temporal and spatial coupling corresponds to the anterior-posterior direction of corpus callosum myelination as reported in the literature. Furthermore, development of in-phase and antiphase temporal coordination was not different across age groups, indicating that improved inhibition of mirror activity is not the main effector of improvements in bimanual coordination. 2-F-63 Neuronal correlates of running speed in the dorsal striatum Pavel Rueda-Orozco1, David Robbe1 1 Institut d'Investigacions Biomèdiques August Pi i Sunyer (IDIBAPS) (NIF:Q5856414G) Dysfunction of the basal ganglia results in severe motor disabilities but how this subcortical network contributes to normal motor control is still poorly understood. We designed a new behavioral paradigm to study how the striatum contributes to the control of the locomotion dynamics. Rats were trained to run on automatized treadmill equipped on one side of a salient photo-detector and a reward delivery port. Breaking the beam of the photo-detector stops the treadmill and triggers the delivery of a drop of sucrose. Rats are progressively trained to run for at least 7 seconds before stopping the treadmill. Early breaks of the photo-detector beam result in a 15 seconds-long running penalty and absence of sucrose delivery. Rats succeeded in this task by learning to perform very stereotyped runs during which they modulate their speed in a timely manner. Following training, rats were implanted with tetrode arrays in the dorsolateral part of striatum. We found that the spiking activity of about 70% of the recorded cells was strongly modulated during the task. Specifically, while the animals ran, cells fired across successive trials in a reliable order. Remarkably a large fraction of those cells had their firing rate sharply correlated with running speed and those correlations could not be accounted by spatial or temporal bias on the firing rate. Our results suggest that dorsolateral striatum plays a central role in the control of the kinematic of well-learned movements. G - Theoretical & Computational Motor Control 2-G-64 Gravity-compensating muscular torque explains biases in perceived arm-movement extent Nienke Debats1, Jeroen Smeets1, Robert J van Beers1 1 VU University Amsterdam In daily life, humans seem to adequately control their arm movements. This suggests that the nervous system is quite accurate in sensing the limbs' position and movements. However, several examples of kinesthetic biases prove otherwise. One example of such a bias is the haptic radialtangential illusion: if a human perceiver traces the outline of a rectangular shape in the horizontal plane, the length of the radial segment is substantially overestimated relative to the length of the tangential segment. For a rectangle to feel square, the radial segment has to be shorter by about 13% to 20%. This illusion thus indicates substantial biases in the perceived extent of arm movements in the horizontal plane. At present, the underlying cause of these biases is unclear. Poster Sessions Full Abstracts Recently, it was proposed that the radial-tangential illusion is related to gravity (Debats et al., 2010) When keeping your unsupported arm horizontal at shoulder level, a certain torque is needed to counteract gravity from pulling the arm down. The magnitude of this torque depends on the position of the arm's center of mass, and thus on its orientation in the horizontal plane. When making an outward or inward radial arm movement in the horizontal plane, the magnitude of this torque increases or decreases, respectively. The difference in torque between the start and endpoint of the movement is what we refer to as ∆Torque. The present study tests the ∆Torque-hypothesis (Debats et al., 2010), that is, whether the magnitude of ∆Torque affects the strength of the illusion. Blindfolded participants (n = 18) performed a two-alternative forced-choice perceptual task in which they traced an L-shaped figure with the index finger of their right hand. They verbally indicate whether the radial segment of the L-figure was either longer or shorter than the tangential segment. The L-figure was presented at shoulder height in the horizontal plane. It had a fixed tangential segment length (20 cm), and eleven possible radial segment lengths (13.5 cm to 18.5 cm with steps of 0.5 cm). Each size was presented ten times. From the psychometrical functions we obtained the point of subjective equality, which indicates the relative overestimation of the radial segment and thus the strength of the illusion. We increased the magnitude of ∆Torque by adding mass (0.5 kg) to the participants' wrists. There were two control conditions: one in which no extra mass was placed, and one in which the extra mass was attached to the participants' elbow. In this latter condition, the absolute amount of torque needed to counteract gravity was increased, while ∆Torque was not. between sensory and motor areas of the brain of anaesthetized rats and a simulated point mass moving in a viscous medium. This can be achieved by programming the interface for generating control policies in the form of force fields acting on the controlled external device. We implemented this behavior through a sensory interface which maps the state of the device into a pattern of electrical stimuli, and a motor interface which translates the recorded neural activity into a force vector. The spiking activity recorded from multiple units is converted into a twodimensional force vector by using Principal Component Analysis (PCA). However, a limitation of our previous work is that the mathematical model for sensory and motor maps implemented so far required the desired force field to be invertible. This limited severely the range of control problems that the interface can afford to address. Moreover, the algorithms for extracting information from neural activity that we used in this previous work were not yet able to extract an optimal amount of information and this limited the control that neurons could exert on the dynamical system interacting with the brain. Debats, N.B., Kingma, I., Beek, P.J. & Smeets, J.B.J (2010). "Muscular torque can explain biases in haptic length perception: a model study on the radial-tangential illusion". Lecture Notes in Computer Science 6192, 392-397. Here we present novel techniques that can overcome these limitations and we compare the performances of the resulting algorithms with respect to the original one on simulated data. In particular we study the effect of substituting PCA with Isomap, a nonlinear dimensionality reduction technique able to find nonlinear degrees of freedom. We also study a new version of the algorithm in which the sensory interface is set such that the electrical stimuli encoding neighbouring portion of space will elicit more similar spike trains and electrical stimuli that are encoding more distant portions of space will elicit more diverse spike trains. We achieve this by computing distances in terms of spike train metrics between stimulusevoked activities of recorded neurons, and by projecting them onto the sensory domain by Multi Dimensional Scaling. We then associate each stimulus to the center of mass of its evoked spike trains and partition the space into sensory regions with the nearest neighbour algorithm. The motor interface then decodes the evoked activity by computing the most likely stimulus eliciting it. The force that is associated with the portion of space corresponding to the decoded stimulus is then applied to the dynamical system. 2-G-65 Algorithms for shaping the dynamics of a bidirectional neural interface 2-G-66 Transitions between rhythmic and discrete performance in unimanual movements Marianna Semprini1, François D Szymanski1, Francesco Grussu2, Ferdinando A Mussa-Ivaldi3, Stefano Panzeri1, Alessandro Vato1 Hamal Marino1, Neville Hogan2, Marcos Duarte3, Steven Charles4, Lauren DiPietro2, Dagmar Sternad5 We found no difference in illusion strength between the two control conditions and a 7% increase in strength with an increased ∆Torque. These results demonstrate that an increase in ∆Torque is sufficient to alter the strength of the radial-tangential illusion, whereas an increase in muscular torque alone is not. This suggests that the perceived extent of arm movements is influenced by the changing amount of torque that is needed to keep the arm lifted during the movement. 1 Istituto Italiano di Tecnologia, 2University of Genoa, Northwestern University 3 Brain Machine Interfaces (BMIs) offer a promising route towards the recovery of motor functions in patients paralyzed by stroke or spinal cord injury. While BMIs conventionally use the decoded neural activity recorded in the motor cortex in order to control actuators, bidirectional BMIs do also make use of microstimulation to directly provide the brain with information about the outside world. The establishment of a two-way communication between brain and devices is crucial for restoring motor functions after paralysis. Our group has recently developed a novel computational and experimental framework called dynamic BMI (dBMI), with the goal of reproducing some of the mechanisms by which the spinal cord operates the translation of cortical commands into motor behavior. The emulation of spinal cord operations is achieved by establishing and shaping the bidirectional communication 1 Scuola Superiore Sant'Anna, 2Massachusetts Institute of Technology, 3University of Sao Paulo, 4Brigham Young University, 5Northeastern University While everyday actions flexibly combine rhythmic and discrete movements, motor control research has largely studied these movement types in isolation. Our previous work provided some arguments and evidence from brain imaging data that rhythmic and discrete movements may form two distinct primitives. The present experiment used behavioral data in a parametric scaling paradigm to test whether abrupt transitions between the two types of movement can be elicited. Abrupt transitions would further support the hypothesis that these are two separate control primitives. Ten subjects performed planar two-joint forearm movements w on a low-friction horizontal surface, with their wrist-joint immobilized, moving their arm between two large targets (i.e., with a minimal end-point accuracy constraint). 73 Poster Sessions Full Abstracts The back-and-forth movements were paced by a metronome: Starting with 20 sounds at 2 sec intervals, the metronome intervals decreased uniformly by 36 ms each over the next 50 intervals to reach an interval of 200 ms. After a plateau with 20 sounds at 200 ms intervals the intervals lengthened again by the same 36 ms over the next 50 intervals; after return to the initial 2 sec this interval was presented for a final 20 sounds. The instruction to subjects was to end each movement by remaining at rest in the target position for a duration (dwell time) equal to the movement time. The experiment was performed under two metronome conditions: each sound was of the same (brief) duration or the duration of the metronome sound matched the instructed dwell time. Results showed that subjects responded imperfectly to the changes in interval. Despite the systematic and predictable pattern of metronome intervals, subjects did not anticipate the constant change correctly and consistently lagged behind. Although dwell time was instructed to be equal to movement time, it decreased substantially below that value--as expected, subjects were unable to sustain discrete movements at the fastest pace--and dwell time reduced to zero on average at a metronome interval of 940 ms. These results were observed even if information about dwell time was perceptually enhanced by the duration of the metronome sound. Remarkably, the transition between movements with non-zero dwell time and movement with zero dwell time was abrupt. Acceleration profiles showed discontinuous changes, with different subjects exhibiting different transition points. Velocity profiles showed non-uniform changes in symmetry and discrete relative phase between position and acceleration peaks profile showed discontinuous changes. Overall, results were consistent with the hypothesis that discrete and rhythmic movements form two distinct movement types. Support: NIH: R01 HD045639, NSF: BCS-0450218 2-G-67 Modeling gaze-dependent errors when reaching to visual and proprioceptive targets Joost Dessing1, Masahiro Kokubu1, Armin Abadeh1, Patrick A Byrne1, J. Douglas Crawford1 1 York University Reaching movements in the dark overshoot memorized visual targets relative to the gaze direction held just before reach onset, even if a saccade intervenes between target presentation and reach onset. Traditionally, these errors have been suggested to reflect misestimates of target position, arising when transforming visual target position into body centered coordinates. Recently, however, we showed that these gaze dependent reach errors were entirely suppressed with online visual feedback of the hand. This was most parsimoniously explained by a model in which gaze dependent biases arise within the transformation of proprioceptive hand position into gaze centered coordinates; when vision of the hand is continuously available the brain would mostly rely on accurate, visual hand position, thus eliminating the reach errors. Here, we expand on these observations. We included movement planning in multiple reference frames into our model and allowed for all possible transformation errors, as well as (visual and proprioceptive) workspace-dependent errors. To constrain the model parameters, we will record pointing errors in paradigms involving multiple target and fixation directions in a variety of tasks (continuous visual targets or proprioceptive targets [with or without online visual feedback]). Preliminary findings for proprioceptive targets showed that gaze direction influenced the reach errors (largely undershoots), while this effect increased with online visual feedback for some subjects but decreased for others. To account for these and other observations, we will fit our model to the reach errors of individual subjects. This will reveal the contribution of different biases within the visuomotor transformation to the observed reaching behaviour, thus pinpointing the relative role of target and hand- 74 related transformation biases. The individual variations in the preliminary data for instance suggest that, besides proprioception-to-vision transformation biases, some subjects also show evidence of vision-to-proprioception transformation biases. 2-G-68 Behavioral insights into neural mechanisms of movement planning: Continuous and abrupt updating of a motor plan following changes in task goals David Huberdeau1, Adrian M Haith1, John Krakauer1 1 Johns Hopkins University Producing the commands to successfully reach a movement goal requires a period of preparation prior to movement onset, evidenced by the so-called "reaction time advantage" whereby reaction time is lower if movement goals are known in advance. Neurophysiological studies have revealed clear neural correlates of movement preparation in motor and premotor cortex. However, the underlying representations and computations occurring during the period prior to movement execution are not yet clearly understood. One hypothesis is that the each potential movement corresponds to a unique state of the motor cortex and the goal of the preparatory period is to guide the motor cortical activity towards the state that represents the best movement for the present task (Churchland, 2010). An alternative hypothesis is that multiple potential movements can be simultaneously represented, with the preparatory period constituting a period of competitive selection between potential movements (Cisek, 2011). Here we sought to address this question behaviorally by forcing subjects to generate movements at intermediate stages of planning. We used a timed response paradigm (Ghez, 1997) in which subjects were trained to launch center-out reaching movements at a pre-specified time. One of eight targets equally distributed around a circle was visible for 1s prior to movement onset, allowing ample preparation time for the movement. On a subset of trials (15%), we jumped the target by 45° or 90° to the left or right at a random time prior to movement onset. This allowed us to vary the time permitted for movement replanning. Subsequent movement kinematics were measured and the initial launch angle computed. Fourteen subjects participated in our experiment. We predicted that, if movement preparation occurs through a continuous adjustment of a single control policy, then at intermediate stages of re-planning following a target jump, we would observe initial movement directions that were intermediate between the initial and jumped targets. Alternatively, if multiple movements can be planned simultaneously, there should be an abrupt switch from movements towards the original target to movements aimed towards the new target location as re-planning time increases. For 45° jumps, we found that launch angle varied continuously as a function of re-planning time. By contrast, for 90° jumps, launch angles tended to clustered around either the initial or final target directions, with no substantial intermediate behaviors. The emergence of intermediate movements for incompletely replanned movements when changes in task goals were small (45°), lends credence to the hypothesis that movement planning occurs through continuous specification of a single movement, rather than through competition between multiple potential goals. However, the abrupt switching we observed in movement kinematics for large changes in task goals (90°) supports the notion that two distinct movement plans can be held simultaneously. Churchland et.al. (2010) Cortical preparatory activity: Representation of movement or first cog in a dynamical machine? Neuron 68(3):387-400. Poster Sessions Full Abstracts Cisek et.al. (2011) Neural correlates of biased competition in premotor cortex. J Neuro Sci. 31(19):7083-88. Ghez, et. al. (1997) Discrete and continuous planning of hand movements and isometric force trajectories. Experimental Brain Research 115(2):217-33. 2-G-69 Heterogeneous attractor modules for motor planning in macaque premotor cortex Maurizio Mattia1, Stefano Ferraina2, Pierpaolo Pani2, Giovanni Mirabella2, Stefania Costa2, Paolo Del Giudice1 1 Istituto Superiore di Sanità, 2Sapienza University Cognitive functions like motor planning rely on the concerted activity of multiple neuronal assemblies underlying still elusive computational strategies. Here we show that in macaque monkeys performing a reaching countermanding task, motor plans coded in dorsal premotor cortex (PMd) can be detected as multi-unit activity (MUA) patterns resulting from a network of cortical modules14. We found sudden and stereotyped MUA sharp transitions (STs), signalling an increase or a decrease in the population firing, as a late reaction to target appearance, and predicting at single trial level forthcoming actions. Occurrence of such STs was observed even when movement was successfully cancelled after a stop signal or during delay epochs of delayed reaching task, excluding that they are the mere substrate of the motor execution. We developed an attractor multi-modular network of spiking neurons which accounted for STs and predicted a peculiar modulation of high-frequency Fourier components of unfiltered local field potentials (LFP). In vivo observations confirmed such theoretical framework, providing a strong evidence that local synaptic reverberation was in action, making neuronal modules bistable. Our results demonstrate that motor plans mature in PMd as an emergent cooperative representation driven by a subset of "active" modules with strong self-excitation, capable to amplify subthreshold input and to recruit other "passive" modules with weaker synaptic local feedback like in a chain reaction ending up in a stereotyped distributed representation. What emerges is a general computational machinery composed of a web of bistable modules acting as interacting "flip-flops", as prescribed by the longstanding theory of associative networks. 2-G-70 The traveling salesman problem in human motor tasks Jakob Uecker1, Aldo Faisal1 1 Imperial College London Finding the shortest route that connects a number of points is known as the traveling salesman problem (TSP), which is known for the exponential complexity of its computation. Previous studies of human performance on the TSP in various cognitive tasks (MacGregor et al., 1996; Vickers et al., 2001; Best, 2005) have reported that subjects routinely solve problems of sizes up to 20 points nearly optimally within a few minutes, with several subjects finding the actual optimal solution. This is considerably better than the performance of algorithms on present day computers and arguably achieved with far less raw computing power. Most of these studies have allowed subjects ample time to evaluate and alter their solutions until satisfied (e.g. providing pencil and eraser) and all have treated the problem as purely combinatorial with the goal of optimizing Euclidean distance. In this study, we present a novel experimental paradigm for testing human performance in a visual motor TSP reaching task not previously used in studies with human subjects. Specifically, we set tight limits on motor planning and movement time to observe how planning time scales with an increasing number of points. Subjects had to solve a visually presented TSP instance in a virtual reality environment by moving their dominant hand to touch all targets as quickly as possible. Targets were generated at random positions within a circle of 42cm radius. Each subject (n=10) solved 800 trials, with each trial presenting 5, 10, 15 or 20 targets. Subjects received visual performance feedback via a screen message after each trial -- positive if they completed it in time smaller than 0.4 seconds per target and negative otherwise. We find that subjects behave optimally or near optimally for N=5 targets and that performance decays sub-linearly for larger N. Performances on the standard measure for optimization (total Euclidean distance) are on average worse than those reported for nonmovement tasks in other studies, which can be explained by the fact that the allowed time per trial differs by one to three orders of magnitude as compared to those studies. However, we find that those movement decisions of human subjects which are suboptimal with respect to minimizing distance have a predictable pattern. Based on this pattern we propose that the Euclidean distance cost function may not accurately capture the characteristics of good performances in conditions of high density targets where the optimal path features frequent changes of movement direction that are atypical of human reaching movements. Under these conditions, humans appear to minimize the time needed for task completion by taking into account the dynamic mechanical state of their arm: we find that deviations from the distance-wise optimal movement trajectory exhibit a consistent preference for straight-line motion over sharp turns. We propose an alternative to the Euclidean distance cost function and model the dynamics of our experiment to evaluate both the performance benefits of such a cost function and its accuracy in predicting subjects' movements. 2-G-71 Towards the metabolic basis of the cost function in human motor control Scott Taylor1, Aldo A Faisal1 1 Imperial College London Optimal feedback control theory - which derives control policies from the minimisation of a cost function - has been very successful in explaining human movements in a principled manner (Todorov & Jordan, 2002; Diedrichsen & Shadmehr, 2010). However, little is known about its neuronal implementation or the cost functions used by the motor system (Scott, 2004). Here we take a top-down approach, by conducting reaching tasks and analyzing the data. Crucially, we solve the inverse optimal control problem in order to infer the cost function underlying human motor coordination for planar reaching tasks and relate it to a trade-off of two biophysically motivated cost functions: signal-dependent motor noise (Harris & Wolpert, 1998) and, as novelty, the metabolic cost of muscle activation. Mammalian muscle can be modelled as populations of fast and slow muscle fibres, which have characteristic metabolic efficiencies (Reggiani et al. 2000) and activation regimes (Tansey et al. 1996). At low muscle force levels slow muscle fibres are predominantly activated with metabolically less efficient fast fibres being activated only at higher force levels. While the relationship between metabolic energy consumption and force production in a single fibre is linear, considering a population of muscle fibres allows the derivation of a quadratic cost function dependent on the ratio of fast to slow muscle fibres. Crucially, this fibre ratio varies over a muscular system in a manner not related to the size of motor pools (the leading explanation for variations in signal dependent noise (Hamilton et al. 2004)), suggesting a difference between optimal behaviours in 75 Poster Sessions Full Abstracts signal-dependent noise and metabolic efficiency derived cost functions. We model the human arm as a 6-muscle 2-joint system operating under non-linear optimal feedback control (Todorov & Li, 2005). The cost function is defined as a weighted sum of two biologically motivated costs: signal dependent motor noise and metabolic cost. These are evolutionarily sensible costs that maximise the fitness of an organism's movements, minimizing task-relevant variability and maximising energy efficiency. Simulated annealing optimisation of the cost function was used to identify an optimal fit with experimentally measured human behaviour. This allowed a quantitative description of the cost function implemented in the human motor system. We have analysed experimental data collected in two motor tasks: a dynamic centre-out reaching task and a static force production task. In the dynamic task, subjects performed planar reaching movements from the centre of the workspace to a target in one of 8 directions. In the static task, subjects were asked to produce a number of target forces as accurately as possible for 10 seconds. The force supplied by the subject was defined as the sum of forces acting upon two perpendicular force sensors: one at the wrist, and one just below the elbow. We use bootstrapping to estimate the distribution of the cost function parameters. This statistical technique shows that the observed changes in human behaviour are indeed the result of interpolating between signal dependent noise and metabolic cost. Greater understanding of the costs minimised by the human motor system will allow us to better describe the neuronal implementation of motor control strategies. This work was supported by BBSRC. p of 0.1 and 0.9. This created very different worst case loads - a 9-fold difference wrt the b=0 baseline (b = 3σ vs σ/3, as in our paradigm negative values of b actually required less GF than b=0). Thus worst case scenario GF control would predict changes from the baseline GF that were 9 times stronger for the first environment, however, variability-measure based control would predict equal GF responses. We found essentially equal responses for the two cases (2.33±0.38 vs. 2.36±0.33 Ns/m above baseline) indicating variability-measure based control. We next investigated the nature of the variability measure used by the motor system by comparing GFs across 4 environments with the same μ and σ but different high-order statistics: the two skewed bimodal ones, a Gaussian and a balanced bimodal (b of {-σ, σ}). Interestingly, we found that GFs were the smallest for the environments with the largest high-order moments of variability, and that a measure of variability based on a moment of order 1.3 best explained the GF data across groups; significantly better (p<0.02) than a statistic of order 2 (corresponding to σ) or any higher order statistic. Note that a measure of variability with order<2 can be viewed as a more robust estimator of variability than σ because of lower sensitivity to extremal conditions. Our results indicate that environmental variability determines GF control based on a robust measure of variability that discounts higher order statistical moments. 2-G-73 Tapping along with ADAM: Synchronizing with an adaptive and anticipatory virtual partner Maria C van der Steen1, Merle T Fairhurst1, Peter E Keller1 2-G-72 Mechanisms for variability estimation in the motor system 1 Maurice Smith1, Alkis Hadjiosif1 Interacting with a virtual partner (VP) is a relatively novel but promising approach to investigate different aspects of human coordination (Kelso et al., 2009; Repp & Keller, 2008). Interpersonal coordination in dynamic environments relies on sensorimotor synchronization (SMS). SMS is the temporal coordination of self-generated motor rhythms with external rhythmical events (Repp, 2005). Temporal adaptation and anticipation mechanisms seem to be involved with SMS (Keller, 2008). Adaptation mechanisms enable humans to modify the timing of their actions online when synchronizing with external event sequences. Temporal error correction processes, such as phase and period correction, influence the timing of upcoming movements and therefore facilitate the maintenance of synchrony (Repp, 2005). Anticipatory mechanisms concern predictions about the unfolding external event sequence with which the action is to be coordinated. It is suggested that these anticipatory processes are related to online action simulation and internal models (Keller, 2008). The current study explores the role of adaptation and anticipatory mechanisms in a SMS task between human participants and a VP. The VP is based on an ADaptation and Anticipation Model, ADAM. ADAM is created in Simulink® and combines an established formal model of adaptation (phase and period correction) (Repp & Keller 2008) with an anticipatory internal model. During a paced finger tapping task, healthy participants interacted with the VP. The VP produced an auditory pacing signal, and parametrically adjusted the timing of this signal based on the human participant's timing so as to reduce asynchronies (mismatch between VP tone and human's action). Participants were instructed to synchronize with the VP and to maintain a stable tempo. Task conditions included different combinations of adaptive and anticipatory processes, which were modeled within ADAM to simulate human-like 1 Harvard University In order to skillfully manipulate an object, for example a wine glass, one must not only compensate for its dynamics but also maintain a stable grasp avoiding slippage. The minimum grip force (GF) required to prevent slip is a function of both the glass's weight & the friction coefficient between its surface & our fingers. A safety margin above this minimal GF is often maintained, allowing us to maintain a grasp robust to unexpected perturbations, misestimation of load forces, or errors in GF production. Here we hypothesize that this safety margin is determined by the uncertainty about environmental dynamics: if the motor system is certain about its load force estimate it can afford to keep the safety margin low without risking slip, but a large safety margin would be beneficial if uncertainty is high. To test this hypothesis we designed a task where participants performed point-to-point reaching movements while grasping a small object with dynamics that changed randomly from one trial to the next. These dynamics were characterized by a viscous force-field of strength b, chosen from a Gaussian PDF with standard deviation (σ). We found that GFs increased when σ was higher (p<0.001); in fact GFs were twice as sensitive to σ as to the mean of b. This sensitivity of GFs to environmental variability could result from the motor system adjusting GFs under a worstcase scenario: trying to avoid slip against the highest loads in recent memory. Alternatively, GFs could be directly programmed based on a measure of environmental variability in combination with an estimate of the mean environment. To distinguish between these possibilities, we compared GF adaptation in two environments with different PDFs for b. These PDFs were oppositely skewed but shared the same μ & σ, and had equal values for the amplitudes of all higher order moments about the mean. In the first, b took on values of -σ/3 and 3σ with p of 0.9 and 0.1, whereas in the other b took on values of -3σ and σ/3 with 76 Max Planck Institute for Human Cognitive and Brain Sciences Poster Sessions Full Abstracts behavior. Different weightings of the two mechanisms within the model were tested to probe subsequent effects on participants' tapping behavior. Variables of interest include tapping accuracy (mean absolute asynchronies) and tapping variability (standard deviation of asynchronies). The relative importance of these two mechanisms will be discussed in terms of the variable effect on SMS. 2-G-74 Coordinate dependence and distribution dependence of blind source separation for motor synergies: Robust separation behavior of ICA across transforms Corey Hart1, Simon Giszter1 1 Drexel University College of Medicine There has been some recent controversy over the extent to which different variance-based and other statistical decomposition techniques can detect fundamental motor synergies or primitives. For example, are techniques applied to EMG activity capable of uniquely isolating functional related groups of muscles. Hogan and Sternad have argued that often these relationships are merely a consequence of an arbitrary choice of scale and coordinates for motor data. In a related argument, it is also possible to maintain that even information-based algorithms such as independent component analysis (ICA) can be made to fail by applying a highly nonlinear mapping to the data such that the resultant transformed data are very nearly normal. ICA will fail for the transformed Gaussian data, as ICA, by definition, cannot be applied to Gaussian distributed data sets. We set out to determine just how fragile ICA is and how much of a problem such re-mappings are for a typical implementation of ICA (i.e., Makeig-Sejnowski's infomax ICA algorithm). This study relied on artificial mixture data generated in the MATLAB ? programming environment. Two kinds of data were generated. Case 1: Data were generated from mixtures of ten source channels of 10000 points each, drawn from Gaussian pseudo-random data generated within MATLAB. Random mixing matrices were multiplied by the source channels, resulting in a 10x10000 matrix of mixtures. These mixtures were then either (a) given to the ICA algorithm for identification of the mixing matrix, or (b) scaled by an exponential function and then supplied to the ICA algorithm. Case 2: Data were generated from sources that were initially nonnormal (created by first taking Gaussian values and then applying the exponential function to them prior to mixing, yielding a 10x10000 source matrix with a more-or-less log-normal distribution in data of sources. Data were then mixed as above and given to the ICA algorithm for factoring (c). To examine the effect of "warping" the initially non-Gaussian Case 2 data back toward a normal distribution, then we used the fmincon() function in MATLAB to find an optimal polynomial function that minimized both kurtosis and skewness of the warped mixture data (resulting in a more or less Gaussian transform of the mixed data). In these simulations ICA unmixing was surprisingly much better than anticipated, even for very small deviations from true Gaussian in Case 1 'normal data'. Although ICA will in principle fail for truly Gaussian distributed data, almost any real world example will have small deviations from true normality. Furthermore, any invertible transform used in an attempt to rescale the mixed data in such a way as to make it normal (as in Case 2), only 'crushes' the extra information that ICA relies on into smaller and smaller bins in the analysis. This does not destroy it, and this information can still be successfully resolved with sufficient computing power and time, allowing identification and unmixing of the original information-based groups. We conclude ICA is significantly more robust and immune to the coordinate-dependence concerns voiced by Hogan and Sternad than many other techniques in use, and thus less subject to this critique. 2-G-75 Influence of arm velocity on haptic localization Femke Maij1, W. Pieter Medendorp2, Alan M Wing1 1 University of Birmingham, 2Donders Institue for Brain, Cognition and Behaviour, Radboud University Nijmegen Different sensory signals are integrated in our brain to construct a perceptual representation of the world. Here we study how the brain integrates tactile and proprioceptive signals by investigating systematic spatial errors that people make when localizing briefly presented tactile stimuli on their index finger while making arm movements. We hypothesize that spatial localization errors originate from uncertainty about the time of the tactile stimulus with respect to the movement. Subjects moved their extended right arm horizontally through the air. At different times before, during and after the movement they felt a tactile stimulus. After the arm movement subjects indicated where they had perceived the tactile stimulus in space. Varying the velocity of the arm movements resulted in different patterns of localization errors. We explain our results with the hypothesized temporal uncertainty model. 2-G-76 Co-articulate of straight movements with an artificial neural network Andre Lemme1, Yaron Meirovitch2, Tamar Flash2, Jochen Steil1 1 Research Institute for Cognition and Robotics, 2Weizmann Institute of Science In our daily lives, we perform tasks that require complex movements such as drawing or handwriting. It has been suggested that complex movements are composed of simpler movement primitives, which represent basic movement units or simple strokes, characterized by (e.g.,in the case of reaching movements) bell-shaped-velocity profiles[1].The notion of co-articulation suggests the possibility that with extensive training a new movement primitive can emerge [2]. The process may start with simple straight movements which are eventually substituted with more complex curved primitives. The contribution of the present work is in proposing a flexible learning setup that is able to generate biologically plausible movements based on co-articulation of simple movement primitives. Methods: The used learning module is a high-dimensional, single-layer feed-forward network. The weights are initialized randomly and only the output weights are obtained by linear regression, called Extreme Learning Machine (ELM) [3].The ELM represents the movement primitive as a mapping from position to velocity. To generate a movement the velocity is integrated over time to reproduce the learned basic (curved)strokes. We qualitatively compare the learned new primitives to velocity profiles from human data and to the predictions of the Minimum Jerk Model[4], which plans a trajectory starting from a given starting point to a given end-point through a via-point. The constraint for planning the trajectory is to be as smooth as possible (minimum jerk). This module was also used in[2] and showed to provide a good approximation of the movements emerging from human co-articulation, but no learning algorithm was proposed in that study. Learning setup: The algorithm starts from a set of straight movements each having a bell shaped velocity profile and form a new curved movement primitive. The task setup has three targets (ABC), which need to be connected. The algorithm repeatedly connects the three points with straight strokes.The learning paradigm requires a certain amount of 77 Poster Sessions Full Abstracts variance in the data. Therefore Gaussian noise was added to the position of target B. These recorded trajectories were subsequently used as training data for the ELM. Further more we implement an iterative learning process, in which the learning module creates its own training data to further evolve the primitive. During these iterations we follow the trajectories as the learning progresses and compare this progress to the one observed in human subjects. The result of our work is a learning mechanism that simulates the co-articulation process observed in human subjects with quantitatively similar velocity profiles. Acknowledgment: Many thanks to Ronen Sosnik who provided the human data to this work. This work was supported by the European Communitys FP7 under grant agreement No 248311 AMARSi Ref:[1]W.Abend, E.Bizzi,and P.Morasso. Human arm trajectory formation. Brain: a journal of neurology, June 1982. [2]R.Sosnik, T.Flash, B.Hauptmann, and A.Karni.The acquisition and implementation of the smoothness maximization motion strategy is dependent on spatial accuracy demands. Experimental Brain Research, 2007. [3]Guang-Bin Huang and Chee-Kheong Siew. Extreme learning machine:Theory and applications. Neurocomputing,2006.[4]T.Flash, E.Henis, R.Inzelberg, and A.D.Korczyn. Timing and sequencing of human arm trajectories:Normal and abnormal motor behaviour. Human Movement Science,1992 2-G-77 Task-dependent structure of neuronal variability during abstract BMI control Kianoush Nazarpour1, Tom M Hall1, Andrew Jackson1 1 Newcastle University Primates can volitionally modulate neuronal activity according to arbitrary reward rules and learn to control Brain-Machine Interfaces (BMIs) with randomized decoders. We have previously shown that in redundant myoelectric-control tasks, subjects improve accuracy by buffering trial-to-trial variability into taskirrelevant dimensions of the muscle space. However it is not known whether variability in the neural space can also be shaped in a task-dependent manner during control of a redundant BMI. We therefore investigated the structure of neural noise covariation in two macaque monkeys performing a simple brain control task involving two neurons, C1 and C2. Over separate sessions, 81 pairs of neurons were sampled from chronically-implanted electrode arrays in M1 and PMv. The one dimensional position of a cursor was determined by the instantaneous firing rate of C1 alone or the sum (C1 C2) or difference (C1-C2) of the firing rates. The monkeys' task was to hold the cursor for 500ms in targets that appeared in one of four different locations. We calculated trial-to-trial variability of the firing rate trajectory in neural space. Trajectories for each trial were aligned to the first time that cursor entered the target. To remove any structure that this alignment imposed on the trajectory distribution, we characterised the evolution of neural variability relative to the point in neural space at which the cursor entered target. Variability was calculated along dimensions of positive (C1 C2) and negative (C1-C2) covariation and defined an index of covariation (IoC) to quantify correlation structure. A zero IoC indicated a uniform pattern of variability while the limiting values of 1/-1 imply the variability is entirely constrained to axes of positive/negative covariation in the neural space. IoC was evaluated up to 500ms after the alignment point. We found an effect of task condition on neural IoC. Overall, the distribution of IoC tended towards positive values, consistent with a general pattern of positive noise correlation between neurons. During the C1-C2 control block, IoC was increased further 78 suggesting greatest neuronal variability in the dimension of positive correlation (the task-irrelevant dimension). In contrast, IoC was, on average, slightly negative in the C1 C2 control condition. IoC during control by C1 alone fell between that of C1-C2 and C1 C2 conditions. This modulation of IoC suggests that trial-to-trial variability was to some extent shaped to reflect the task-relevant and irrelevant dimensions of the neural space. It remains to be seen whether the task-dependent structure in neuronal variability emerges from divergence in feedforward inputs or feed-back correction of errors. In either case, our results suggest that redundancy in abstract BMIs can be exploited to improve accuracy of control by buffering variability into task-irrelevant dimensions of the neural space. 2-G-78 A common motor optimisation principle in healthy subjects and parkinsonian patients Pierre Baraduc1, Stéphane Thobois2, Jing Gan2, Emmanuel Broussolle2, Michel Desmurget1 1 CNRS / U. Lyon, 2Lyon Pierre Wertheimer Neurological Hospital Recent research on Parkinson's Disease (PD) has emphasized that parkinsonian movement, although bradykinetic, shares many attributes with healthy behavior. This observation has led to the suggestion that bradykinesia in Parkinson's Disease could be mostly due to a reduction in motor motivation. This hypothesis can be investigated in the framework of the optimal control theory which models account for many characteristics of healthy movement, while providing a link between the motor behavior and a cost/benefit tradeoff. Here we studied 14 PD patients with bilateral subthalamic nucleus (STN) stimulation and 16 age-matched healthy controls and asked whether reaching movements are subserved by similar execution rules in these two groups, but with a different allocation of effort. We show that a single optimal control model accounts for the reaching movements of healthy subjects and PD patients, whatever the condition of STN stimulation (On or Off). The choice of movement speed was explained in all subjects by the existence of a preset dynamic range for the motor signals. This range was idiosyncratic and applied to all movements irrespective of their amplitude. In PD patients this dynamic range was abnormally narrow and correlated with bradykinesia. STN deep brain stimulation widened this range in all patients, but did not restore it to a normal value. These results, consistent with the motor motivation hypothesis, suggest that optimization of motor effort is the main determinant of movement production in both healthy and PD subjects. 2-G-79 Arm-EMG control for assistive lower limb exoskeleton Francesca Sylos Labini1, V La Scaleia1, M J MacLellan1, T Hoellinger2, K Seetharaman2, M Petieau2, A Bengoetxea2, G Cheron2, Y P Ivanenko1 1 Santa Lucia Foundation, 2Université libre de Bruxelles Success in locomotor rehabilitation programs can be improved with the use of brain-computer interfaces. Although a wealth of research has demonstrated that locomotion is largely controlled by spinal mechanisms, the brain is of utmost importance in monitoring or shaping locomotor patterns in humans. In addition, there is also a Poster Sessions Full Abstracts tight coordination between the upper and lower limbs, which can also be useful in controlling locomotion. The current study critically investigates different approaches for using upper limb electromyogram (EMG) that are applicable to this field of controlling assistive lower limb exoskeletons. The control may be based on the spatiotemporal aspects of arm muscle activity or using dynamic recurrent neural networks (DRNNs). Taking into account the high involvement of some shoulder muscles (e.g., deltoid) in most locomotor-related movements in humans we took advantage of this natural coordination for the control of leg kinematics. It is important to emphasize that these muscles are naturally involved in locomotion, in contrast to distal (wrist or finger) muscles that are more cortically controlled during locomotion. Once properly parameterized, DRNN may acquire independent knowledge patterns (attractor states) representing EMG-kinematics identification. The expected final output corresponds to the kinematics-related signals of the intent movement that will be exploited as an input to the exoskeleton controller combined with virtual reality training of individuals with reduced mobility, such as spinal cord injury patients. We investigated different experimental conditions for the DRNN procedure in healthy subjects when using this approach. In conditions of prominent arm muscle activity (such as during fast walking speeds or enhanced arm swinging) a high correspondence between original and predicted leg kinematics was observed. The current work is aimed at integration of DRNN with virtual reality locomotor training and exoskeleton supervisor controller. The aim of the clinical validation for the arm-EMG control approaches would be to evaluate effectiveness, usability and comfort of this control for patients. Supported by EU FP7-ICT program (MINDWALKER grant). These new methods make it possible to construct optimal control models for more complex behaviors than what is currently possible. How well such models will agree with experimental data remains to be seen, and indeed there are many different ways to construct them which will yield different predictions. The work presented here aims to enable the construction of such models. This work is a summary of two recent technical papers whose PDFs can be found on our website: Synthesis and stabilization of complex behaviors through online trajectory optimization Tassa Y, Erez T and Todorov E (2012), under review Discovery of complex behaviors through contact-invariant optimization Mordatch, Popovic and Todorov (2012), to appear in ACM SIGGRAPH 2-G-80 Automatic synthesis of complex behaviors with optimal control Emo Todorov1, Igor Mordatch1, Yuval Tassa1, Tom Erez1, Zoran Popovic1 1 Computer Science and Engineering, University of Washington In this video poster, we will show animations of complex motor behaviors synthesized automatically using new optimal control methods, as well as explain how these methods work. The behaviors include getting up an arbitrary pose on the ground, walking, hopping, swimming, kicking, climbing, hand-stands, and cooperative actions. The synthesis methods fall in two categories. The first is online trajectory optimization or model-predictive control (MPC). The idea is to re-optimize the movement trajectory at every step of the estimation-control loop, up to some time horizon (in our case about half a second), execute only the beginning portion of the trajectory, and repeat the re-optimization at the next time step (say 10 msec later). This approach has been used extensively in domains such as chemical process control where the dynamics are sufficiently slow and smooth to make online optimization possible. We have now developed a number of algorithmic improvements, making it possible to apply MPC to biomechanical systems. The second method is based on the realization that most movements performed on land are made for the purpose of establishing contact with the environment, and exerting contact force. This suggests that contact events should not be treated as side-effects of multi-joint kinematics and dynamics, but rather as decision variables that the controller can reason about directly. We have developed a method in which the optimizer can explicitly specify the desired contact events, using continuous decision variables that facilitate optimization, and at the same time optimize the movement trajectory in a way consistent with the specified contact events. This makes it possible to optimize movement trajectories with a large sequence of contact events which are discovered automatically. 79 Poster Session Floor Plans Venetian Ballroom A & B & Foyer Food Foyer 9 10 11 12 13 14 15 16 17 18 19 20 21 22 44 43 42 41 40 39 38 37 36 35 34 33 32 31 30 29 28 27 26 25 24 23 1 Entrance 89 105 90 91 103 92 102 93 Exhibit Tables 104 3 4 5 6 7 8 Food 106 2 Venetian Ballroom A&B To Foyer 45 46 47 48 49 50 51 52 53 54 55 56 57 58 59 60 61 62 63 64 65 66 88 87 86 85 84 83 82 81 80 79 78 77 76 75 74 73 72 71 70 69 68 67 Coffee Food Venetian Ballroom C, D & E THEMES 94 Poster Layout 100 95 Session 1 99 96 98 97 101 All meeting sessions will be held here. A - Adaptation & Plasticity in Motor Control B - Integrative Control of Movement C - Control of Eye & Head Movement D - Disorders of Motor Control E - Posture & Gait F - Fundamentals of Motor Control G - Theoretical & Computational Motor Control Tuesday, April 24 & Wednesday, April 25 Venetian Ballroom A & B and Foyer Foyer Food Foyer 9 10 11 12 13 14 15 16 17 18 19 20 40 39 38 37 36 35 34 33 32 31 30 29 28 27 26 25 24 23 22 21 1 Entrance 2 3 4 5 6 7 Food Venetian Ballroom A&B To Foyer 41 42 43 44 45 46 47 48 49 50 51 52 53 54 55 56 57 58 59 60 80 79 78 77 76 75 74 73 72 71 70 69 68 67 66 65 64 63 62 61 81 92 91 82 90 83 Exhibit Tables 8 Coffee Food Venetian Ballroom C, D & E 84 Poster Layout 88 85 Session 2 87 86 89 Friday, April 27 & Saturday, April 28 Venetian Ballroom A & B and Foyer Foyer All meeting sessions will be held here. THEMES A - Adaptation & Plasticity in Motor Control B - Integrative Control of Movement D - Disorders of Motor Control F - Fundamentals of Motor Control G - Theoretical & Computational Motor Control The 22nd Annual NCM Meeting thanks our sponsors, supporters and donors
© Copyright 2025