University of South Florida EDF 6211 Foundations of Educational Psychology Feral and Autistic Children: Integrating Disabled Children Who are Inept of Human Socialization & Development into Education and Society - A Literature Review Carolyn G. Ford Burac 7/26/2009 Abstract The issues of integrating socially inept children into the classrooms have been challenging for the students, teachers and parents alike, as well as understanding Autism Disorder and the Mowgli Syndrome on a human level remains a work in progress. Although the root cause of these two conditions is different, both are very much similar in nature regardless of the biological or environmental For the purpose of this literary review and simplicity, the term feral will be used instead of Mowgli Syndrome. Mowgli Syndrome is a term that is often given to children that are found with severe cognitive and/or physical deficiencies that are not the result of any biological cause, but rather are due to severe neglect. These children originally comprised two recognized categories: outcome of the children’s state. The purpose of 1. this paper is to explain the connection between 2. Victims of psychogenic dwarfism due to severe abuse and neglect by their parents or guardians, or Feral children, who are children that grew up outside of the influence of civilization. autism (Nature caused condition) and feral (Nurture caused condition) children by evaluating hypotheses, theories, and studies. Research will reveal that these socially developmental conditions in fact are only differentiated merely by cause. In addition, this paper will discuss how developmental disorders impact education on a global scale and the purposed solutions of integrating these students in education. The study of children reared in complete or nearly complete isolation from human socialization or born with developmental inflictions can provide important information to psychologists studying various aspects of socialization and education (Candland). Case in point, when children who were born healthy become feral (from abandonment and/or abuse) return to human society, they often continue to be seriously retarded, raising the question of whether or not such children (autistic and feral) with developmental abnormalities can receive both fair and equal educational efforts as well as improve their intellectual ability and function in a normal, civilized society. 2 Introduction From the early 1900s, autism has been referred as a range of several psychological conditions. The word “autism,” which has been in use for almost 100 years, describes conditions in which a person is removed from social interaction − hence, an isolated self (Williams). The autistic disorder was first described by Dr. Leo Kanner of the Johns Hopkins Hospital, who discovered eleven cases of a uniquely different “mental” disorder and created the new label ´early infantile autism´. (Please note that self isolation is the main symptom of autism.) Often, autism was confused with several other disorders which may have similar behaviors such as Heller's Disease, Rett's Syndrome, Tourette's Syndrome, and Asperger's Syndrome to name a few. However, neither the term feral nor the Mowgli Syndrome was ever mentioned to be a form of autism with the exception of the feralchildren.com website. The word feral comes from the Medieval Latin version of feralis, meaning uncultivated. A “feral” child is a human child who has lived isolated from human contact from a very young age, and has no (or little) experience of human care, loving or social behavior, and, crucially, of human language. In the U.S., the first pronounced studies of cognitive and behavioral development in human social interaction was presented in the early 1970s, when a social worker discovered a possibly autistic young teenager− Susan M.Wiley (commonly known as Genie). Later, it was revealed by authorities that Susan was a victim of abuse and had been severely neglected by her parents in their home. Causes of Developmental Disorders The Autism Society of America, ASA, purports that some researchers are investigating the possibility that under certain conditions, a cluster of unstable genes may interfere with brain development, resulting in autism. Still other researchers are investigating problems during pregnancy or delivery as well as environmental factors, such as viral infections, metabolic imbalances, and exposure to environmental chemicals. Nonetheless, there is no definitive answer on the single most accountable root of autism. One 3 of the main characteristics shared by feral children is that they lack memory and self-awareness (McCrone, London). Feral children are minimally exposed to human social interactions or they may experience complete isolation and confinement of human social interaction. Feral children, can be viewed through one of Charles Horton Cooley's theories on human interaction. Cooley, who lived in the late 1800s, created a theory that summed up how human development occurs, capturing the theory in the concept of 'the looking glass self'. This theory had three primary elements: we imagine how we appear to those around us, we interpret others' reactions, and we develop a self concept. The basic gist of it is that we look at those around us, and base our appearance and social interactions on what they do and what they expect. If an isolated child base his actions on other isolated individuals or no one, they will develop little or no basic interaction skill (Hehrer). Aside from the causes of both conditions, feral children and autistic children share the common theme of self isolation. Nature vs. Nurture: Language Language is the human use of spoken or written words as a communication system and it includes a system of communication based on signs, gestures, or inarticulate sounds. According to Lev S. Vygotsky, an educational psychologist, a clear understanding of the interrelations between thought and language is necessary for the understanding of intellectual development. Language is not merely an expression of the knowledge the child has acquired. "Language shapes the way we think, and determines what we can think about." (Benjamin Lee Whorf, Language, Thought, and Reality, 1964) There is a fundamental correspondence between thought and speech in terms of one providing resource to the other; language becoming essential in forming thought and determining personality features 4 (Vygotsky). Based on the work of Vygotsky, sociocultural theory presents the perspective that children’s cognitive structures are developed through the actions and speech of their caretakers and are transmitted through social interactions. It follows then that there will be culturally coded styles of speech and interaction, which will result in culturally related patterns of thought. There are five stages in language acquisition: cooing, babbling, one-word utterances, telegraphic speech and normal speech. The critical period hypothesis was proposed by linguist Eric Lenneberg in 1967. The Critical Period hypothesis states that the first few years of life constitute the time during which language develops readily and after which (sometime between birth and ending as early as age 4 and as late as early puberty) language acquisition is much more difficult and ultimately less successful (Siegler). This brief window of time for language acquisition also applies to the development of other attributes that separate humans from animals, such as abstract thinking. Depending on variables, this is why some researchers speculate that feral children may never acquire the ability to assimilate into society, while recent research has shown that when mothers follow their children’s focus during play interactions with their 4- and 5-year-old children with autism, later gains in social communication and language development are enhanced. However, at age 14, when the critical period should be finished, Susan Wiley made progress. She had learned how to feed herself, dress, act more appropriately in company, and remarkably learned many new words with the assistance of linguist Dr. Susan Curtiss. However, when the National Institute of Mental Health disqualified Susan’s research grant, her linguistic therapy stopped (Johnson). Susan’s development regressed severely, returning to her coping mechanism of silence, and adding a new fear of opening her mouth. The new fear of opening her mouth developed after being severely punished for vomiting in one of her foster homes; she didn't want to open her mouth, even to speak, for fear of vomiting and facing punishment again. With the exception of the unfortunate lack of funding for Susan’s case, research showed that it is possible to learn some language outside of the critical period, but also that syntax appears to have some privileged role. The amount of language that can be learned after the critical period seems very limited (Harley). 5 Social Behavior and Interaction Research suggests that it takes the interaction with other humans to develop a form of communication with any degree of complexity. Learned humans are the result of complex interactions between the environment and our genes. Feral children raised in isolation are found to be quite uncivilized and barely able to walk or talk. They are unable to empathize with of the needs and desires of other humans −they don't even identify themselves as human. The concepts of morals, property and possessions are alien to them. Many of them prove to be surly, uncooperative and self-centered individuals (Perry). Unlike feral children, whose condition resulted from abuse and neglect after birth by caregivers, autism disorder is present from birth or very early in development. Autism is a developmental disorder of neurobiological origin that is defined on the basis of behavioral and developmental features (Rumsey, Vitiello, Cooper, and Hirtz) and it affects essential human behaviors such as social interaction, the ability to communicate ideas and feelings, imagination, and the establishment of relationships with others. In addition, some of these symptoms are often common in feral children. Children who are autistic resist cuddling and holding, they prefer playing alone — retreats into his or her "own world", and may be unusually sensitive to light, sound and touch and yet oblivious to pain (Mayo). Interestingly, feral children also lack sensitivity to neurological stimuli. For example, it was reported by Marilyn Rigler (wife of psychologist, Dr. David Rigler), when Susan Wiley was asked to run her own shower water, she observed the water to be “Icy cold.” Mrs. Rigler stated to Susan, “Oh! This is so cold, you’re going to freeze.” Mrs. Rigler stated in a video documentary it was as though it didn’t make any difference to Susan about the temperature of the freezing water (NOVA). Autism-like conditions generally has life-long effects on how children learn to be social beings, to take care of themselves, and to participate in the community. 6 Obviously research studies in this field are of major importance to educators to whom knowledge of language, moral, and overall neurological development of students of all ages is of vital concern when designing educational programs and instructional techniques. Integrating Autistic Children in Education The global problem: Teacher Shortage for Special Education in America Nowadays, the only way of solving the problem of autism is rehabilitation, the aim of which is for an autistic person to achieve the best level of functioning in a society. Aside from the overall shortage of teachers looming over the heads of the educational system in America, recruiting qualified special education teachers seems to be as challenging as researching the causes of autism and treating autisticlike children. In a series of reports written for the National Commission on Teaching and America’s Future− Stanford University, Professor Linda Darling-Hammond found that even when newly certified teachers enter the workforce, they are frequently not certified in areas of greatest need, such as math, science, and special education (Chaika). The reality of the circumstances are that autistic children and their families live in every cornerstone of America− even rural, urban and inner-city communities, areas where there are already a plethora of significantly lower educational support, resources, funding, high student expectations and achievements. The National Commission on Teaching and America’s Future 2007 report estimates teacher attrition costs of over $7 billion each year in America’s public schools. This figure encompasses all aspects of finding, employing, and preparing new teachers to replace those who have left. Measuring the costs of teacher attrition in relation to the academic progress of public school students is much more difficult to assess, but no less significant. Low income and low achieving schools experience high teacher turnover rates. 7 Oftentimes teachers hired to replace those who have left are young, inexperienced, and may not be the most effective with high-need students (NCTAF). Other contributing factors of the shortage of special education teachers that aren’t mentioned are the increased requirements for earning special education teacher credentials while at the same time offering enhanced incentives to experienced teachers to elect early retirement. Special Education as a field has historically suffered from high rates of attrition, especially in categories serving students with significant needs, such as the seriously emotionally disturbed or developmentally challenged. The need for learning behavior specialists, speech-language pathologists, and special education technology specialists are in dire need. The goal for the U.S. is to establish a special education teacher campaign to offer incentives such as a full scholarship to college-bound high school students and bonuses to existing college students to enter into the field of special education. The global problem: Special Ed teachers' attitudes on autistic kids in Kuwait− − a case study A case study was conducted in Kuwait to examine special education teachers' attitudes toward autistic children in the Autism School in Kuwait, where they were employed. The outcome of the study revealed that there was a need for improvement in special education programming/curriculum & instruction, especially with regard to autism. It was determined that future Kuwaiti teachers and present teachers would benefit from both pre-service and in-service education. Since the diagnosis of autism is relatively new in Kuwait, teachers were not fully equipped with how to best teach students with autism. The goals of Kuwaiti teachers were to see their autistic students become a part of society; fluency in language could be a key factor in integrating children into society. As the result of the study, the researcher found that there was a need for a curriculum that is designed for autistic children. Also, based on the interviews of the study, special education teachers had positive attitudes toward students with autism and seek parental involvement to support their work (Al-Shammari). 8 The global problem: China’s struggle to integrate autistic children In China, a greater social attention to the education of children with special needs began to occur in the late 1970s, when President Xiaoping introduced the Reform and Open Door policy. Since, special education in China have experienced significant reform and fast development. However, education for children with severe developmental disabilities such as autism has been a great challenge. China has an estimated population of 1.3 billion people and approximately 1.95 million of those individuals have various functioning levels of autism. Since 80% of the Chinese population reside in rural areas and are economically challenged, about 75% of the 1.95 million individuals with autism have not yet been identified or properly diagnosed. Consequently, the education of children with autism has become a major concern in the field. Today, most school-age children with autism are still kept out of both regular and special schools for several reasons. First, approximately 75% of children with autism also have cognitive impairments (Gray). Secondly, special schools were initially established for children with physical, visual, hearing, and speech-related disabilities, or moderate cognitive impairments. Instead, children with autism are still being institutionalized and kept from social integration. As the practice of inclusion becomes a common in China, more educational opportunities have been established for children with autism (McCabe). China's educational system is standardized testing oriented, and teacher performance is evaluated based on students' academic performance. Under pressure, teachers are motivated to increase students' test scores (Deng & Manset). To facilitate the process of including more children with autism in regular schools, the State Department of Education decided to disregard the test scores of children with disabilities in the evaluation of general education and teacher performance. 9 Although this practice can promote regular schools' acceptance of more children with autism, it fails to ensure quality and appropriate education in China. In practice, students with disabilities, including autism, in regular schools use the same curriculum as typically developing students, but are exempt from taking tests designed for their typically developing peers (Sun). This practice not only impedes the proper documentation and assessment of their behavioral and academic progress, but it reveals the ignorance of autism disorder in both the educational field and society, implementing the practice of inclusion and training teachers and instructional designers to develop a specialized curriculum for autistic children. The global problem: Types of Instructional Strategies Naturalistic behavioral strategies are forms of discrete-trial teaching in which the child’s own motives or behavior initiate the instruction and lead to a reinforcing event (“natural reinforcer”). These approaches are more child-centered than massed trial teaching, in that children’s motivations, interests, favored activities, and choices figure strongly in the teaching. Two examples of naturalistic strategies are pivotal response training and incidental teaching. Incidental teaching consists of a chain of pre-specified child-tutor interactions. The interactions involve materials that are highly preferred by the child, prompting and shaping techniques embedded in natural contexts, and child-initiated (“natural”) interactions. Incidental teaching has been demonstrated, with replication, to be an effective technique for increasing language learning in both typical children (Hart and Risley) and in children with autism (McGee et al.). In pivotal response training (Koegel et al.), certain behaviors are seen as central to wide areas of functioning. Changing these pivotal behaviors is thought to change other associated behaviors without specifically targeting the associated behaviors. Pivotal response techniques include child choice, reinforcement, and correcting behaviors. 10 Peer-mediated strategies (Strain and Kohler) also demonstrate a naturalistic application of behavioral teaching. The typical peers of a child with an autistic spectrum disorder are instructed in a more adultcentered, mass-trial approach, while children with autistic spectrum disorders are taught by their peers in a more child-centered, naturalistic type of approach. Conclusion Because autism is not a single ailment but a complex collection of behaviors, it is extremely difficult to bridge the educational gap between the learning needs of autistic & feral children and school preparedness. Although autism have been discovered for almost a century and studies of feral children have been established for centuries, the modern world is still foreign to developmental abnormalities in children. Whatever the cause of biological or environmental autism, there is no doubt that autism poises an extreme impact on public schools all over the globe. In addition, even if an individualized education program were to be implemented, it is only as good as the teacher. It is quite apparent that teachers need continuous specialized training in providing an effective learning environment for these children. A report released by the Journal of Autism Research showed that a follow-up study by University of Utah psychiatry researchers of 41 Utahans diagnosed with an autism disorder 20 years ago shows that they have generally been more independent and more social, and a few even showed increases in IQs. If the government and researchers put the attention on autism as it deserves, perhaps educators and parents would have a better idea on early intervention and treatment. In addition, psychologists would also have a better idea on how to help the autism in even more rare cases of feral children like Danielle Crockett (Lierow), a 6 year old abandoned child kept in confinement since birth in Plant city, Florida. 11 Resources: 1. Candland, D. K., (1993). Feral Children and Clever Animals: Reflections on Human Nature. Retrieved July 23, 2009, New York: Oxford University Press 2. Williams Jr., Robert (2000). Health24 News: “Autism through Ages Baffles Science.” Retrieved July 23, 2009, Pediatric Services 3. McCrone, J., and London, M., (1993). The Myth of Irrationality - the science of the mind from Plato to Star Trek Retrieved July 22, 2009 4. Hehrer, H., (2009). Nature Vs Nurture - A Sociological Approach to Feral, Isolated, and Institutionalized Children. Retrieved July 25, 2009, from http://ezinearticles.com 5. Vygotsky, L. S., (1985). Thought and Language. Retrieved July 24, 2009, Cambridge, MA: The M.I.T. Press 6. Perry, B.D., (2001). The euro-archeology of childhood maltreatment: the neuro-developmental costs of adverse childhood events Retrieved July 24, 2009 7. Mayo Clinic Staff, (2008). Autism: Symptoms. Retrieved July 26, 2009, from Mayo Foundation for Medical Education & Research Web site: http://www.mayoclinic.com 8. Cronkite,W., (1994). The Secret Wild Child. Retrieved July 02, 2009, DVD format, Boston: NOVA Star of Science Television/WGBH, Released 2007 9. Rumsey J.M., Vitiello, B., Cooper, J., and Hirtz, D., (2000). Special issue: Treatments for people with autism and other pervasive developmental disorders: Research perspectives Retrieved July 25, 2009 (Editorial preface.− Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders 30(5):369–371.) 10. Siegler, R., (2006). How Children Develop, Exploring Child Develop Student Media Tool Kit & Scientific American Reader to Accompany How Children Develop. Retrieved July 25, 2009, New York: Worth Publishers 11. Johnson, W., (2007). Genie: The Wild Child. Retrieved July 25, 2009, from Knol Web site: http://www.knol.google.com 12. Harley, T.A., (2001). Psychology of Language: From Data to Theory, 2nd edition. Retrieved July 25, 2009New York: Psychology Press 13. Chaika, G., (2001). Scrambling for Staff: The Teacher Shortage in Rural Schools. Retrieved July 26, 2009, from Education World Web site: http://www.educationworld.com/a_admin/admin/admin142.shtml 14. National Commission on Teaching and America’s Future. (2007).The high cost of teacher turnover (Policy Brief). Retrieved July 26, 2009, from NCTAF Web site: http://www.nctaf.org 15. Al-Shammari, Z., (2006). Special education teachers' attitudes toward autistic students in the autism school in the State of Kuwait: a case study. Retrieved July 26, 2009, Journal of Instructional Psychology 16. Gray, D. E. (1998). Autism and the family: Problems, prospects, and coping with the disorder. Retrieved July 26, 2009, Springfield, IL: Charles C Thomas Publisher. 12 17. McCabe, H. (2003). The beginnings of inclusion in the People's Republic of China. Retrieved July 26, 2009, Research and Practice for Persons with Severe Disabilities, 28(1), 16-22. 18. Deng, M., & Manset, G., (2000). Analysis of the "learning in regular classrooms" movement in China. Retrieved July 26, 2009, Mental Retardation, 38(2), 124-130. 19. Sun, J., (1990). The experiment of learning in regular classrooms for the blind. Retrieved July 26, 2009, Special Education in China, 2, 30-37. 20. Hart, B., and Risley, T.R., (1975). Incidental teaching of language in the preschool. Retrieved July 26, 2009, Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis 8(4):411–420. 21. McGee, G.G., Morrier, M.J., and Daly, T., (1999). An incidental teaching approach to early intervention for toddlers with autism. Retrieved July 26, 2009, Journal of the Association for the Severely Handicapped 24:133–146. 22. Koegel, L.K., Koegel, R.L., Harrower, J.K., and Carter, C.M., (1999). Pivotal response intervention I: Overview of approach. Retrieved July 26, 2009, Journal of the Association for the Severely Handicapped 24:174–185. 23. Strain, P.S., and Kohler, F.W., (1998). Peer-mediated social intervention for young children with autism. Retrieved July 26, 2009, Seminars in Speech and Language 19:391–405. 13
© Copyright 2024