The Heuristic Value of Opinions in the Market for News Marcel Garz Hamburg Media School and University of Hamburg April 28, 2015 Address for correspondence: Marcel Garz, Hamburg Media School, Finkenau 35, 22081 Hamburg, Germany. E-mail: m.garz@hamburgmediaschool.com. I thank Christian Kolmer, Sven Meyer, Armin Rott, Johannes Schneller, and Matthias Vollbracht for their support in obtaining and compiling the data, as well as seminar participants in Hamburg and Esbjerg for helpful comment 1 Abstract This study investigates the variation in the supply of and demand for opinionated news. Due to different predispositions regarding systematic and heuristic information processing, news consumers have varying preferences over opinionated and objective reports. To verify the theoretical argument, I combine content data on the news output of leading German media and survey information from news consumers. The findings indicate consumption patterns that largely correspond with the information-processing predispositions. I conclude that the demand side of the news market determines the amount (more or less) and kind (one-sided or ambivalent) of opinionation. Keywords: news; opinionation; information processing; media bias JEL classification: D11; D12; D80; L82 2 1. Introduction Opinions are an essential part of the news. Often, viewpoints are not restricted to editorials or columns, but can be found in purportedly objective forms of journalism too. Among many other explanations, opinionated reporting is a consequence of the complexity of world events. Journalistic work allows nonexperts to comprehend intricate issues, for example in politics and economics. Giving opinion – that is, exercising judgment or quoting the views of others (Benson & Hallin, 2007; Feldman, 2011a, 2011b) – is one journalistic method of simplification. An external point of view serves as a cognitive shortcut if it relieves the recipients from gathering and analyzing the underlying information to form an own opinion. If certain conditions are met, judgments in the news thus have a heuristic value for consumers. Despite its role in audience persuasion and polarization (e.g., DellaVigna & Gentzkow, 2010; Prior, 2013; Boukes et al., 2014), little is known about the use of opinion as a tool of simplification. Why do media outlets vary in their provision of opinionated versus objective news? Do recipients have different preferences regarding the consumption of opinionated news, and if so, why? One answer to these questions lies in the heuristic value of opinions. Due to biological, evolutionary, and socio-cultural factors, for instance, some people are predisposed to make more use of cognitive shortcuts than others. For example, gender differences in information processing are well researched in the psychology and consumer literature (for a recent review, see Meyers-Levy & Loken, 2015): Women are known as comprehensive processors, whereas men tend to be heuristic ones. As a consequence, male recipients can be expected to have a greater demand for opinionated reports than female news consumers. Based on information-processing theory, I derive further hypotheses about preferences over opinionated and objective news. In particular, differences are hypothesized with 3 respect to the individual’s sex and gender, age, cognitive abilities, religion, and socialization. The empirical part of this study focuses on the German market for national print news in the time from October 2001 to April 2007. Due to its corporatist journalistic tradition, Germany is an ideal candidate for this evaluation: Tight connections between the media, political parties, and interest groups favor partisan and opinionated reporting (Esser & Umbricht, 2013). To verify the abovementioned hypotheses, I combine survey data from 70,292 news consumers with information obtained from analyzing 119,838 political and economic reports of the leading German print media. On this basis, I construct an indicator that measures the respondents’ personal level of consumption of opinionated reports. This indicator is then regressed on various explanatory variables that capture individual information-processing predispositions. The results indicate consumption patterns that largely correspond with the information-processing predispositions: Characteristics that are associated with systematic information processing increase the consumption of objective or balanced news, whereas predispositions towards heuristic strategies favor the consumption of news with one-sided opinions. In large parts, this relationship can be interpreted causally, because people’s predispositions are based on characteristics that are usually fixed (sex, gender, location of socialization), or only change seldom or gradually (religion, cognitive abilities). This study is closely related to the literature on demand-driven news coverage and media bias (Mullainathan & Shleifer, 2005; Gentzkow & Shapiro, 2006, 2010; Chan & Suen, 2008). According to this literature, profit-maximizing media companies slant their news coverage towards the beliefs of the recipients. Media bias arises because there is a demand for the confirmation of views. This study provides evidence that people’s information-processing predispositions are another driver of opinionation and therefore media bias. 4 The next section provides the theoretical background and the hypotheses. Afterwards, I describe the data and the empirical strategy. The estimation results are discussed before the last section concludes. 2. Theory 2.1 Dual-Process Models In social and cognitive psychology, dual-process models distinguish two modes of human information processing: the systematic and the heuristic one (e.g., Chaiken, 1980; Chaiken & Trope, 1999). The former mode is characterized by analytic thinking and effortful processing of all available information. In contrast, the heuristic mode is based on limited information and the use of cues, rules of thumb, and schemata that depend on past observations and experiences. Cognitive shortcuts allow people to form opinions with little knowledge and effort. In general, individuals tend to employ a heuristic approach when information costs are high, and a systematic one when reliability is important. Both modes are ideal constructs, because in reality there is a continuum of processing strategies. 2.2 Opinions as Cognitive Shortcuts The heuristic value of certain kinds of information has been widely acknowledged, for instance, in political decision making. Prominent examples are a candidate’s party affiliation, ideology, and appearance, as well as endorsements and poll results (Lau & Redlawsk, 2001). By investigating headlines as cognitive shortcuts, Andrew (2007) highlights the role of news media in providing cues. Opinions in news coverage can serve as cognitive shortcuts too. Although professional journalism usually emphasizes the importance of objectivity, opinions are not restricted to editorials or columns. Communications research shows that news coverage in the US, Germany, and other countries has been changing in the last decades. Instead of reporting mere events and facts, 5 journalists increasingly offer context, analysis, interpretation, and judgment (Schudson, 1978; Wilke & Reinemann, 2001; Barnhurst, 2003; Fink & Schudson, 2014). Among other explanations, this development reflects the growing complexity of many topics. As “makers of meaning”, journalists help their audiences to understand complicated issues (Barnhurst & Mutz, 1997). Impartial reporting often involves the provision of the whole spectrum of viewpoints, which makes it difficult for people to comprehend (Graber, 2001; Patterson & Seib, 2005), especially in the case of political and economic topics. Judgments can reduce the cognitive effort in processing information though, which facilitates the formation of own opinions (Dalton, Beck, & Huckfeldt, 1998; Smith & Searles, 2013). However, only reports that provide a clear, one-sided viewpoint can be assumed to be of additional heuristic value. Balanced or ambivalent news coverage – or two-sided messages in the phrasing of Zaller (1992) – requires an increase in cognitive effort, because the recipients have to analyze contradicting information on their own. The following hypotheses about the consumption of opinionated news thus refer to the case of one-sided, unambiguous viewpoints. 2.3 Hypotheses Hypothesis 1 (H1): Men consume more opinionated news than women. Previous research shows that social and cultural gender roles influence personality traits, which in turn cause differences in the way men and women process information (Meyers-Levy, 1988; Meyers-Levy & Maheswaran, 1991; Meyers-Levy & Sternthal, 1991; Darley & Smith, 1995; Laroche et al., 2000; Meyers-Levy & Loken, 2015). Male gender roles are often characterized by assertiveness and goal orientation. In contrast, female roles exhibit tendencies towards interpersonal affiliation and communal objectives. These role differences predispose women to be more sensitive to socio-emotional content and subtle 6 cues, which involves the comprehensive processing of information and low thresholds of cognitive elaboration. Male gender roles, however, often cause men to be selective processors that focus on self-relevant information and readily available cues. Moreover, biological and evolutionary factors cause differences in information processing to prevail, even when traditional gender roles dissolve in modern societies (Buss, 1995; Putrevu, 2001): Several tasks that are associated with childbirth and raising children require the ability to interpret subtle messages and information that pertains to others. Similar to other areas of consumption, sex- and gender-related predispositions should be detectable in the market for news. Hypothesis 2 (H2): The consumption of opinionated news increases with age. Certain physical and mental abilities decline when people get older, which often involves the speed at which the central nervous system processes information. Older people compensate for the loss of processing speed by making more use of their experience to employ cognitive shortcuts (Craik & Byrd, 1982; Reder, Wible, & Martin, 1986). Previous research confirms the age-related shift from effortful, systematic processing towards schema-based, heuristic strategies, for example in the context of consumption decisions (Yoon, 1997; Yoon, Cole, & Lee, 2009) and political evaluations (Riggle & Johnson, 1996). Hypothesis 3 (H3): The consumption of opinionated news decreases with cognitive abilities. Cognitive abilities allow individuals to comprehend complex issues and nuanced news coverage. According to the psychological concept of the need for cognition (e.g., Cacioppo & Petty, 1982; Cacioppo et al., 1996), these abilities enable people to process information systematically. Because engaging in effortful, comprehensive thinking can provide competence feedback and social 7 reinforcement, people with high cognitive skills also develop the motivation for mental elaboration. Cognitive abilities thus influence individual tendencies towards systematic or heuristic information processing, which in turn shape preferences over opinionated and objective news. The concept of political sophistication – which refers to the number, range, and degree of organization of a person’s political cognitions (Luskin, 1987) – further supports the hypothesis. Politically sophisticated people are assumed to possess high cognitive skills. Not only do these people consume more political information than less sophisticated individuals, they also process it differently (Rhee & Cappella, 1997). In particular, less sophisticated people more often rely on cognitive shortcuts to form opinions than sophisticates (Kam, 2005; Boudreau & McCubbins, 2010). Hypothesis 4a (H4a): People with a religious affiliation consume more opinionated news than people without a religious affiliation. A religion constitutes a system of values, standards, and principles, according to which a believer thinks and acts. Such a system provides mental schema that have an impact on the cognitions of a believer (Lau, 1989). The nature of religious values and principles often causes believers to adhere to higher levels of conservative, authoritarian, and dogmatic beliefs than non-believers. As a consequence, religious people tend to be more close-minded than non-religious ones (Delener, 1990, 1994; McDaniel & Burnett, 1990). In contrast to openminded thinking – which involves the active search for and evaluation of competing viewpoints – close-mindedness is usually associated with selective processing of information and relying on heuristics. Thus people with and without a religious affiliation likely differ in their consumption of opinionated news. 8 Hypothesis 4b (H4b): Catholics consume more opinionated news than Protestants. The two largest religious groups in Germany – Catholics and Protestants – exhibit differences that influence consumption behavior too. In Catholic families, for example, patriarchal and authoritarian structures are more common than in Protestant ones. Moreover, the Catholic Church mediates the believer’s relationship to God to a larger extent than in Protestantism. Accordingly, Catholics tend to be more traditional, dogmatic, and obedient to existing order (Delener, 1990, 1994; McDaniel & Burnett, 1990). Due to these differences, Protestants usually display greater levels of reliance on own judgments, and they more readily engage in information-seeking activities (Choi, 2010). Compared to Catholicism, Protestantism can thus be expected to predispose people towards systematic information processing. Hypothesis 5 (H5): East Germans consume more opinionated news than West Germans. During its separation after World War II, Germany was governed by two different political systems, which has been affecting people’s attitudes and behavior regarding information gathering and processing. With its market-based approach, the West German democracy encouraged its citizens to live individualist and self-responsible lives. In contrast, the communist East Germany was characterized by a far-reaching social security system, guaranteed employment, and the integration of its citizens into mass organizations. Moreover, East Germans were subject to constant surveillance by the Ministry of State Security, and subversive or dissident behavior was sanctioned severely. The internalization of the system’s norms has been causing East Germans to exhibit greater levels of obedience to authorities than West Germans (Mummert & Schneider, 2001; Torgeler, 2003). Empirical research shows that east-west differences in attitudes and preferences persist, even decades after reunification. 9 For example, East Germans are still more open to state intervention (Alesina & Fuchs-Schündeln, 2007; Kuhn, 2012) and display lower levels of self-reliance and entrepreneurial spirit (Bauernschuster et al., 2012). In addition, East and West Germans experienced different kinds of news coverage. In West Germany, freedom of speech and a free press facilitated the provision of multiple opinions and balanced news. In contrast, the widely statecontrolled, East German press often provided only one-sided viewpoints. It is likely that these reporting differences also have a persistent influence on people’s news preferences. In this case – and considering the comparatively strong predisposition towards belief in authority – it can be expected that the heuristic value of unambiguously opinionated news is larger for East than for West Germans. 3. Data 3.1 Sources and Sampling To evaluate the hypotheses, I compile data from consumption surveys and media content analyses. The survey data come from Allensbach Institute, one of Germany’s most important opinion and market research organizations. This institute conducts the Allensbach Media Market Analysis (AWA, see www.ifdallensbach.de/awa/service/english/overview.html), which features representative surveys of the German-speaking, resident population age 14 and over. Among other things, the data contain information about demographic characteristics, attitudes, and consumption habits. The six waves from 2002 to 2007 are available for the purpose of this study. Each year’s wave comprises about 14,000 face-toface interviews, conducted between October of the previous year and April of the respective year. The whole sample includes data from 85,757 interviews. 10 Information about the news output of German print media comes from Media Tenor International (www.mediatenor.com). This media research institute specializes on content analyses of political and economic news. The analyses are conducted by trained experts who follow consistent instructions in codebooks; their remuneration is based on inter-coder reliability and validity. The data used in this study comprise the daily newspapers Frankfurter Allgemeine Zeitung, Süddeutsche Zeitung, Die Welt, Frankfurter Rundschau; the daily tabloid Bild; the weekly newspaper Die Zeit; and the weekly news magazines Spiegel, Stern, and Focus. This selection includes all leading print media of national significance, except for Financial Times Deutschland and Handelsblatt. I exclude these two outlets because of the lack of consistent data. For the other nine outlets, the sample includes all news on politics and economics, which amounts to 119,838 reports. 3.2 An Indicator of the Individual Consumption of Opinionated News The AWA asks its respondents about their reading behavior regarding the abovementioned media. For each outlet, the AWA provides the respondent’s reading frequency on an ordinal scale that ranges from 0 (“never/unknown”) to 5 (“all issues”). The media content data can be used to compute the share of opinionated reports in the news coverage. I calculate this share separately for each medium, as well as for the time periods corresponding to the distinct AWA waves. Reports are treated as opinionated, if they contain an explicit or implicit judgment. According to Media Tenor International, an explicit judgment refers to a clear evaluation of the actor(s) or issue(s) by the means of positive/favorable/approving or negative/unfavorable/disapproving words. Giving an implicit opinion is defined as embedding the actor(s) or issue(s) in a judgmental context, for example, when a report describes government spending as vote catching. As discussed in the theory 11 section, only reports with unambiguous, one-sided judgments are considered as opinionated news. [Table 1 here] Table 1 shows the resulting shares. Accordingly, news outlets vary in their provision of opinionated news in politics and economics. For instance, the intellectual Die Zeit had an average share of 43.9% of opinionated reports in the periods under consideration, whereas the tabloid Bild exhibited an average of 59.2%. Moreover, the shares of judgmental reports varied over time. While the German print media provided opinionated articles in 57.6% of the cases in 2002, two years later this share amounted to 47%. Next, the shares of opinionated news are weighted by the respondents’ reading frequencies to obtain an indicator of the personal consumption of opinionated news. If medium at time , and denotes the reading frequency of the individual for the the share of opinionated news for the same medium and time, the indicator can be written as: ∑ = (1) ∑ The denominator accounts for individual-specific differences in the overall level of news consumption, so that personal shares of opinionated news are comparable across individuals, irrespective of general reading patterns. By construction, may vary between 0 and 100%. The indicator only captures people who at least sometimes consume print news. After subtracting the 15,465 nonreaders, the sample comprises 70,292 respondents. indicates that these respondents consume one-sidedly opinionated news in 53.4% of the cases. 12 3.3 Explanatory Variables The empirical goal of this study is to verify whether variation in the consumption indicator can be explained by individual information-processing predispositions. Due to the broad range of questions in the AWA surveys, several variables are readily available to measure or approximate these predispositions: • A dummy variable that takes the value 1 for men and 0 for women serves to verify hypothesis 1. • Verification of the second hypothesis is straightforward too. The AWA provides an age variable that categorizes the respondents into 14 equally divided age groups. • It is not possible to obtain a direct measure of cognitive ability from survey data. However, the empirical literature on the need for cognition, as well as on political sophistication, suggests various proxy variables; for reviews, see Cacioppo et al. (1996) and Luskin (1987), respectively. Based on these suggestions, I use dummy variable sets that indicate the respondents’ school degree, kind of job training, and occupation group. Hypothesis 3 is further evaluated by the respondents’ interest in politics and economics (both measured on an integer scale ranging from 1 to 3), and the households’ net income (in euro, measured on a scale with 13 equally divided categories). • A dummy variable set that captures the respondents’ religious denomination serves to verify hypothesis 4a and 4b. • Hypothesis 5 is evaluated in two ways. First, a dummy variable indicates whether a respondent resides in East or West Germany at the time of the interview. Because this dummy variable fails to account for relocations after reunification, another dummy variable set captures the respondents’ location in the fall of 1989. 13 4. Estimation and Results 4.1 Baseline Specification The AWA is a series of independent samples of respondents. Therefore, it is possible to estimate a model of repeated cross sections by ordinary least squares: = The dependent variable + + (2) + denotes the indicator of the consumption of opinionated news, which varies across individuals and over time . The vector contains the explanatory variables described in the previous section. The model also contains a vector of time dummies ( ) to account for exogenous variation in the amounts of opinionated news, such as related to general trends, business cycles, or political constellations. Because is based on equilibria of supply and demand, it is not necessary to control for outlet- or issue-specific characteristics (e.g., copy price or advertising expenditures). The ’s denote the (vectors of) parameters to be estimated. The error term is assumed to be independently and normally distributed. However, the assumption of homoscedastic residuals is relaxed; robust standard errors account for heteroscedasticity that might be introduced by estimating equation (2) with the sampling weights provided by the AWA. [Table 2 here] Table 2 shows the estimation results. Accordingly, men have significantly higher levels of consumption of opinionated news than women, which confirms hypothesis 1. The coefficient of the age variable has the expected positive sign. It is not significantly different from 0 though, so that hypothesis 2 cannot be confirmed. The respondents’ cognitive abilities are approximated by a multitude of variables. To begin with, people with a school degree other than higher secondary 14 education consume significantly more opinionated news. The decreasing magnitude of the coefficients corresponds to the rank order of the degrees, being largest for people with no degree (2.532) and smallest for respondents with a technical one (0.412). The estimates regarding the respondents’ job training reveal a similar pattern. People with no or vocational training consume more judgmental reports than individuals with a university degree. Respondents with technical training, however, exhibit lower consumption levels than university graduates. Grouping people into occupation groups shows that farmers and blue-collar workers exceed white-collar workers in the consumption of opinionated news; other differences are not significant. Moreover, the consumption of judgmental reports significantly decreases with rising interest in economics and especially politics. Finally, the decline that is associated with increasing household income is significant too. Hence the estimates confirm hypothesis 3, assuming that there is a positive relationship between cognitive abilities on the one hand, and schooling, vocational training, white-collar occupations, interest in politics/economics, and income on the other. The findings also confirm hypothesis 4b. Catholics consume more opinionated news than Protestants, although the coefficient is significant at the 5% level only. Other denominations do not differ from a Protestant one. People without a religious affiliation exhibit significantly lower consumption levels than Protestants – and therefore lower levels than people with a religious affiliation in general – so that hypothesis 4a can be confirmed too. Finally, the data support hypothesis 5. Having lived in West Germany in the fall of 1989 implies a significantly higher consumption of opinionated news than having lived in East Germany at this time. In addition to this socialization effect, there is geographical divide, because the difference between people currently living in East and West Germany is also significant. 15 4.2 Sensitivity Analysis These results rest on crucial assumptions about the measurement of the individual consumption of opinionated news. It is therefore advisable to evaluate modifications in the dependent variable. The first modification involves weighting daily and weekly media differently. In the baseline specification, the dependent variable does not account for the frequency of publication; weekly media are assumed to have the same contribution to the individuals’ share of opinionated news as daily media. Although this is a reasonable assumption – because people often read weekly magazines and newspapers over several days – a test can help to verify it. For that purpose, an alternative version of the dependent variable weights the contribution of the daily media in the sample six times higher than that of the weekly outlets (i.e., to account for publication from Monday to Saturday). The resulting variable is very similar to the baseline version; the bivariate correlation amounts to 0.932 (p < .01). As the first column in Table 3 shows, the coefficient estimates are generally robust towards the different weighting scheme. An exception is the age variable, which has a significantly negative coefficient now. Moreover, the difference between Catholics and Protestants is not significant anymore. [Table 3 here] The second modification of the dependent variable addresses the assumption that ambivalent or two-sided news do not have the same heuristic value as clear, one-sided reports. To check the validity of this assumption, an alternative version of the dependent variable is constructed that uses all reports that offer at least two different viewpoints to determine the shares of opinionated news. According to this variable, people consume reports with multiple opinions in about 16.4% of the cases. The alternative and baseline dependent variable are 16 negatively correlated (-0.133, p < .01), which suggests that people tend to prefer either one-sided opinions or multiple viewpoints. The corresponding estimates are almost a mirror image of those of the baseline specification, as the second column in Table 3 indicates: Women, Protestants, people without a religious affiliation, and West Germans consume more news with multiple viewpoints than their counterparts; and the consumption of this kind of news increases with cognitive abilities. Thus it can be concluded that processing news with one-sided opinions requires less cognitive effort than processing reports that provide diverse views. Third, the results depend on the accuracy of the content analyses conducted by Media Tenor International. Although the identification of opinions is subject to strict codebook criteria, as well as frequent tests of inter-coder reliability and validity, the possibility of inaccuracies or bias caused by subjective assessments of the coders cannot be ruled out completely. A more objective alternative to the baseline version of the dependent variable can be constructed by using counts of editorials and other explicit opinion articles. Based on this identification procedure, the respondents consume opinionated news in about 10% of the cases. The low bivariate correlation of this variable with the baseline version (0.176, p < .01) suggests large differences in measuring openly declared versus undeclared opinions. As the third column in Table 3 indicates, the estimates remain fairly stable though. Only the coefficients for male, older, Catholic, and West German respondents reveal major deviations from the baseline specification. Considering the very low model fit (R2 = 0.178), however, it is likely that these deviations result from the inaccurate way of capturing the amounts of opinionated reports in the news. Fourth, sampling weights are necessary to draw valid conclusions from the data at hand. Because sampling weights may cause inefficient estimates due to heteroscedasticity, it is important to calculate robust standard errors. These standard errors tend to be larger than conventional ones though, which might lead 17 to false conclusions about the coefficients’ statistical significance. For this reason, the final modification involves estimating the baseline specification without sampling weights, in which case the calculation of conventional standard errors is possible. As the fourth column in Table 3 shows, this alternative estimation method provides results that are very similar to the baseline specification; the coefficients do not differ substantially. 5. Conclusion Overall then, there is no evidence for the expected effect of the respondents’ age on the consumption of opinionated news (H2). The evidence for the hypothesized difference between Catholics and Protestants (H4b) is weak. The baseline specification provides the expected coefficient estimate, whereas most of the alternative models do not. Considering the other hypotheses, the findings are fairly robust though. The estimates confirm the presumed influence of the information-processing predispositions regarding sex and gender (H1), cognitive abilities (H3), religious affiliation (H4a), and east-west differences (H5). Thus it can be concluded that opinionated reports with one-sided (but not ambivalent) messages are of additional heuristic value to news consumers. Individual predispositions matter; they determine whether unambiguous opinions in the news coverage are valuable or disturbing to the recipient. Predispositions towards systematic information processing increase the consumption of objective or balanced news, whereas predispositions towards heuristic strategies favor the consumption of news with one-sided opinions. In large parts, this relationship can be interpreted causally, because people’s predispositions are based on characteristics that are usually fixed (sex, gender, location of socialization), or only change seldom or gradually (religion, cognitive abilities). In this constellation it is very unlikely for media companies to influence consumer preferences. Instead, individual predispositions determine the observed patterns in 18 the consumption of news. When it comes to the share of opinionated reports, the demand side thus drives the market: Consumers choose their preferred kind of news; media companies adjust their coverage to the preferences of the readers; or both. Hence the findings support news-market theories of consumer-based profit maximization (Mullainathan & Shleifer, 2005; Gentzkow & Shapiro, 2006, 2010; Chan & Suen, 2008). Media companies determine how challenging the consumption of their product is. Similar to many other product decisions – such as choices over cover stories, headlines, illustrations, or free supplements – changing the amount (more or less) and kind (one-sided or ambivalent) of opinion thus serves as a sales instrument. In contrast to previous, often experimental research on the use of heuristics, this study’s evidence is based on large-scale observational data. Although such a comprehensive approach has the advantage of a high level of external validity, there are some limitations. In particular, the data only refer to German, national print news about politics and economics. Further research is necessary to evaluate whether the relationship between audience predispositions and the opinionation of news holds in countries with other journalistic models, for other types of media, and other topics. 19 References Alesina, A., & Fuchs-Schündeln, N. (2007). Good-bye Lenin (or not?): The effect of communism on people’s preferences. American Economic Review, 97(4), 1507–1528. Andrew, B. C. (2007). Media-generated shortcuts: Do newspaper headlines present another roadblock for low-information rationality? Press/Politics, 12(2), 24–43. Barnhurst, K. G. (2003). The makers of meaning: National Public Radio and the new long journalism, 1980–2000. Political Communication, 20(1), 1–22. Barnhurst, K. G., & Mutz, D. (1997). American journalism and the decline in event-centered reporting. Journal of Communication, 47(4), 27–52. Bauernschuster, S., Falck, O., Gold, R., & Heblich, S. (2012). The shadows of the socialist past: Lack of self-reliance hinders entrepreneurship. European Journal of Political Economy, 28(4), 485–497. Benson, R., & Hallin, D. C. (2007). How states, markets and globalization shape the news: The French and US national press, 1965–97. European Journal of Communication, 22(1): 27–48. Boudreau, C., & McCubbins, M. D. (2010). The blind leading the blind: Who gets polling information and does it improve decisions? Journal of Politics, 72(2), 513–527. Boukes, M., Boomgaarden, H. G., Moorman, M., & De Vreese, C. H. (2014). News with an attitude: Assessing the mechanisms underlying the effects of opinionated news. Mass Communication and Society, 17(3), 354–378. Buss, D. M. (1995). Psychological sex differences: Origins through sexual selection. American Psychologist, 50(3), 164–168. Cacioppo, J. T., Petty. R. E. (1982). The need for cognition. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 42(1), 116–131. 20 Cacioppo, J. T., Petty, R. E., Feinstein, J. A., & Jarvis, W. B. G. (1996). Dispositional differences in cognitive motivation: The life and times of individuals varying in need for cognition. Psychological Bulletin, 119(2), 197–253. Chaiken, S. (1980). Heuristic versus systematic information processing and the use of source versus message cues in persuasion. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 39(5), 752–766. Chaiken, S., & Trope, Y. (Eds.). (1999). Dual-process theories in social psychology. New York: Guilford Press. Chan, J., & Suen, W. (2008). A spatial theory of news consumption and electoral competition. Review of Economic Studies, 75(3), 699–728. Choi, Y. (2010). Religion, religiosity, and South Korean consumer switching behaviors. Journal of Consumer Behavior, 9(3), 157–171. Craik, F. I. M., & Byrd, M. (1982). Aging and cognitive deficits: The role of attentional resources. In F. I. M. Craik & S. Trehub (Eds.), Aging and cognitive processes (pp. 191–211). New York: Plenum Press. Dalton, R. J., Beck, P. A., & Huckfeldt, R. (1998). Partisan cues and the media: Information flows in the 1992 presidential election. American Political Science Review, 92(1), 111–126. Darley, W. K., & Smith, R. E. (1995). Gender differences in information processing strategies: An empirical test of the selectivity model in advertising response. Journal of Advertising, 24(1), 41–56. Delener, N. (1990). The effects of religious factors on perceived risk in durable goods purchase decisions. Journal of Consumer Marketing, 7(3), 27–38. Delener, N. (1994). Religious contrasts in consumer decision behaviour patterns: Their dimensions and marketing implications. European Journal of Marketing, 28(5), 36–53. 21 DellaVigna, S., & Gentzkow, M. (2010). Persuasion: Empirical evidence. Annual Review of Economics, 2(1), 643–669. Esser, F., & Umbricht, A. (2013). Competing models of journalism? Political affairs coverage in US, British, German, Swiss, French and Italian newspapers. Journalism, 14(8), 989–1007. Feldman, L. (2011a). The opinion factor: The effects of opinionated news on information processing and attitude change. Political Communication, 28(2), 163–181. Feldman, L. (2011b). The effects of journalist opinionation on learning from the news. Journal of Communication, 61(6): 1183–1201. Fink, K., & Schudson, M. (2014). The rise of contextual journalism, 1950s– 2000s. Journalism, 15(1), 3–20. Gentzkow, M., & Shapiro, J. M. (2006). Media bias and reputation. Journal of Political Economy, 114(2), 280–316. Gentzkow, M., & Shapiro, J. M. (2010). What drives media slant? Evidence from U.S. daily newspapers. Econometrica, 78(1), 35–71. Graber, D. A. (2001). Adapting political news to the needs of twenty-first century Americans. In W. L. Bennett & R. M. Entman (Eds.), Mediated politics: Communication in the future of democracy (pp. 433–452). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. Kam, C. D. (2005). Who toes the party line? Cues, values, and individual differences. Political Behavior, 27(2), 163–182. Kuhn, A. (2012). Inequality perceptions, distributional norms, and redistributive preferences in East and West Germany. German Economic Review, 14(4), 483–499. Laroche, M., Saad, G., Cleveland, M., & Browne, E. (2000). Gender differences in information search strategies for a Christmas gift. Journal of Consumer Marketing, 17(6), 500–524. 22 Lau, R. R., & Redlawsk, D. P. (2001). Advantages and disadvantages of cognitive heuristics in political decision making. American Journal of Political Science, 45(4), 951–971. Lau, S. (1989). Religious schema and values. International Journal of Psychology, 24(2), 137–156. Luskin, R. C. (1987). Measuring political sophistication. Journal of Political Science, 31(4), 856–899. McDaniel, S. W., & Burnett, J. J. (1990). Consumer religiosity and retail store evaluative criteria. Journal of the Academy of Marketing Science, 18(2), 101–112. Meyers-Levy, J. (1988). The influence of sex roles on judgment. Journal of Consumer Research, 14(4), 522–530. Meyers-Levy, J., & Loken, B. (2015). Revisiting gender differences: What we know and what lies ahead. Journal of Consumer Psychology, 25(1), 129– 149. Meyers-Levy, J., & Maheswaran, D. (1991). Exploring differences in males’ and females’ processing strategies. Journal of Consumer Research, 18(1), 63– 70. Meyers-Levy, J., & Sternthal, B. (1991). Gender differences in the use of message cues and judgments. Journal of Marketing Research, 28(1), 84–96. Mullainathan, S., & Shleifer, A. (2005). The market for news. American Economic Review, 95(4), 1031–1053. Mummert, A., & Schneider, F. (2001). The German shadow economy: Parted in a united Germany. Public Finance Analysis, 58(3), 286–316. Patterson, T., & Seib, P. (2005). Informing the public. In G. Overholser & K. Hall Jamieson (Eds.), The press (pp. 189–202). Oxford: Oxford University Press. 23 Putrevu, S. (2001). Exploring the origins and information processing differences between men and women: Implications for advertisers. Academy of Marketing Science Review, 10(1), 1–14. Prior, M. (2013). Media and political polarization. Annual Review of Political Science, 16(1), 101–127. Reder, L. M., Wible, C., & Martin, J. (1986). Differential memory changes with age: Exact retrieval versus plausible inference. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 12(1), 72–81. Rhee, J. W., & Capella, J. N. (1997). The role of political sophistication in learning from news: Measuring schema development. Communication Research, 24(3), 197–233. Riggle, E. D. B., & Johnson, M. M. S. (1996). Age differences in political decision making: Strategies for evaluating political candidates. Political Behavior, 18(1), 99–118. Schudson, M. (1978). Discovering the news: A social history of American newspapers. New York: Basic Books. Smith, G., & Searles, K. (2013). Fair and balanced news or a difference of opinion? Why opinion shows matter for media effects. Political Research Quarterly, 66(3), 671–684. Torgler, B. (2003). Does culture matter? Tax morale in an East-West-German comparison. Public Finance Analysis, 59(4), 504–528. Wilke, J., & Reinemann, C. (2001). Do the candidates matter? Long-term trends of campaign coverage: A study of the German press since 1949. European Journal of Communication, 16(3), 291–314. Yoon, C. (1997). Age differences in consumers’ processing strategies: An investigation of moderating influences. Journal of Consumer Research, 24(3), 329–342. 24 Yoon, C., Cole, C. A., & Lee, M. P. (2009). Consumer decision making and aging: Current knowledge and future directions. Journal of Consumer Psychology, 19(1), 2–16. Zaller, J. R. (1992). The nature and origins of mass opinion. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. 25 Table 1: Shares of Opinionated News across Outlets and over Time (%) 2002 2003 2004 2005 2006 2007 Frankfurter Allgemeine 53.1 46.4 43.9 50.4 56.0 55.2 50.8 Zeitung Süddeutsche Zeitung 56.4 47.5 45.7 52.2 55.6 55.7 52.2 Die Welt 57.9 55.6 48.1 56.8 58.5 60.4 56.2 Frankfurter Rundschau 59.1 49.0 47.2 52.4 53.3 58.0 53.2 Bild 60.5 54.1 49.9 65.7 57.4 67.7 59.2 Die Zeit 46.9 39.9 44.9 44.7 36.0 50.9 43.9 Spiegel 59.0 47.2 53.2 51.5 53.1 46.7 51.8 Stern 58.5 52.8 41.0 50.9 40.8 49.6 48.9 Focus 67.0 45.8 49.3 53.1 59.4 36.2 51.8 57.6 48.7 47.0 53.1 52.2 53.4 Notes: Own calculations based on data from Media Tenor International. Italic figures are row and column means. 26 Table 2: Estimation Results Coefficient Standard error Hypothesis Male 0.668 *** 0.042 1 Age 0.008 0.007 2 School degree (ref.: higher secondary) None 2.532 *** 0.149 3 Lower secondary 2.034 *** 0.065 3 Intermediate secondary 1.093 *** 0.060 3 Technical 0.412 *** 0.072 3 Job training (ref.: university degree) No degree 0.718 *** 0.084 3 Vocational training 0.253 *** 0.065 3 Technical training -0.229 *** 0.075 3 Occupation group (ref.: white collar) Blue collar 0.888 *** 0.055 3 0.072 3 Civil servant -0.027 Farmer 0.345 ** 0.154 3 Self-employed 0.099 0.096 3 0.098 3 N/A 0.021 Interest in politics -0.636 *** 0.033 3 Interest in economics -0.386 *** 0.032 3 Household income -0.029 *** 0.007 3 Denomination (ref.: Protestant) Catholic 0.099 ** 0.047 4 0.159 4 Other 0.014 None -0.157 *** 0.050 4 Current residence West -0.614 *** 0.111 5 Residence fall 1989 (ref.: East) West -0.719 *** 0.107 5 0.200 5 Abroad 0.110 Born after 0.409 0.382 5 No response -0.097 0.233 5 Notes: The model contains an intercept and time dummies (output omitted). R2 = 0.490; N = 70,292. *p < .10. **p < .05. ***p < 0.01. 27 Table 3: Alternative Estimates 1 0.845 *** -0.016 ** Modification 2 3 -0.441 *** -0.405 *** 0.005 0.011 ** 4 Male 0.669 *** Age -0.004 School degree (ref.: higher secondary) No degree 2.344 *** -2.202 *** 0.967 *** 2.487 *** Lower secondary degree 2.111 *** -1.813 *** 0.944 *** 2.058 *** -1.026 *** 0.532 *** 1.082 *** Intermediate secondary degree 1.182 *** Technical degree 0.462 *** -0.526 *** 0.238 *** 0.421 *** Job training (ref.: university degree) -0.757 *** 0.340 *** 0.796 *** No degree 0.773 *** Vocational training 0.422 *** -0.485 *** 0.104 ** 0.361 *** Technical training -0.044 -0.088 * -0.237 *** -0.167 ** Occupation group (ref.: white collar) Blue collar 0.809 *** -0.713 *** 0.372 *** 0.893 *** Civil servant 0.026 0.023 -0.090 * 0.099 Farmer 0.213 -0.295 *** -0.147 0.267 ** -0.108 * -0.014 0.125 Self-employed 0.134 N/A -0.164 * -0.098 0.152 ** -0.090 Interest in politics -0.512 *** 0.508 *** -0.364 *** -0.631 *** Interest in economics -0.348 *** 0.340 *** -0.254 *** -0.337 *** Household income -0.026 *** 0.031 *** -0.021 *** -0.025 *** Denomination (ref.: Protestant) Catholic -0.016 -0.028 0.025 0.111 *** Other -0.085 0.115 -0.086 -0.047 0.233 *** -0.058 * -0.148 *** None -0.202 *** Current residence West -0.523 *** 0.473 *** 0.412 *** -0.629 *** Residence fall 1989 (ref.: East) 0.793 *** -0.022 -0.729 *** West -0.582 *** Abroad 0.163 0.415 *** 0.135 0.055 Born after 0.137 0.556 *** 0.194 -0.122 0.488 ** 0.460 *** 0.636 *** No response -0.191 R2 0.496 0.600 0.178 0.492 Notes: Modification 1: different weighting of daily and weekly media; modification 2: only treating reports with multiple viewpoints as opinionated news; modification 3: shares of explicit opinion articles instead of all opinionated reports; modification 4: calculation of conventional standard errors and no sampling weights. All models contain an intercept and time dummies (output omitted). N = 70,292. *p < .10. **p < .05. ***p < 0.01. 28
© Copyright 2024