Ecological Modelling 192 (2006) 618–626 Why fruits rot, seeds mold and meat spoils: A reappraisal Thomas N. Sherratt a,∗ , David M. Wilkinson b , Roderick S. Bain a a b Department of Biology, Carleton University, 1125 Colonel By Drive, Ottawa, Ont., Canada K1S 5B6 Biology and Earth Sciences, Liverpool John Moores University, Byrom Street, Liverpool L3 3AF, UK Received 3 December 2004; received in revised form 2 June 2005; accepted 18 July 2005 Available online 28 September 2005 Abstract It has been argued that micro-organisms may gain a selective advantage by rendering fruit, seeds and meat as objectionable to larger animals as possible, thereby increasing the likelihood that the micro-organisms retain the resource. Here, we demonstrate that if spoiling carries a cost then not even group selection can enable a spoiling strategy to persist. In the absence of such a cost, then spoilers will be able to persist even without the actions of a larger animal, yet spread from rarity only under a limited set of conditions. We therefore question whether this verbally attractive theory is tenable, and offer alternative explanations for why rotting fruit, seeds and meat tend to be repellent to larger animals. © 2005 Elsevier B.V. All rights reserved. Keywords: Microbial competition; Spoiling; Group selection; Free riders 1. Introduction In 1977, Daniel Janzen proposed a characteristically imaginative and unorthodox theory. He argued that the consumption of a microbe and its resources by an animal would be deleterious to most microbes, hence microbes would be (p. 691) “under strong selection pressure to render seeds, fresh fruit, or carcasses as objectionable or unusable to larger organisms as is possible in the shortest period of time”. To dramatise the largely overlooked possibility of microbe–macrobe competition, Janzen (1979) described a scenario of a ∗ Corresponding author. Tel.: +1 613 520 2600x1748; fax: +1 613 520 3539. E-mail address: sherratt@ccs.carleton.ca (T.N. Sherratt). 0304-3800/$ – see front matter © 2005 Elsevier B.V. All rights reserved. doi:10.1016/j.ecolmodel.2005.07.030 youngster left alone for a short time in the kitchen with two strawberries, one fresh and one moldy. If this youngster pops the fresh one in its mouth, then “the microbe has won”! Like the allegorical youngster, a range of bird species do indeed exhibit preferences for consuming fresh fruits over putrefying fruits (e.g. Borowicz, 1988; Buchholz and Levey, 1990; Cipollini and Stiles, 1993). While Janzen (1977, 1979) stressed that he did not believe that all repellent chemicals released by microbes were solely selected to deter larger animals, he did propose that animals may have “played a large and virtually unrecognized role in evolution of their production”. Thus, fruits rot, seeds mold and meat spoils in large measure because that is the way microbes compete with bigger organisms. T.N. Sherratt et al. / Ecological Modelling 192 (2006) 618–626 Since its publication, Janzen’s paper has been widely cited in the primary literature (a Web of Science® search on May 27, 2005 indicated 150 citations) as well as in more general texts (Cockburn, 1991, p. 333; Stiling, 2002, p. 140). The vast majority of these citations have viewed the idea favourably, indeed one of us has previously described this idea as “a fine example of the importance of thinking about microbial ecology” (Wilkinson, 1998). However, somewhat surprisingly, the conditions (if any) under which Janzen’s proposal might work have never been formally identified. Here, we show using a numerical simulation model that if the generation of spoiling chemicals carries an individual cost to the “spoiler”, then larger animals cannot by themselves facilitate selection for spoiling. In this case, spoiling forms will be rapidly undermined by “free-riding” (Axelrod and Dion, 1988) non-spoilers that enjoy the same benefit as spoilers (namely, not being eaten) but do not pay the cost. Many microbes are predominantly clonal (Maynard Smith et al., 1993), and while it was proposed that “a kind of group selection” (Janzen, 1977) at the level of the patch might allow spoiling to persist, here we argue that the rate of movement of microbes among fruit, seeds, and cadavers is likely to be far too high for this form of selection to operate. If spoiling does not carry an individual cost then clearly we do not need to invoke preferential foraging behaviour of animals to explain the persistence of this strategy. Yet even here, spoiling microbes will only spread from rarity under a restricted set of conditions. 2. The model In the following model, we consider an environment containing a fixed number of available microbial resource patches (n), which for simplicity we call fruit, but they might equally be thought of as seeds or cadavers. We have wind-fallen fruits in mind, or at least a resource that is potentially available for consumption by a vertebrate. These fallen fruits can be colonised by microbes that render the patch less attractive to large animals (spoilers), and/or by microbes that do not render the patch less attractive (non-spoilers). By definition, individual fruits that support a high amount of spoiling microbes are less likely to be eaten by a frugivore than similar fruits with fewer spoilers. The model combines continuous-time processes (growth 619 of microbial populations and concomitant depletion of resources) with discrete-time processes (consumption of fruit and transmission of microbes between fruit, followed by fruit replacement) which occur at the end of every unit time step. Our simulation was implemented in Microsoft® Visual Basic 6, and utilized a BASIC Numerical Analysis Library (BNALib) for the numerical solution of differential equations via a fourth-order Runge-Kutta method. The continuous and discrete events were combined by first allowing fruit consumption and transmission of microbes between fruit (in random order), then numerically integrating our differential equations over a unit time period (see below) and finally replacing decayed fruit, iterating the whole process over many time steps. 2.1. Growth of microbes and depletion of resources The continuous-time equations for microbial growth on each fruit i were of the form: dRi = −kN Ni Ri − kS Si Ri dt (1.1) dNi = fN Ri Ni − bNi dt (1.2) dSi = fS Ri Si − bSi dt (1.3) where Ri is the amount of resource (comprising everything in the fruit available for microbes), Ni and Si the amount of non-spoilers and spoilers on fruit i, respectively, b the microbe mortality coefficient, kN and kS and fN and fS are the consumption and reproduction coefficients for non-spoilers and spoilers, respectively (see Table 1). In this way, a single fruit starting with a resource of R = 25 and initial amount of microbes of N = 5 (with no spoiling forms present) at t = 0 would be depleted by microbial action to one tenth of its resource value in approximately 25 time units when kN = 0.01, fN = 0.02, b = 0.2 (Fig. 1). As fruit may typically decay in a matter of a few weeks, one can conveniently consider a unit time in many of our simulations as a single day. However, we stress that the qualitative conclusions we draw from our model are only dependent on the relative sizes of key parameters such as fN and fS , and they are in no way dependent on the exact sizes of parameters chosen. 620 T.N. Sherratt et al. / Ecological Modelling 192 (2006) 618–626 Table 1 Glossary of terms used in the model Parameter or rule Interpretation n Ri Ni , S i kN , k S fN , fS c b x R* maxreplace1, maxreplace2 q maxdonatep maxN maxS SD CR1-3 RR1-2 SC1-2 Number of fruits available (assumed constant) Amount of resources on fruit i Amount of non-spoilers and spoilers on fruit i Consumption coefficients for non-spoilers and spoilers Reproduction coefficients for non-spoilers and spoilers Weighting coefficient used in consumption rule 2 Microbe mortality coefficient Number of fruit consumed each discrete-time step Critical fruit resource level, below which replacement occurs Maximum amount of microbes on any fruit that is replaced under microbial replacement rules 1 and 2 Probability that a given fruit donates microbes to another per time step Maximum proportion of microbes donated in a transfer event Initial maximum amount of non-spoilers in each fruit Initial maximum amount of spoilers in each fruit (starting condition 1 only) Starting amount of spoilers in a single fruit (starting condition 2 only) x Fruit selected at: 1 = random; 2 = highest Ri − c Si ; 3 = highest (Ri /Si ) Inoculum in replaced fruit from: 1 = all other fruit; 2 = single random fruit Spoilers initially in: 1 = all fruit; 2 = single random fruit 2.2. Fruit consumption At each discrete unit time step, a fixed number x of the available n fruit (x < n) were consumed by frugivores. Three different fruit consumption rules (CR) were considered. Under CR1 (a control condition), the x fruit were selected at random from the available fruit, independent of their resource value or microbial community. Under CR2, the x fruit with the highest Ri − cSi were selected for consumption, where c is a weighting coefficient (the higher c, the more important it is for foragers to avoid spoiling microbes than to obtain high value resources). Under CR3, the x fruit with the highest Ri /Si were chosen. In all cases of tied preference for the most desirable fruit, the fruit with the highest R within this equally preferred subset were taken. Clearly, CR2 and CR3 represent extreme cases of selection against non-spoilers given complete information—if such behaviour cannot generate selection for spoilers, then it is unlikely that spoilers could evolve through another form of frugivore behaviour. 2.3. Fruit replacement Fig. 1. Standard graph for the depletion of a single fruit resource (continuous line) by microbes, and the subsequent decline of microbes (dotted line). R(0) = 25, kN = 0.01, fN = 0.02, b = 0.2. The fruit starts with a microbial amount of 5 units. A unit time is considered a day. When fruit were eaten, or depleted by microbial activity to a value R* , then they were replaced with “fresh” fruit with initial resource R(0). These fresh fruit were seeded with an initial inoculum of microbes, whose exact composition was dependent on the specific replacement rule (RR) invoked. Under RR1, any new fruit was given an inoculum drawn from a discrete uniform distribution ranging from 1 to maxreplace1 while the proportion of spoilers in this inoculum was equal to the weighted proportion of spoilers in the rest of the global microbial community (excluding T.N. Sherratt et al. / Ecological Modelling 192 (2006) 618–626 those microbes on fruit selected for consumption in the current time step). Under RR2, the size of the inoculum was simply maxreplace2 and the proportion of spoilers in the inoculum was equal to the proportion of spoilers in a single randomly chosen surviving fruit. These two rules clearly lie at opposite extreme ends of a continuum. Of course, our assumption of maintaining a fixed number of available fruit is somewhat arbitrary, but preferable to any other set of rules given that the net number of fruit in natural systems will vary in a complex way in both space and time. 621 3. Results In the following sections, we have independently varied several key parameters to elucidate their effect. However, we have confirmed that our qualitative conclusions are the same for a range of other parameter combinations. We have also reached an identical set of conclusions using discrete-time models with different sets of assumptions (not shown), and draw on a simple phenomenological model (Maynard Smith, 1998, see discussion) to interpret our findings. 3.1. Oscillatory solutions for resources 2.4. Microbial dispersal To represent chance dispersal events, a small amount of microbes on any given fruit i were passed to any given fruit j (i = j) with probability q per pair per unit time step. All possible pairs of fruit in the system were considered for dispersal events, both as potential donors of microbes and as potential recipients. If dispersal occurred, then the proportion of a donor’s microbes transferred was randomly drawn from a uniform distribution between 0 and maxdonatep and comprised the same mixture of spoilers and non-spoilers present in the donor. Microbial dispersal could occur either before or after the fruit consumption (and concomitant replacement) at each discrete-time step. To avoid any priority effects (e.g. see Ruxton and Saravia, 1998), we let the probability that dispersal occured before consumption be 0.5. The mean amount of resource per fruit and the number of microbes per fruit frequently exhibited oscillatory dynamics (Fig. 2). This form of periodic dynamics was evident even without consumption of fruit, and included both damped oscillations and more permanent cycles (see Fig. 2). The oscillations arise simply as a consequence of “pulses” in resource availability as decayed fruit are replaced with fresh fruit. Thus, under most foraging rules frugivores tend to concentrate on 2.5. Starting conditions Our starting conditions (SC) were of two different forms, mirroring our fruit replacement rules. In both cases, all n fruit started with R(0) resources, and each fruit was given a quantity of non-spoilers drawn from a discrete uniform distribution between 1 and maxN. Thus, the starting densities of microbes on each fruit were not identical, although the initial amount of resource that each fruit contained was. In SC1, each fruit also received an integer amount of spoilers drawn from a uniform distribution 1 to maxS. However, to analyse whether a rare mutant spoiling form might spread from rarity, in SC2 we allowed only a single randomly chosen fruit to have spoilers, and the amount of these spoilers was given by SD. Fig. 2. How the mean amount of resources per fruit (continuous line) and the mean amount of spoiling and non-spoiling microbes per fruit (dotted lines) fluctuate over time in a single replicate simulation. Parameter values: n = 100 fruit, kN = 0.01, kS = 0.01, b = 0.2, fN = 0.03, fS = 0.03, R(0) = 25, R* = 2.5, q = 0.1, maxreplace2 = 0.1, maxreplacep = 0.001, maxN = 5, maxS = 5, x = 20 fruit consumed, consumption rule 3 (highest difference), starting rule 1 (spoilers initially on all fruit) and replacement rule 2 (inoculum from a single fruit). Note that the oscillations do not represent fluctuations in any individual fruit (Ri always declines), but reflect changes in the total amount of resources in the system as a consequence of decayed and consumed fruit being replaced with fresh fruit. 622 T.N. Sherratt et al. / Ecological Modelling 192 (2006) 618–626 Fig. 3. Change in the proportion of spoilers in the population over time under the parameter combinations listed in Table 2. Error bars represent ±1 standard deviation of the mean proportion of spoilers per fruit per unit time, calculated over 50 replicate simulations. Starting condition 1 (spoilers initially on all fruit) and replacement rule 2 (inoculum from a single fruit) assumed. the freshly available fruit—when a cohort of decayed and unattractive fruit is eventually replaced, it generates a temporary increase in fruit availability. One way to see this is through varying the resource threshold R* beyond which a fruit is replaced. When we reduce the threshold R* from 2.5 to 0.5 and keep all other variables the same as Fig. 2, then the resource dynamics moves to permanent cycles (not shown) and the period increases from 16 to 52 units (as estimated using autocorrelation analysis). Clearly by lowering R* , fruit stays around for longer before it is completely decayed and replaced, and this alteration consequently lengthens the period of any cycle that is generated. It is important to note however that in all of our simulations, the proportion of microbes that were spoilers did not oscillate and always reached an equilibrium, generally within 500 time steps (see Fig. 3, for example). 3.2. Scenario A: spoiling is cost free to the spoiler Here, we ask whether a spoiling form would spread in a microbial population if it could spoil fruit at no cost to its reproductive output (fN = fS ). Such a condition would arise, for instance, if the repellent chemicals were a by-product of microbial metabolism (alcohols are a possible example). If this condition holds, we find that spoiling microbes can persist, and indeed (depending on starting conditions), they may come to represent a reasonably high proportion of the population. Here, the proportion of spoilers increases in the overall microbial population directly as a result of the selective action of frugivores. For example, we note that the mean proportion of spoilers in the microbial population was significantly higher in all selective predation simulations compared to no predation and random predation simulations (Table 2). As such, our analysis supports Janzen’s argument that selective predation may help promote the evolution of spoiling microbes when spoiling carries no reproductive cost. However, there is an important caveat. Note that the starting conditions have a significant influence on the equilibrium mean proportion of spoilers in the population, and that spoilers never evolve to fixation. The central reason for these observations is that the nonspoilers in any fruit gain just as much as spoilers. Thus, as more and more exchange between spoiling and nonspoiling microbes occurs among the fruit (and fruit are replaced, in some cases with a representative general inoculum), then fruit begin to support increasingly similar proportions of spoilers and non-spoilers, and selection can no longer act. This phenomenon can be seen in several ways. For example, in a single run of the model under conditions in Fig. 3 the mean proportion of spoiling microbes per fruit rose from 0.500 to a stable 0.673 in 500 time steps but the standard deviation of the proportion of spoiling microbes among individual fruit within the system fell from 0.120 to 0.001. Secondly, when the microbial dispersal parameter was reduced from 0.1 to 0.001 then a higher proportion of spoiling microbes could be reached before fruit became homogeneous in content, and further selection was prevented (cf Table 2 versus Table 3). To take an extreme, when dispersal was removed altogether (q = 0, all other conditions as for Table 3) then under starting rule 1 and replacement rule 2 the proportion of microbes that were of the spoiling form rose from an overall mean of 0.524 (S.D. 0.011 over replicates) in generation 1 to stable mean proportion of 0.892 (S.D. 0.021) by generation 1000, based on 50 replicate simulations. Clearly, there is no simple dispersal threshold at which spoilers will flourish. However, lower dispersal rates simply extend the period of time before all fruit become similar in their mix of spoilers and non-spoilers. When there is little or no variability among fruit, frugivores cannot exercise their preference for non-spoiled fruit, and selection can no longer act on this basis. T.N. Sherratt et al. / Ecological Modelling 192 (2006) 618–626 623 Table 2 The mean proportion of microbes that were spoilers after time step 1 (first line, with standard deviation in brackets) and after time step 1000 (second line in cell, with standard deviation) under different predation rules, fruit replacement rules and starting conditions Predation Highest Ri − cSi (CR2) Highest Ri /Si (CR3) Spoilers initially in all fruit (SC1) Replacement inoculum from all other fruit (RR1) 0.49817 (0.00876) 0.49983 (0.01136) 0.49628 (0.01239) 0.49697 (0.01967) 0.52256 (0.01108) 0.61507 (0.02144) 0.52248 (0.00986) 0.59103 (0.01550) Replacement inoculum from single fruit (RR2) 0.50048 (0.01028) 0.50034 (0.01230) 0.00000a (0.00000) 0.49677 (0.03549) 0.52163 (0.01277) 0.68940 (0.03141) 0.52081 (0.01286) 0.68638 (0.02938) Spoilers initially in single fruit (SC2) Replacement inoculum from all other fruit (RR1) 0.00018 (0.00001) 0.00018 (0.00006) 0.00056 (0.00052) 0.00026 (0.00046) 0.00020 (0.00001) 0.00070 (0.00064) 0.00020 (0.00001) 0.00059 (0.00031) Replacement inoculum from single fruit (RR2) 0.00018 (0.00001) 0.00018 (0.00006) 0.00000a (0.00000) 0.00016 (0.00028) 0.00021 (0.00001) 0.00498 (0.00644) 0.00020 (0.00001) 0.00860 (0.00825) No predation Random predation (CR1) Mean and standard deviations are all based on 50 separate simulations, each for 1000 time units (effectively 1000 days based on natural decay rates). Here, we assume no reproductive costs to spoiling (fN = fS ). Parameter values: n = 100, kN = 0.005, kS = 0.005, b = 0.2, fN = 0.02, fS = 0.02, R(0) = 25, R* = 2.5, c = 1000, x = 10, q = 0.1, maxreplace1 = 20, maxreplace2 = 0.1, maxdonatep = 0.001, maxN = 10, maxS = 10, SD = 0.1. a All microbes eventually go extinct due to continual depletion of resources. Finally, we note that changing the actual number of fruit consumed per unit time step (x) had little influence on our general conclusions. For instance, increasing the number of fruit consumed from 10 to 30 but keeping all other conditions the same as in Table 3 tended to increase the overall mean proportion of spoilers in the microbial population slightly, but the same general patterns were apparent. 3.3. Scenario B: spoiling incurs a small reproductive cost In this instance, we assume that there is a metabolic cost to the production of spoiling chemicals, such that a non-spoiling colonist would eventually multiply and out-compete any spoilers within the same fruit (assuming the fruit lasts that long). If these spoiling chemicals were produced at least in part to deter large animals from consuming the resource, then we feel that such a cost would be likely. For instance, the antibiotic tetracycline requires over 70 separate enzymatic steps in its synthesis (Madigan et al., 2000) and it is known that in soils, populations of antibiotic-producing bacteria grow more slowly than many non-antibiotic producers (Wiener, 2000). When fN > fS then spoiling microbes invariably go globally extinct. For instance, in replicate simulations identical to that depicted in Table 2 but with fS = 0.019 then all spoiling microbes went extinct by t = 1000 in all simulations. Even if dispersal of microbes between fruit was reduced to very low levels, then spoiling microbes were either extinct by t = 1000 or at least declining towards extinction (Table 4). Since nonspoilers will eventually be selected over spoilers within any given fruit, then the only way spoilers can persist is if they have a chance of occupying a fruit alone for much of the fruit’s existence. With even low levels of dispersal then this group-beneficial trait is outcompeted by non-spoilers in the same fruit that share the benefit, but pay none of the costs. 4. Discussion We have argued that for Janzen’s provocative spoiling theory to work, then there should be no benefits from “free-riding”, otherwise microbes would be selected to take the benefits of not being eaten by a larger animal without paying the cost. Such instability is widely reported in n-player cooperative games 624 T.N. Sherratt et al. / Ecological Modelling 192 (2006) 618–626 Table 3 The mean proportion of microbes that were spoilers after time step 1 (first line, with standard deviation in brackets) and after time step 1000 (second line in cell, with standard deviation) under different predation rules, fruit replacement rules and starting conditions Predation Highest Ri − cSi (CR2) Highest Ri /Si (CR3) Spoilers initially in all fruit (SC1) Replacement inoculum from all other fruit (RR1) 0.50062 (0.00850) 0.49879 (0.01135) 0.50023 (0.01103) 0.50125 (0.02094) 0.52168 (0.01244) 0.61932 (0.01949) 0.52020 (0.01103) 0.58706 (0.02003) Replacement inoculum from single fruit (RR2) 0.49924 (0.00942) 0.50045 (0.01200) 0.00000a (0.00000) 0.49467 (0.16296) 0.52251 (0.01199) 0.88747 (0.02752) 0.51909 (0.01131) 0.88930 (0.02654) Spoilers initially in single fruit (SC2) Replacement inoculum from all other fruit (RR1) 0.00018 (0.00001) 0.00017 (0.00007) 0.00047 (0.00062) 0.00026 (0.00038) 0.00020 (0.00001) 0.00062 (0.00068) 0.00020 (0.00001) 0.00079 (0.00055) Replacement inoculum from single fruit (RR2) 0.00018 (0.00001) 0.00019 (0.00006) 0.00000a (0.00000) 0.00003 (0.00016) 0.00020 (0.00001) 0.02209 (0.02721) 0.00020 (0.00001) 0.02490 (0.02196) No predation Random predation (CR1) Mean and standard deviations are all based on 50 separate simulations, each for 1000 time units. Here, we assume no reproductive costs to spoiling (fN = fS ). Parameter values as for Table 2 except dispersal parameter q = 0.001. a All microbes eventually go extinct due to continual depletion of resources. (e.g. Dugatkin, 1997). The presence of putrefying chemicals in rotting food may well dissuade potential consumers from eating it, but this in itself does not provide sufficient grounds to argue that the repellent chemicals have arisen, or are maintained, for this reason. As we have shown, even group-level selection is unlikely to facilitate the persistence of spoilers because the dispersal rates of microbes are almost certainly Table 4 The mean proportion of microbes that were spoilers after time step 1 (first line, with standard deviation in brackets) and after time step 1000 (second line in cell, with standard deviation) under different predation rules, fruit replacement rules and starting conditions Predation Highest Ri − cSi (CR2) Highest Ri /Si (CR3) Spoilers initially in all fruit (SC1) Replacement inoculum from all other fruit (RR1) 0.49516 (0.01059) 0.49532 (0.01110) 0.00000 (0.00001) 0.00000 (0.00000) 0.51636 (0.01558) 0.00000 (0.00000) 0.51674 (0.01391) 0.00000 (0.00000) Replacement inoculum from single fruit (RR2) 0.49202 (0.00918) 0.49131 (0.01219) 0.00000 (0.00000) 0.00000 (0.00000) 0.51698 (0.01111) 0.02162 (0.00280) 0.51836 (0.01201) 0.00059 (0.00012) Spoilers initially in single fruit (SC2) Replacement inoculum from all other fruit (RR1) 0.00018 (0.00001) 0.00018 (0.00005) 0.00000 (0.00000) 0.00000 (0.00000) 0.00020 (0.00001) 0.00000 (0.00000) 0.00020 (0.00001) 0.00000 (0.00000) Replacement inoculum from single fruit (RR2) 0.00018 (0.00001) 0.00019 (0.00005) 0.00000 (0.00000) 0.00000 (0.00000) 0.00020 (0.00001) 0.00017 (0.00031) 0.00020 (0.00001) 0.00000 (0.00000) No predation Random predation (CR1) Mean and standard deviations are all based on 50 separate simulations, each for 1000 time units. Here, we assume a small reproductive costs to spoiling (fN > fS ). Parameter values as for Table 2 except q = 0.001 (as Table 3) and fS = 0.019. T.N. Sherratt et al. / Ecological Modelling 192 (2006) 618–626 too high to allow patches containing only spoilers to arise. Our consideration of the dynamics of spoilers and non-spoilers reflects more general questions in evolutionary biology; indeed in some ways our simulations can be seen as unnecessarily detailed. For example, an illuminating model for the spread of an altruistic trait that promotes the persistence of individuals within patches has already been presented by Maynard Smith (1998). As with our study, Maynard Smith argued that the dispersal rates of more selfish forms between groups would have to be exceptionally low to promote the spread of altruistic traits. Edible fruit typically decay or are eaten in a matter of days or weeks. Microbes may be transferred between fallen fruit in a variety of ways ranging from wind-assisted movement, to transfer by fruit feeding organisms such as insects (e.g. see Ehlers and Olesen, 1997). Many microbes are also likely to be present even before the fruit falls. Despite the ephemeral nature of the resource, and considerable uncertainty over the way microbes disperse, we feel that a dispersal rate as low as that required to allow spoiling microbes to spread significantly from rarity via group selection, would be extremely unlikely. Some plausible alternative explanations for why fruits rot, seeds mold and meat spoils, are simply that (i) it has arisen as a result of chemically mediated competition between microbes and/or that (ii) repellency has arisen primarily as a by-product of other metabolic activities, such as extra-cellular digestion. In effect, explanation (i) moves the assumed competitor from vertebrate back to microbe, but the nature of the competitive interactions can be qualitatively different. For example, in relation to explanation (i), it is possible that the repellent compounds are made without cost by the spoilers, but that they also inhibit the growth of the nonspoilers. This might happen for instance, if spoilers are immune to their own repellent by-products. In parallel work (not shown) we have found that it is the nonspoilers invariably go extinct under these conditions. Non-spoilers in these circumstances effectively face the “double whammy” by being poorer competitors and eliciting higher extinction rates through frugivory. However, it should be noted that non-spoilers go extinct even if frugivores are non-selective, suggesting that this type of spoiling behaviour does not owe its success to frugivore behaviour. 625 In relation to explanation (ii) above, one might wonder why it does not pay microbes to free-ride on the extra-cellular digestion products of others, in the same way non-spoilers free-ride on spoilers. An analogous example is of the production of the precursors of dimethyl sulfide by marine plankton, which some workers have argued may benefit the population as a whole (see review by Simo, 2001). Here, however, there is perhaps more potential for asymmetries in that the chemicals are likely to benefit the individual that produced it and its relatives more than its unrelated conspecifics or heterospecifics (see Brown, 1999 for a game-theoretical solution to this type of problem). Indeed many bacteria appear to have adaptations that keep the enzymes used in extracellular digestion in very close proximity to the cell membranes (Fenchel et al., 1998). Costly traits that benefit the individual and the group may spread when populations exhibit high viscosity. However, in the case of rotting fruit, it is likely that dispersal can reduce the between-group variability sufficiently to undermine the selective advantage of any group-beneficial trait that appears in the system. Overall, our analyses indicate that spoilers can coexist with non-spoilers if creating repellent chemicals carries no higher costs to the spoilers within the same resource as non-spoilers (scenario A, fN = fS ). Yet even under these circumstances, it is important to note that spoilers can only spread from rarity under a restricted set of conditions. Of course, as we have seen, given this selective neutrality of spoilers and non-spoilers within a fruit, then spoilers could in theory persist in any system where they were present, even in the absence of selective behaviour of larger animals. By quantifying and formalizing Janzen’s imaginative idea, we have argued that the spoiling of fruit, seeds and meat is unlikely to have arisen primarily as a mechanism to deter larger animals from consuming them. The theory simply will not work when, as might be expected, there is an individual cost to spoiling and microbes readily disperse. In the absence of such a cost, then spoilers will be able to persist even without the actions of a larger animal, and spread from rarity only under a limited set of conditions. Of course, one could argue that our model is abstract and simplistic, for instance, in considering only dichotomous strategies of “spoil” or “do not spoil”, and that more realistic sets of assumptions with continuous strategies would generate stronger support 626 T.N. Sherratt et al. / Ecological Modelling 192 (2006) 618–626 for Janzen’s idea. At very least we hope this work will stimulate those who advocate Janzen’s fascinating theory, to show just how such a hypothesis could be justified. Acknowledgement We thank NSERC (TNS) and the UK Royal Society (DMW) for funding and Rees Kassen and our anonymous referees for helpful comments. References Axelrod, R., Dion, D., 1988. The further evolution of cooperation. Science 242, 1385–1390. Borowicz, V.A., 1988. Do vertebrates reject decaying fruit? An experimental test with Cornus amomum fruits. Oikos 53, 74–78. Brown, S.P., 1999. Cooperation and conflict in host-manipulating parasites. Proc. R. Soc. B. 266, 1899–1904. Buchholz, R., Levey, D.J., 1990. The evolutionary triad of microbes, fruits and seed dispersers: an experiment in fruit choice by Ceder Waxwings Bombycilla cedrorum. Oikos 59, 200–204. Cockburn, A., 1991. An Introduction to Evolutionary Ecology. Blackwell Science, Oxford. Cipollini, M.L., Stiles, E.W., 1993. Fruit rot, antifungal defense and palitability of fleshy fruits for frugivorous birds. Ecology 74, 751–762. Dugatkin, L.A., 1997. Cooperation Among Animals: An Evolutionary Perspective. Oxford Series in Ecology and Evolution. Oxford University Press, Oxford. Ehlers, B.K., Olesen, J.M., 1997. The fruit-wasp route to toxic nectar in Epipactis orchids? Flora 192, 223–229. Fenchel, T., King, G.M., Blackburn, T.H., 1998. Bacterial Biogeochemistry, The Ecophysiology of Mineral Cycling. Academic Press, San Diego. Janzen, D.H., 1977. Why fruits rot, seeds mold, and meat spoils. Am. Nat. 111, 691–713. Janzen, D.H., 1979. Why food rots. Nat. Hist. Mag. 88, 60– 64. Madigan, M.T., Martinko, J.M., Parker, J., 2000. Biology of Microorganisms, ninth ed. Prentice Hall, NJ. Maynard Smith, J., 1998. Evolutionary Genetics, second ed. Oxford University Press, Oxford. Maynard Smith, J., Smith, N.H., O’Rourke, M., Spratt, B.G., 1993. How clonal are bacteria? Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. 90, 4384– 4388. Ruxton, G.D., Saravia, L.A., 1998. The need for biological realism in the updating of cellular automata models. Ecol. Mod. 107, 105–112. Simo, R., 2001. Production of atmospheric sulfur by oceanic plankton: Biogeochemical, ecological and evolutionary links. Trends Evol. Ecol. 16, 287–294. Stiling, P., 2002. Ecology: Theories and Applications, fourth ed. Prentice Hall, NJ. Wiener, P., 2000. Antibiotic production in a spatially structured environment. Ecol. Letts. 3, 122–130. Wilkinson, D.M., 1998. Fragments of an entangled bank: do ecologists study most of ecology? Oikos 82, 393–394.
© Copyright 2024