Challenges for Alexithymia Research: A Commentary on ‘‘The Construct of Alexithymia: Associations With Defense Mechanisms’’ m Stijn Vanheule Ghent University The author comments on an article by Helmes and colleagues (this issue, pp. 318–331) that studied defense mechanisms related to alexithymia. What these authors observed is theoretically meaningful, and a distinct strength of this study is that the results are replicated in three independent samples. The study is also exemplary for two typical drawbacks— only student samples and self-report measures are used. The author suggests that when studying alexithymia, researchers use samples that contain a substantial number of alexithymic subjects. Furthermore, variables such as emotional processing and regulation should not be measured with self-report questionnaires alone. Emotional processing and regulation are, in part, implicit and researchers should consider this. The author argues that the standard of alexithymia research could be raised by including interdisciplinary perspectives and experimental methods, and by embedding research more explicitly in theories. & 2008 Wiley Periodicals, Inc. J Clin Psychol 64: 332--337, 2008. Keywords: alexithymia; defense mechanisms; assessment; sampling; metacognition; psychoanalysis Introduction Four decades of clinical reflection and systematized research on alexithymia has shown the relevance of this construct for clinical psychology. Alexithymia, which can be defined as a difficulty in processing and regulating affective arousal by means of mental representations, proves to be prominent in a number of clinical disorders (e.g., somatoform disorders, panic disorders, depression with dominance of vital and somatic symptoms, posttraumatic stress disorder [PTSD], and eating disorders). Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to: Stijn Vanheule, Ghent University, Faculty of Psychology and Educational Sciences, Department of Psychoanalysis and Clinical Consulting, H. Dunantlaan 2, B-9000 Ghent, Belgium; e-mail: Stijn.Vanheule@UGent.be JOURNAL OF CLINICAL PSYCHOLOGY, Vol. 64(3), 332–337 (2008) & 2008 Wiley Periodicals, Inc. Published online in Wiley InterScience (www.interscience.wiley.com). DOI: 10.1002/jclp.20467 Challenges for Alexithymia Research 333 It has not only been demonstrated that a substantial number of these patients have pronounced alexithymia, but that alexithymia is significantly related to outcome variables of these disorders (Frewen, Pain, Dozois, & Lanius, 2006; Hund & Espelage, 2005; Mueller & Buehner, 2006; Speranza, Laos, Wallier, & Corcos, 2007; Taylor, Bagby, & Parker, 1997; Vanheule, Desmet, Verhaeghe, & Bogaerts, 2007; Verhaeghe, Vanheule, & De Rick, 2007). Diverse variables, such as style of interpersonal relating, concomitant psychological processes (e.g., dissociation), course of illness, and therapy outcome have been linked to alexithymia (Grabe et al., 2000; Porcellia & Todarello, 2007; Speranza et al., 2007; Vanheule, Desmet, Rosseel, Verhaeghe, & Meganck, 2007). The article by Helmes and colleagues (this issue, pp. 318–331) on the defense mechanisms related to alexithymia adds to this field. The authors demonstrate that alexithymia is systematically related to increased emotional inhibition, immature defense styles, and a reduced self-deception. This finding is important as it indicates that alexithymia is associated with broader subjective strategies of dealing with personally sensitive and conflicting information. The mechanism of emotional inhibition designates the tendency towards blocking off the possibility of being affected and aroused by events; the immature defense styles point out annulling tendencies that come to the fore upon dealing with sensitive and conflicting information. In my interpretation, the correlation with reduced self-deception indicates that the type of defense at work in alexithymia is different from neurotic defense, whereby self-deception is an essential element. Very early in his work, Freud (1894/1962) described neurotic defense as the consequence of experiencing ideas or intentions that are at odds with one’s ego. Examples of such antithetical ideas can be found in what people experience as taboos, like elements in the sexual or aggressive sphere. According to Freud, defense against an antithetical idea consists ‘‘of ‘pushing the thing away’, of not thinking of it, of suppressing it’’ (Freud, 1894/1962, p. 47). A result of this defense is that the antithetical idea is marginalized in mental life and fades into the background. The self-deceiving tendency at work here consists of the ego’s acting as if the antithetical idea was not there. Immature defenses, on the other hand, rather than marginalizing mental contents, are more rudimentary than neurotic defenses in that they aim at altogether erasing or externalizing these contents from mental life, e.g., by denying them or by projecting them to others (Freud, 1936). The radical nature of this operation implies that the ego no longer needs to act in a self-deceptive way because the effect of this type of defense is that the antithetical idea is no longer experienced as part of oneself. To conclude, the pattern of associations Helmes and colleagues found is theoretically sound and in my experience, corresponds to what clinicians can observe in their work with alexithymic patients. A distinct strength of the study by Helmes and colleagues is that their results are replicated in three samples, from two different countries. The replications, of which the value cannot be overestimated (Rosenthal, 1995), indicate that what they have found is solid, and not attributable to coincidental factors. Findings of this type illustrate how alexithymia is not an isolated trait in someone’s functioning, but embedded in broader patterns of functioning. This is very important as it helps researchers and clinicians gain empirically supported insight into the complexity of patients’ problems, which advances the science of clinical psychology (Westen & Bradley, 2005). Nevertheless, the study of Helmes and colleagues has two drawbacks that characterize many studies on alexithymia: the exclusive use of student samples and the exclusive use of self-report measures. Journal of Clinical Psychology DOI: 10.1002/jclp 334 Journal of Clinical Psychology, March 2008 The Exclusive Use of Student Samples There is a clear tendency among psychological researchers to recruit the subjects for their studies in the group that is most easily accessible to them, i.e., psychology students, who frequently get credits for participating in research. However, when studying clinical psychological topics it is of utmost importance that the characteristics one is studying are markedly present in a substantial percentage of the sample. Studying alexithymia or immature defenses solely in a student population is problematic: Occasionally alexithymic subjects will be in the sample (in our experience hardly 5%), but the size of this group tends to be too small to make conclusions that are valid to explain the problem of alexithymic subjects in clinical settings. Helmes and colleagues do not report how many of their subjects actually suffer from severe or moderate alexithymia, but the means and standard deviations they present brings us to presume that few alexithymic subjects were involved in this study. It is advisable that when using student samples to study alexithymia, researchers should also use samples in which a substantial number of participants do suffer from the problem. Moreover, studying alexithymia in student samples with the aim of making inferences that are relevant for clinical groups presupposes that the measurements used are invariant across those groups. Such a presumption that different groups make identical interpretations of questionnaire items is something that should be tested. To our knowledge only one study examined measurement invariance between a clinical and a nonclinical group for the TAS-20 items (Meganck, Vanheule, & Desmet, in press). In this study (among Dutch-speaking subjects) partial invariance between these groups was observed (different interpretation of item 19, which refers to personal problems). The absence of further research on the TAS-20, or other alexithymia questionnaires, urges us to be careful with making generalizations between clinical and nonclinical groups. The Exclusive Use of Self-Report Measures The other drawback in the study of Helmes and colleagues is their exclusive use of self-report measures. There are at least two reasons to object to this practice. A first one is purely methodological: The sole use of self-report measures leaves us with the question as to whether associations found between two variables have been influenced by the use of a single measurement method. In other words, we cannot examine or rule out whether results have been artificially inflated by this method. Using multiple measures, on the contrary, enables us to control for this. Consequently, for measuring alexithymia the TAS-20 self-report questionnaire can best be combined with instruments like the Toronto Structured Interview for Alexithymia (TSIA; Bagby, Taylor, Parker, & Dickens, 2006), the modified version of the Beth Israel Hospital Psychosomatic Questionnaire (M-BIQ; Taylor, Bagby, & Parker, 1997), or the Metagcognition Assessment Scale (MAS; Semerari et al., 2003). These are instruments that researchers and clinicians score based on interview data. A second reason for not exclusively using self-report measures in the study of alexithymia or defense mechanisms is more fundamental. There is a growing consensus that the way in which people represent and regulate emotions is, in part, implicit and not accessible to self-knowledge (e.g., Westen & Blagov, 2007). In this respect, self-report questionnaires, which make an exclusive appeal to explicit self-knowledge, miss a crucial component of the way in which people actually process and regulate affective states. Consequently, researchers that exclusively rely Journal of Clinical Psychology DOI: 10.1002/jclp Challenges for Alexithymia Research 335 on self-report questionnaires for studying emotional representations and strategies of affect regulation tend to distort the accuracy of our knowledge on those topics. For studying defense mechanisms, self-report questionnaires, such as the Defence Styles Questionnaire (Andrews, Singh, & Bond, 1993), could be combined with instruments like the Q-sort Vaillant, developed for measuring defenses (Vaillant & McCullough, 1998). The Q-sort and the clinician-scored questionnaire developed by Westen and colleagues for studying affect regulation could also be used (Westen, Muderrisoglu, Fowler, Shedler, & Koren, 1997; Zittel Conklin, Bradley, & Westen, 2006). Regretfully, however, the literature shows that these combinations are rarely designed. For a problem like alexithymia, the absence of such research designs is all the more surprising because a number of authors note the paradoxical issue of using self-report instruments to measure alexithymia: These self-report instruments ask people to report a capacity that they, in fact, are supposed to lack, i.e., the mental processing of affective states (Lane, Ahern, Schwartz, & Kaszniak, 1997; Waller & Scheidt, 2004). Without simultaneously using instruments like TSIA, M-BIQ, or MAS, it is questionable as to whether a study can actually provide a valid assessment. This remark was also made by Taylor and colleagues (1997), the authors of the TAS-20. They emphasize that researchers should make use of different methods to measure alexithymia. Nonetheless, doing so is not a common practice. Conclusion To raise the standard of alexithymia research and make this research more directly relevant for clinical psychological practice, a number of changes are needed. In line with the study of Helmes and colleagues, replication in independent samples is recommended; researchers should also include clinical samples, or at least a substantial number of subjects with elevated alexithymia scores in their studies; and for the measurement of variables associated with emotional processing and regulation, researchers should not rely on self-report measures alone. I agree with Taylor and Bagby (2004) that the quality of research in the field of alexithymia could be raised by including more interdisciplinary perspectives and experimental methods. In this context, a number of experimental psychological studies have already shown that alexithymic subjects process emotional information differently than nonalexithymic subjects (Luminet, Vermeulen, Demaret, Taylor, & Bagby, 2006; Mueller, Alpers, & Reim, 2006; Vermeulen, Luminet, & Corneille, 2006); functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) studies have detected patterns of neural activation concomitant of alexithymia (Aleman, 2005; Frewen et al., 2006; Moriguchi et al., 2007); and psychophysiological studies into the mental processing of arousal and into correlates of alexithymia are gaining attention (Guilbaud, Corcos, Hjalmarsson, Loas, & Jeammet, 2003). Furthermore, alexithymia researchers should pay more attention to embedding research within theoretical frameworks, and to the elaboration of theories that can guide clinical psychological and psychotherapeutic practice with alexithymic patients. Theory helps us to integrate empirical observations and to grasp the complexity of disorders; it enables us to formulate clear hypotheses, and assists us in making inferences from research to clinical practice. Metacognition theory (Dimaggio, Carcionne, & Nicolo`, in press; Semerari, Carcione, Dimaggio, Nicolo`, & Procacci, 2007) and psychoanalytic theory with respect to attachment and actualpathology (Lemche, Klann-Delius, Koch, & Joranschky, 2004; Verhaeghe, 2004; Verhaeghe et al., 2007) are theories I find very promising in this respect. Journal of Clinical Psychology DOI: 10.1002/jclp 336 Journal of Clinical Psychology, March 2008 References Aleman, A. (2005). Feelings you can’t imagine: Towards a cognitive neuroscience of alexithymia. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 9, 553–555. Andrews, G., Singh, M., & Bond, M. (1993). The Defence Style Questionnaire. Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease, 181, 246–256. Bagby, R. M., Taylor, G. J., Parker, J. D. A., & Dickens, S. E. (2006). The development of the Toronto Structured Interview for Alexithymia: Item selection, factor structure, reliability and concurrent validity. Psychotherapy and Psychosomatics, 75, 25–39. Dimaggio, G., Carcione, A., & Nicolo`, G. (in press). Metacognition disorders: Research and therapeutic implications. Psychotherapy Bulletin. Elzinga, B. M., Bermond, B., & Van Dyck, R. (2002). The relationship between dissociative proneness and alexithymia. Psychotherapy and Psychosomatics, 71, 104–111. Freud, A. (1936). The ego and the mechanisms of defence. New York: International Universities Press. Freud, S. (1962). The neuro-psychoses of defence. In J. Strachy, (Ed. & Trans.), The standard edition of the complete psychological works of Sigmund Freud (Vol. 3, pp. 41–61). London: Hogarth Press. (Original work published 1894) Frewen, P.A., Pain, C., Dozois, D.J.A., & Lanius, R.A. (2006). Alexithymia in PTSD: Psychometric and FMRI studies. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 1071, 397–400. Grabe, H.J., Rainermann, S., Spitzer, C., Graniscke, M., Freyberger, H.J. (2000). The relationship between dimensions of alexithymia and dissociation. Psychotherapy and Psychosomatics, 69, 128–131. Guilbaud, O., Corcos, M., Hjalmarsson, L., Loas, G., & Jeammet, P. (2003). Is there a psychoneuroimmunological pathway between alexithymia and immunity? Immune and physiological correlates of alexithymia. Biomedicine and Pharmacotherapy, 57, 292–295. Helmes, E., McNeill, P.D., Holden, R.R., & Jackson, C. (2008). The construct of alexithymia: Associations with defense mechanisms. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 64, 318–331. Hund, A.R., & Espelage, D.L. (2005). Childhood sexual abuse, disordered eating, alexithymia and general distress: A mediation model. Journal of Counseling Psychology, 52, 559–573. Lane, R.D., Ahern, G.L., Schwartz, G.E., & Kaszniak, A.W. (1997). Is alexithymia the emotional equivalent of blind sight? Biological Psychiatry, 42, 834–844. Lemche, E., Klann-Delius, G., Koch, R., & Joranschky, P. (2004). Mentalizing language development in a longitudinal attachment sample: implications for alexithymia. Psychotherapy and Psychosomatics, 73, 366–374. Luminet, O., Vermeulenm, N., Demaret, C., Taylor, G.J., & Bagby, R.M. (2006). Alexithymia and levels of processing: Evidence for an overall deficit in remembering emotion words. Journal of Research in Personality, 40, 713–733. Meganck, R., Vanheule, S., & Desmet, M. (in press). Factorial validity and measurement invariance of the 20-items Toronto Alexithymia Scale in Clinical and nonclinical samples. Assessment. Mueller, J., Alpers, G. W., & Reim, N. (2006). Dissociation of rated emotional valence and Stroop interference in observer-rated alexithymia. Journal of Psychosomatic Research, 61, 261–269. Mueller, J., & Buehner M. (2006). Two facets of non-expression of emotions: Relationships between dimensions of alexithymia and the repressive coping style. Personality and Individual Differences, 41, 1337–1347. Moriguchi, Y., Decety, J., Ohnishi, T., Maeda, M., Mori, T., Nemoto, K., et al. (2007). Empathy and judging other’s pain: An fMRI study of Alexithymia. Cerebral Cortex, 17, 2223–2234. Porcellia P., & Todarello, O. (2007). Psychological factors affecting functional gastrointestional disorders. Psychological factors affecting medical conditions, 28, 34–56. Journal of Clinical Psychology DOI: 10.1002/jclp Challenges for Alexithymia Research 337 Rosenthal, R. (1995). Progress in clinical psychology—is there any? Clinical Psychology: Science and Practice, 2, 133–150. Semerari, A., Carcione, A., Dimaggio, G., Faclone, M., Nicolo`, G., Procacci, M., et al. (2003). The evaluation of metacognitive function in psychotherapy: The Metacognitive Assessment Scale and its applications. Clinical Psychology and Psychotherapy, 10, 238–261. Semerari, A., Carcione, A., Dimaggio, G., Nicolo`, G., & Procacci, M. (2007). Understanding minds: Different functions and different disorders? The contribution of psychotherapy research. Psychotherapy Research, 17, 106–119. Speranza, M., Laos, G., Wallier, J., & Corcos, M. (2007). Predictive value of alexithymia in patients with eating disorders: A 3-year prospective study. Journal of Psychosomatic Research, 63, 365–370. Taylor, G. J., & Bagby, R. M. (2004). New trends in alexithymia research. Psychotherapy and Psychosomatics, 73, 68–77. Taylor, G.J., Bagby, R.M., & Parker, J.D.A. (1997). Disorders of affect regulation. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press. Vaillant, G., & McCoullough, L. (1998). The role of ego mechanisms of defense in the diagnosis of personality disorders. In J. W. Barron, (Ed.), Making diagnosis meaningful: Enhancing evaluation and treatment of psychological disorders (pp. 139–158). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association. Vanheule, S., Desmet, M., Rosseel, Y., Verhaeghe, P., & Meganck, R. (2007). Relationship patterns in alexithymia: A study using the core conflictual relationship theme (CCRT) method. Psychopathology, 40, 14–21. Vanheule, S., Desmet, M., Verhaeghe, P., & Bogaerts, S. (2007). Alexithymic depression: Evidence for a depression subtype? Psychotherapy and Psychosomatics, 76, 135–136. Verhaeghe P. (2004). On being normal and other disorders. New York: Other Press. Verhaeghe, P., Vanheule, S., & De Rick, A. (2007). Actual neurosis as the underlying psychic structure of panic disorder, somatization, and somatoform disorder: An integration of Freudian and attachment perspectives. The Psychoanalytic Quarterly, 76, 1317–1350. Vermeulen, N., Luminet, O., & Corneille, O. (2006). Alexithymia and the automatic processing of affective information: Evidence from the affective priming paradigm. Cognition and Emotion, 20, 64–91. Waller, E., & Scheidt, C. E. (2004). Somatoform disorders as disorders of affect regulation. A study comparing the TAS-20 with non-self-report measures of alexithymia. Journal of Psychosomatic Research, 57, 239–247. Westen, D., & Blagov, P. S. (2007). A clinical-empirical model of emotion regulation. In J. Gross, (Ed.), Handbook of emotion regulation (pp. 373–393). New York: Guilford Press. Westen, D., & Bradley, R. (2005). Empirically supported complexity—Rethinking evidencebased practice in psychotherapy. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 14, 266–271. Westen, D., Muderrisoglu, S., Fowler, C., Shedler, J., & Koren, D. (1997). Affect regulation and affective experience: Individual differences, group differences, and measurement using a Q-sort procedure. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 65, 429–439. Zittel Conklin, C., Bradley, R., & Westen, D. (2006). Affect regulation in borderline personality disorder. Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease, 194, 69–77. Journal of Clinical Psychology DOI: 10.1002/jclp
© Copyright 2024