Irish National Teachers' Organisation Early Childhood Education Report of a Seminar .Pubiished by the Irish National Teachers' Organisation. 35 Parnell Square, Dublin 1, © INTO. 7983 Origination by Healyset. Dublin 1. Printed by Oval Printing Company, Dublin 7. Contents Page Introduction 4 Trends in early childhood education 5 Thomas Kel/aghan Education and the phychological and physical development of young children 23 Anne T. McKenna Education for young children: infant classes in primary schools 35 Siobhtm M. Hurley Early education for children with special needs 47 Anne O'Sul/ivan Home and school in the education of the young child 57 Elizabeth McGovern Preschools, playschools, nursery schools and classes 69 Kathleen Day Appendix 78 Introduction In August 1981 the then Minister for Education, Mr. John Boland TD, decided to raise the age of entry to primary schools and thereby he initiated, albeit inadvertently, a public debate about early childhood education. The Executive Committee of the Irish National Teachers' Organisation decided, as part of its contribution to the debate, to hold a seminar on this important educational and social problem. The seminar, which was held in Mary Immaculate College of Education in Limerick in June 1982, was attended by more than 150 people. The participants heard an interesting and stimulating series of lectures and contributed to the success of the seminar through their involvement in group discussions on various aspects of the central theme. This report contains the text of the lectures delivered at the seminar. It is intended to publish separately a supplement giving a summary of the group discussions. The Executive Committee would like to record its appreciation and gratitude to all those who contributed to the success of the seminar and in particular to the lecturers, chairpersons and rapporteurs and to the Principal and staff of Mary Immaculate College of Education. E. G. Quigley, General Secretary February, 1983 Trends in early childhood education Thomas Kellaghan Director, Educational Research Centre St. Patrick's College, Dublin The author is indebted to Mary Campbell for assistance in the preparation of this paper. The care and, in the broadest sense, the education of the young is, and always was, one of the most basic of human tasks. On it depends not only the survival of individuals but the survival of the race. Further, differences in style and practice in child care seem to affect the psychological characteristics of children throughout their lives as well as the characteristics of the society of which they form a part. Almost invariably across all societies, the care of the infant during the first year or two of its life is primarily the responsibility of its mother. After that, practice varies and other individuals or institutions may begin to take over some aspects ofthe care and education of the child. In many countries in western society, there has been an increase in recent years in the availability of resources outside the home to assist in the early phases of the education of children. This can be seen in general terms as an extension of national educational enterprises which began in the last century and have been steadily expanding ever since, though there are also more specific reasons for the intense interest in early childhood education and the expansion of facilities which were features of the 1970s (ct. European Economic Community, 1979; van der Eyken, 1982; Woodhead, 1979). The tasks facing the child in the first five or six years of its life are not inconsiderable and the accomplishments of the vast majority of children during these years are impressive. However, the achievements of some are not satisfactory and when we consider the range of tasks facing the child, that is hardly surprising. These tasks include meeting organic needs and establishing routine habits (such as eating, sleeping, and washing): learning motor skills and 5 associated confidences (such as walking, running, and climbing): developing manipulatory skills (building with blocks, beads, tying things, buttoning one's coat); learning control and restraints (listening to stories, sitting still); developing social behaviour (sharing with others, developing appropriate dependent and independent behaviour); emotional development (coping with fear, aggression, anger, frustration, guilt; psycho-sexual development (identification, sex-role learning); language development; and intellectual development covering a broad spectrum of activities from preceptual discrimination to concept formation (Sears' Dowley, 1963). Despite the fact that early child development has become a major focus of attention and policy formation in recent years many people still seem uncertain about the role of educational provision in aiding that development. There has, for example, been a cut-back in preschool provision for disadvantaged children in the United States. In this country, as recently as 1979, a former secretary of the Department of Education claimed that some teachers would regard four-year old entrance to school as 'an extravangantly costly babysitting service' (0 Conchobhair, 1979), while more recent government decisions about the age of entry to school must at least raise questions about the understanding of and commitment to early childhood education among teachers, politicians, and administrators. These statements and actions suggest that the value of early childhood education is not universally accepted. Indeed not only in this country, but throughout the world, education at this level ranks below elementary, secondary, and third-level education in priority(cf. Psacharopoulos, 1980). And yet, a number of changes and developments in recent years has ensured that politicians, administrators, and educators can no longer afford to ignore the area of early childhood education. Some of these changes and developments have occurred in the family and in society; others are the result of a renewed interest in the study of early development. Among changing family conditions is an increase in the number of single-parent families and of mothers working outside the home, either for economic reasons or for personal fulfilment. Thus the major caretaker is no longer available in many families on a full-time basis to look after the young child. Further, other adults (grandparents, aunts) who often formed an extended family are becoming less a feature of the family circle, particularly in urban areas. Households are getting smaller and there are fewer people to share in looking after young children (Hunt, 1970). This is happening at a time when there is growing appreciation of the importance of the early childhood years for the development of the child. Emphasis on the importance of early childhood is not of course new; the early experiences of the child have been regarded as crucial in theories of child development, particularly ones relating to personality. What is new is the attempt to translate into educational practice on an extensive scale the belief that environmental factors 6 exercise considerable influence on the development of perceptual and higher cognitive skills in children before the age of five. Such educational practice is also supported by the belief that if certain aspects of development do not take place at a certain time, they may not take place at all or at least later development will be seriously impaired. Much of the rationale for these views has its basis in the work of Donald Hebb and his colleagues at McGill University which was carried out in the 1 940s and 1950s. A series of experimental studies with animals (rats, dogs, and chimpanzees) examined the effect of deprivation on perceptual skills and on 'intelligence' or problemsolving ability. In general, the studies indicated that the performance of animals that had been deprived of certain types of experience early in life was seriously impaired at a later date. On the 'basis of such studies, Hebb (1949) argued that a great deal of perceptual learning is necessary before we see the world as the normal adult sees it. For example, he disputed the view, associated with Gestalt psychology, that the human infant immediately perceives a square shape as a unified structure or whole. By his second year, the child may do so, but this is the result of a vast number of visual experiences and muscular explorations, and it is in the combinafion of these that a shape gets its consistent structure. The popularization of this work in the writings of Bruner (1961) and Hunt (1961), received considerable attention. Interpreting the work, Bruner (1961) concluded that an environment that is impoverished (that is, one that is monotonous with limited opportunities for different kinds of stimulation, discrimination, and manipulation) 'produces an adult organism with reduced abilities to discriminate, with stunted strategies for coping with roundabout solutions, with less taste for exploratory behaviour, and with a notably reduced tendency to draw inferences that serve to cement the disparate events of its environment' (p. 199). Around the same time, the importance of early childhood received further support in Bloom's (1964) conclusions following reanalysis of data collected in longitudinal studies of the development of children. His finding that scores on intelligence tests at the age of five or six correlated highly with scores at the age of 171ed him to conclude that a considerable amount of intellectual development took place in the preschool years and that the pattern for later development was well established by the time the child began formal schooling. All this might have remained of academic interest only and might have had little impact on educational practice if it had not happened at a time when Americans were looking critically at the performance of their schools and particularly at the role of schools in dealing with problems of poverty. Concern was being expressed at the poor attainment and early drop-out of children from low socio-economic status homes and the possible loss of talent which this involved. This concern seemed to fit in well with another dominant theme of the 7 1960s - that of equality of opportunity. For a number of reasons, educational reform was perceived as the main avenue through which more general social reform could be attained (Madaus, Airasian, & Kellaghan, 1980). The United States Congress turned to 'compensatory' preschool education as the major means of attaining equality. This appro\lch was predicated in part on the belief that since intelligence was malleable in the preschool years, education could have a large impact if children from disadvantaged backgrounds were 'treated' early in their educational careers. The now well-known Head Start programme, designed to provide health, nutritional, day-care, and educational services, set out to overcome the educational deficiencies which children from poor backgrounds manifested when beginning school by providing preschool experiences for these children. It was believed, and this reflects long-cherished American beliefs about the utility of schooling, that children from 'high-risk' families, if they were given the resources and the opportunities, would succeed in school and as adults in later life. This early education was seen as being a major factor in breaking the cycle of underachievement and poverty in American society. In this, a tradition in the use of early childhood education as an instrument of progressive reform was revived. In attempts to deal with problems caused by industrialization and urbanization towards the end of the last century, preschool programmes were established in all major American cities, first on a voluntary basis and later under public sponsorship (Lazerson, 1972). The crusade of the 1960s looked like a revival of the efforts of the 1890s, but on a grander scale. However, the roots of early childhood education outside the home are to be found in Europe, not in America. In tracing those roots, one is brought back to pioneers in the championing of children'S rights, such as Comenius (1592-1670) and Rousseau (1712-1778), whose writings did much to promote the concept of childhood and rights of children. Early childhood education in a communal setting actually took form mainly through the efforts of Froebel (1782-1852) and, at a later stage, those of Montessori and the McMillan sisters. The systems they established flourished throughout this century and, despite a number of challenges, still exercise considerable influence today (cf. Weber, 1971). Here we may pause briefly to consider some of the nomenclature that has emerged from early childhood education movements. A large variety of terms has, and still is, being used to describe institutions which cater for early childhood education. We cannot hope to sort out fully the meanings ofthese terms which sometimes vary from country to country or over time in their meaning. Further, anyone term can be used to describe institutions in which a variety of educational practices may flourish. Since a good deal of early childhood education has been and continues to be carried out on a voluntary basis, it is not surprising that there is a lack of uniformity in terminology. 8 Some of the terms which are used to describe educational facilities up to about the age of six are playschool, day nursery, daycare centre or group daycare, family daycare, creche, nursery school, nursery class, academic preschool, shared-rearing preschool, kindergarten, and infant school. Some of the terms emphasize a caring or custodial function. Much daycare (family daycare) takes place in the child's own home with hired minders. In other cases, institutional arrangements are provided. Daycare was the term given to facilities designed to 'warehouse' children in the United States while their mothers worked during the second world war. On the other hand, the term academic preschool suggests a serious educational function and such institutions are often a downward extension of the primary school and may focus on the development of 'school readiness' skills. What goes on in a nursery school is not immediately obvious from ttie name, It is a term particularly associated with English education and has been used to describe facilities for children around the ages ofthree orfour. The first kindergarten was established in 1837 by Froebel, who rejected terms which used the word school, such as infant school and nursery school (Lawrence, 1952); he selected the work kindergarten because it embodied his view of child development as something reflecting the development of plants in a garden. What goes on in an infant sChool is not immediately obvious from its name either. The one thing you will not normally find insuch a school is an infant, The first infant school has been attributed to Robert Owen (established in 1816) and in a number of ways its activities reflected the Froebel philosophy of child development. Both kindergarten and infant school are often used to describe facilities for children who have completed nursery-school or who are in the junior grades of primary school. In this paper when we talk about early childhood education, we will for the most part be talking about arrangements in a group setting away from the child's home that are made for children under the statutory school-entry age. Further, our concern will be more with provision for older children (between the age of three and the age of compulsory schooling) than with provision for younger children, though some consideration will have to be given to the latter since in some countries a distinction is not made between the two kinds of provision. In the case of Ireland, we shall be talking about children who are in schools which also cater for older children: mainly, we shall be concerne.d with children who are in what are called junior and senior infant classes. In other countries, the facilities for children of this age are often separated from those for older children. Our consideration will focus more on publicly provided facilities than on privately provided ones. Many early childhood facilities, especially those for young children, are private and not a great deal is known about them. It is difficult to obtain accurate information on the nature or extent of such faCilities or on the numbers of children who participate in them. In the official statistics supplied for participation for some countries, it is not always clear whether they include children attending private as well as public institutions. 9 Models of early childhood education Of greater importance than the labels one might attach to early childhood education are the characteristics of that education. In keeping with the multiplicity of labels that exist. one also finds a multiplicity of institutional arrangements and practices. Not only does one find a great variety in the preschool provision that exists throughout the world, one finds variety even within individual countries. A major attempt to categorize systems of provision throughout the industrialized world has been made by Robinson, Robinson, Darling, & Holm (1979) who have described the features of a Latin-European model (particularly as exemplified in France and Belgium), a Scandinavian model, a Socialist model (particularly as exemplified in the Soviet Union), and an Anglo-Saxon model (as found in the United States, Britain, and Canada). While the categorization is not entirely satisfactory, we may take it as a basis for considering a number of dimensions along which approaches to early childhood education vary. A consideration of these dimensions will provide some feel forthe range of philosophies and practices which operate in this area of education. Vertical organization First we may note that in many courtries an organizational distinction is made between facilities provided for children up to the age of two and a half or three, in what Robinson et al (1979) call creches, and facilities provided for children from that age upto the age of compulsory schooling, which may vary between five (in the United Kingdom) and seven (in Denmark); the latter facilities are called kindergartens by Robinson at al. The Scandinavian countries do not have this distinction. They have a unified system of preschool education from the age of six months to seven years. There is also a . trend in the Soviet Union to move to a unified 'nursery-kindergarten' system; the reason given is that in separate facilities, there is too much emphasis on physical care to the exclusion of the cognitive, social, and emotional development of the children. National policies for families The national policy for families in a country can have important implications for the provision of early education facilities, particularly for who provides it, who controls what goes on in the schools or centres, and what role is assigned to parents. In the matter of family policy, a distinction may be drawn between Socialist and Scandinavian countries on the one hand and Anglo-Saxon ones on the other, with the Latin countries somewhere in between. In the Socialist and Scandinavian countries, child rearing is seen as something to be shared by family and state. In the Anglo-Saxon countries, child rearing is primarily a family function. which in certain circumstances may be aided by the state. Bronfenbrenner (1970) has 10 drawn the distinction sharply in his discussion of upbringing in the Soviet Union and the United States. While the Soviet system recognizes that parents have a.uthority, this authority is only a reflection of that of the state. Such a position would not generally be accepted in the Anglo-Saxon world. In this context the provisions of Article 42 of the Irish Constitution come to mind; the article recognizes 'the primary and natural educator of the child' to be the family and the state 'guarantees to respect the inalienable right and duty of parents to provide ... for ... the education of their children'. Parents are free to provide education 'in their homes or in private schools recognized or established by the state'. While the Constitution grants to parents the primary moral and legal responsibility of bringing up their children, in this country as elsewhere, there are pressures which are making it increasingly difficult for parents to do that. The result is a growing ·demand for outside services, including state ones, even in countries where traditional family policy has emphasized the primacy of parents. Control of child care Closely related to national family policy is the control of child care. In general, Socia list economies are associated with standardization of programmes while free enterprise ones tend to encourage diversification (Peters, 1980). While tight control is exercised by the central authorities on school curricula and there is a high degree of uniformity in educational practice in all schools in socialist countries, there is also some diversification. In the Soviet Union, for example, both creches and kindergartens are sometimes run by factories, farms, and other institutions to provide facilities for employees. In other cases, they are run by a government ministry (Grant, 1964). In Scandinavian and Latin countries, the central government controls general policy, but programmes may be administered and operated locally. France, for example, does not provide a standard curriculum for kindergartens (teachers work with inspectors in doing this), while in Scandinavian countries, programme guidelines are provided by the National Board of Health and Welfare, but teachers may adapt these to their own circumstances and needs. The least amount of central control occurs in Anglo-Saxon countries; while sectors of early childhood provision in these countries have some centrally organized features, these occur in the context of considerable decentralization. Central control is exercised by a variety of government ministries. In Scandinavian countries, it is pxercised by Departments of Social Affairs, Social Welfare, and Hea"n. In other countries, the tendency is for a Ministry of Health or Welfare to exercise control over institutions attended by children up to about the age of three, while a Ministry of Education or Public Instruction is responsible for institutions attended by children from the age of three onwards. In all the European Community countries, with the exception of Denmark, the responsibility for the education of children over the age offour, and in 11 some cases for children as young as two (France). two and a half (Belgium), and three (Italy, United Kingdom), lies with the Ministry of Education (European Economic Community, 1979). Curriculum objectives and content Perhaps the area in which there has been the greatest debate over early childhood education has not related to its' organization or control but to the curricula which are followed. There are two basic dimensions along which curricula can be differentiated (Stodolsky, 1972). One relates to curriculum objectives and the second relates to the structure of the curriculum. The major distinction that is usually drawn regarding objectives is that between curricula or programmes which emphasize intellectual-scholastic goals and those which emphasize socio-emotional ones. We shall return to this distinction later; at this point, we will look at the emphasis of the various systems on these two sets of goals. In general, the Anglo-Saxon and Scandinavian systems have emphasized socio-emotional development throughout the whole period of early childhood education. In Latin countries, health, hygiene, and socio-emotional development are emphasized during the early years of life in creches, while the kindergarten attends more to cognitive goals and in particular to the preparation of children for formal schooling. The Socialist countries seems to have adopted a more mixed approach. From an early age, a heavy emphasis is placed on social·aspects of development; experience in 'collective' living is provided almost from birth so that the child will develop an awareness of the group or collective and its needs. Attention is also paid to language development, to some extent as a vehicle for developing social behaviour, to cognitive growth, to creativity, and to school readiness. Attention to the cognitive aspects of development is receiving increasing emphasis, even with very young children. Curriculum structure The second dimension on which curricula and programmes may be differentiated is structure. In a structured curriculum, the teacher emphasizes specific goals. Further, the pursuit of those goals is adhered to by allocating specific times for relevent activities, by providing suitable materials, and by prescribing children's responses. By comparison with unstructured approaches, the structured one is more teacher-directed and more homogeneous in its activities. Unstructured curricula are sometimes called traditional, childcentred, or discovery curricula. The child selects its own activities rather than being directed towards a particular activity by its·teacher. Though such curricula are often termed child-centred by comparison with more structured approaches, they are child-centred only insofar as the selection of activities is concerned. A structured curriculum, though its main focus is on the content being presented to the child, could be child-centred to the extent that thc choice of content 12 (materials and activities) is based on developmental principles and related to individual differences between children (Kellaghan, 1977). The highest structure in early childhood education is associated with the Socialist model. In this model, detailed plans of activities for children at different age levels are laid down and systematic teaching programmes are used. A core principle ofthese programmes is the socalled regime. Each child, as Bronfenbrenner (1970) describes it. is on a series of reinforcement schedules. The teacher or 'upbringer' spends a specified amount of time with each child, stimulating and training sensory-motor functions. "For example, at the earliest age Isvels, shewill present a brightly colored object, moving it to and fro to encourage fo(.fowing. A bit later, the object is brought nearer and moved slowly forward to induce the infant to move toward it. Still later, the child is motivated to pull himself up by the barred sides of the playpen to assume a standing position. And infants learn to stand in such playpens not only in Moscow, but 2,000 miles away in Soviet Asia as well" (Bronfenbrenner, 1970, p. 17). By contrast, Anglo-Saxon and Scandinavian early childhood education (with the possible exception of Finnish education which is moving toward the development of more structured programmes) consists mostly of supportive, unstructured, socialization programmes rather than structured informational ones. Such programmes have been described as providing. "a warm accepting atmosphere in which a child may achieve his own maximum social and physical development, and an ordered atmosphere in which selected equipment and activities are offered in sufficient variety to meet each child's level of interest and ability" (Gordon & Wilkenson, 1966, p. 48). Early childhood education in Latin countries may be regarded as occupying an intermediate position in structure between the Socialist model on the one hand and the Anglo-Saxon and Scandinavian models on the other. The kind of objectives a curriculum has and the degree of structure associated with it are not necessarily related. For example, one could have a highly structured curriculum designed to attain socioemotional objectives and there is some evidence in the Socialist model of early childhood education that structure is a feature of programmes designed to achieve a variety of objectives. In practice, however, it is easier to prescribe a structure for programmes with cognitive objectives and the best known examples of highly structured curricula in the United States were concerned with language development (Bereiter & Engelmann, 1966; Lavate"i, 1972) and general cognitive development (Lavatelli, 1970). 13 Preparation of teachers or upbringers There is SDme variatiDn, nDt .only acrDSS mDdels, but even within cDuntries, in the methDds and standards emplDyed in the preparatiDn .of teachers Dr 'upbringers' tD use the mDre general term. Even in the SDcialist cDuntries, where .one might expect the highest level .of prDfessiDnal training and unifDrmity, .one finds diversity. In PDland, fDr example, .one may take either a tWD-year vDcatiDnal cDurse Dr a threeyear university CDurse, while in the SDviet UniDn, SDme take CDurses in teacher training cDlleges and .others in specialized secDndary SChDDls. In the EurDpean CDmmunity, there are five cDuntries in which the preparatiDn and status .of persDnnel wDrking in early childhDDd educatiDn are basically the same as the preparatiDn and status .of teachers in elementary SChDDls: France, Ireland, Italy, LuxembDurg, and the United KingdDm. In tWD cDuntries, the preparatiDn and status of early childhDDd educatDrs have been traditiDnally IDwer, but these are In the prDcess .of being raised: Belgium and the Netherlands.lnthe Federal Republic .of Germany, the teacher .of preschDDI children is almDst equal in status and preparatiDn tD the nDrmal teacher, while preschDDI assistants are IDwer. Only in Denmark, amDng E.E.C. cDuntries, is the preparatiDn and status .of the teacher .of preschDDI children definitely IDwer (Elvin, 1981; European ECDnDmic CDmmunity, 1979). Participation Finally in IODking at vanatlOn in early childhoDd educatiDn provisiDn, we may cDnsider participatiDn. MDSt Dfthe figures we have relate tD E.E.C. cDuntries. BefDre cDnsidering these figures, we may nDte that participatiDn even in SDcialist cDuntries is nDt particularly high. FDr example, it is estimated that abDut 10% .of children between six weeks and three years attend creches in the U.S.S.R. while abDut 50% between the ages .of three and seven years attend kindergarten. The figures fDr the U.S.A. are nDt very dissimilar. Asmaller prDpDrtiDn attend creches while the figure fDr children aged three tD six years attending kindergarten is abDut the same (50%) as in Russia (RDbinsDn et aI, 1979). AmDng E.E.C. cDuntries the highest participatiDn rates are tD be fDund in Belgium, France, LuxembDurg, and the Netherlands. FDr example, 90% .of three-year Dlds and 97% .of fDur-year Dlds attend a preschDDI in ~rllgium. Practically all fDur-year Dlds attend such an establishment in France (97%). LuxembDurg (93%). and the Netherlands (93%). LDwest participatiDn rates are fDund fDr the United KingdDm (40% .of fDur-year Dlds, but all five-year Dlds) and Denmark (60% .of five and six-year Dlds,). In Ireland. 65% .of fDur-year Dlds and 95% .of five-year Dlds attend primary SChDDI (EurDpean ECDnDmic CDmmunity, 1979). 14 Controversy on objectives and structure of early childhood education. Controversy on content and structure has been a feature of early childhood education for a long time. A search for its roots brings us back to two basically different traditions and sets of assumptions regarding the nature of childhood and of development. One is the Enlightenment tradition which regards education as the path to righteousness and rationality. If one accepts this view, then an emphasis on school-related skills and preparation for life is indicated. The other tradition is the Romantic one which views childhood as a period that is important in its own right and not just as a preparation for adulthood; further, children are naturally good and their instincts, emotions, and feelings should be allowed full expression: Given this view of childhood, educational provision should avoid imposing on the child objectives and structures, particularly ones of a cognitive nature. Rousseau (1712-1778). who applied the Romantic ideas to education, postulated that development is made up of a series of internally regulated stages which are transformed one into another. It follows a regular order based on maturation and external influences should not be allowed to interfere with it. In applying these beliefs to educational practice, the pioneers of early childhood education, Pestalozzi (1746-1827) and Froebel (1782-1852), stressed the importance of the child's own contribution to development and to the need to provide a noncoercive environment in which development could take place in a natural way. Towards the end of the last century and into the beginning of the present one, a debate on the objectives and teaching approaches in preschools took place which was very similar to the debate of the 1960s and 1970s on the same issues (Evans, 1975). For most of this century, Dewey's views prevailed and the practice of American and English early childhood education emphasized social and emotional development in an informal unstructured environment. Even Froebel's approach was regarded as too formal and structured and as failing to provide for the child's individuality. While Montessori's philosophy and methods were more in tune with the nondirective influences which were favoured inAmerican classrooms, they did not support America's commitment to play, imagination, creativity, and self-expression, or to the importance of socialization and group activities (Lazerson, 1972). Dewey's position received support from Arnold Gesell whose theory of maturation, like that of Rousseau, emphasized the role of internal regulatory mechanisms on development. Gesell's work on developmental characteristics suggested a more or less consistent pattern in all children duri ng the first five years. Differences in growth between children were attributed to original capacity, rate, or tempo, 15 and to patterns of developmental organization. Environmental factors could support, inflect, and modify development, but they did not generate the progress of development (cf. Gesell, Amatruda, Castner, & Thompson, 1939). These views became known to vast numbers of people throughout the world through the writings of Benjamin Spock. An important educational concept in Gesell's developmental theory was that of readiness. A child will walk when his muscles have matured sufficiently to allow him to do so and similarly he will read when the relevant perceptual and motor abilities have matured. Much educational practice with young children was based on the concept of readiness. Readiness tests were often administered to determine whether or not reading should be taught. Trying to push development, it was thought, would at best do no good and at worst might do harm. If readiness is something determined by the child's intrinsic rate of development, then academic pressure would add 'burdensome pressures upon the child' at a time when the preschool should be fostering self-expression and creativity (Butler, 1970). 'Another feature of traditional early childhood education practice was the relatively little attention that was paid to cognitive development. While this aspect of growth was not completely ignored and while in some programmes language development received considerable attention, the main thrust of most nursery schools was on the social and emotional development of children. Further, most nursery school teachers preferred a curriculum based largely on children's own choice of activity ratherthan one that was planned and structured by the teacher (Taylor, Exon, & Holley, 1972). Gesell's maturational theory and the tradition in early childhood education which it supported had considerable appeal. It fitted into a well-defined tradition of human development, associated with names such as Rousseau, Darwin, and G. Stanley Hall, and there was empirical evidence to support some aspects of it. There were a number of problems associated with it in practice, however. One was that children were not always learning when the maturationalists said they should be readyto(Goodlad, Klein, Novotney, 1973) andthis became particularly obvious when educational and political attention was focussed on children living in disadvantaged areas in the 1960s. Another problem was that where good environmental conditions prevailed in the homes of children, one might not be over-concerned about the relative contributions of environmental and maturational factors to development but where environmental conditions were not good and children on entering school at the age of five or six were already considerably behind their peers in scholastic skills, one had to give further thought to the role of those environmental factors and the possibilities of preventive action. The reaction to the role of the traditional nursery school in helping children from disadvantaged backgrounds meet the requirements of school was perhaps most forcibly put in recent years by Bereiter & Englemann 11966). They argued that the traditional nursery school 16 was not suitable for the disadvantaged because most aspects of such schools complemented the activities of middle-class homes and indeed in ways resembled the activities of a lower-class environment. (For example, the middle class home is rich in verbal experience, the nursery school, like the lower-class home, stresses seeing and doing.) What is needed in the preschool for the disadvantaged are activities that complement the activities of the disadvantaged home and are similar to those of middle-class homes. Further, there is little time in which to help the disadvantaged catch up with the privileged; hence an intensive and selective scheme is required. In the Bereiter & Engelmann approach, the focus is strictly on academic objectives. Since the disadvantaged child's major deficiency is seen to be a language one, a programme designed to teach 'middly-class language' is provided. ' Bereiter & Engelm'ann were not the only commentators to draw a sharp distinction between the objectives and practices of traditional nursery schools or 'shared rearing preschools' and academic preschools. The former were interpreted as providing basic support services to mothers in the rearing of their children during the preschool years in 'a secure benign environment that is compatible with the interests and predispositions of the young child' (Blank, 1974), while the latter were seen to be primarily concerned with the preparation of children for the tasks of formal schooling. No doubt this distinction is exaggerated to point up differences in emphasis between approaches to early childhood education - one non-structured, stressing social and emotional development, the other highly structured with an emphasis on cognitive development. In practice, one suspects that many preschool educational programmes attend to a range of objectives and involve some degree of structure. But the distinction has been of value iffor no other reason than that it forced people involved in early childhood education to examine some of the assumptions on which their practice was based. We have, of course, as well as philosophical and practical positions about child development, a growing area of empirical knowledge about such development and about early childhood education (ct. McKenna, 1979). Models of child development have attended to both cognitive aspects and socio-emotional ones. The former owe much to Piaget's work and have tended to adopt a general structural and model-building view of development (Kamii, 1972; Kellaghan, 1977; Kohlberg, 1968; Lavatelli, 1970; Sigel, 1972; Weikart, Rogers, Adcock, & McClelland, 1971). Their influence has been greater in the field of early childhood education than have approaches which have emphasized socio-emotional development. However, psychological approaches which give greater recognition to the role of socioemotional factors in development have also found applic~tion in early childhood education programmes. For example, a d6velopmental approach, drawing both on the psychological work of Erikson and Werner and the progressive education movement. has been 17 developed at the Bank Street College of Education (Biber, 1977). This approach has stressed the importance of the development of 'a sense of trustfulness in others and trustworthiness in one's self; a sense of autonomy through making choices and exercising control; a sense of initiative expressed in a variety of making, doing, and play activities in cooperation with others and in an imagined projection of the adult sex role' (Franklin & Biber, 1977, p.18). Whatever philosophical or practical position one adopts, empirical knowledge on child development can hardly be ignored by the practicioner or poicy maker in the field of education. This knowledge makes the practice of preschool education at the same time easier and more difficult. It makes it easier in that clearer guidelines are now available about the needs and capabilities of individual children and about programme content and structure. And it makes it more difficult in the diagnosis of children's needs and capabilities and the provision of appropriate educational experience is emerging as a highly skilled professional task, requiring considerable knowledge and experience on the part of the teacher (cf. Tamburrini, 1982). In general, it seems that a wide range of objectives is helpful in the practice of early childhood education, if for no other reason than that a wide range of activities seem important for development (Kohlberg, 1968); many commentators today stress the need for eclectic and wide-ranging curricula (Kamii 1972; Robison & Spodek, 1965; Tamburrini, 1982). It also seems that at least for children from disadvantaged backgrounds, some degree of structure helps cognitive development, though peperhaps even more important than structure is a greater understanding of child development by teachers and a more coherent rationale on which to build a systematic approach to their work. Conclusion In conclusion, we may note that recent trends in early childhood education have been the products of a variety of interests and points of views - national policies regarding the role and pre-eminence of the family in child-rearing, the backgrou nd and preferences of staff of educational and child-care institutions, and the points of view of the sciences as interpreted and expressed by those professionally concerned with education (cf. Luscher, 1981). The kinds of facilities that are provided and the services that are offered reflect differences in these points of view and interests. The increase in resources for early childhood education which has been a feature of many countries in recent years has been due in part to the concern of bureaucratic organizations implementinc; publically legitimated norms and objectives. The principle of equality of opportunity, particularly as applied to the problems of children in disadvantaged areas, has been the guiding m'A;ve in the provision of 18 early childhood education facilities in most countries in Western Europe and in North America. Indeed, most of the public provision of recent years has been directed to children from lower socio-economic groups. Parents from this background have been more reluctant to have their children participate in formal early childhood education than have parents from higher socio-economic groups, a pattern that is repeated throughout the educational system. The interests of the family have also contributed to the expansion of facilities. Of particular importance has been the changing structure of the family and the increasing demands being made on it in industrialized urbanized societies. While parents will normally show considerable concern for their children's welfare, at the same time, they will also have their own interests in mind, which may not always coincide with those of their children. Over the last twenty years, there has been considerable activityin the study of child development which has had a marked influence on the practice of early childhood education. We have seen something of this when considering the objectives and structure of educational programmes. While the practitioner may be confused by differences in approach to the description and understanding of child development, a knowledge of these approaches can add a rationale and richness to the practical work of education. The good practitioner will be sensitized to the issues involved and will select the implications of the models that seem most appropriate for the situation in which he or she finds himself or herself. It is of course the interests and point-of-view of the child which should be central to the practice of early childhood education. Unfortunately these present problems in interpretation since the young child is not very well able to articulate his or her interests. Given this situation it is important that we take more time than perhaps we have in the past to decipher those interests as best we can. It is also important that other constituents in the area of early childhood education, whether they be parents, teachers, bureaucrats, or politicians, in putting forward their own claims, Should always do so with an awareness and consciousness of those of the child, no matter how poorly these might be articulated. For while early childhood education may serve several interests and constituents, and legitimately so, it can only be said to be serving its primary function when the interests of children are regarded as paramount. References BEREITER, c .. & ENGELMANN. S. Teaching disadvantaged children in the preschool. Englewood Cliffs, New Jersey: Prentice Hall. 1966. BIBER. B. The developmental-interaction point of view: Bank Street College of Education. In M.e. Day &R. Parker rEds.). The preschool in action: Exploring early childhood programs (2nd ed.). Boston: Allyn & Bacon. 1977. 19 BLANK. M. Preschool and/or education: A comment. In B. Tizard (Ed.), Early childhood education. A review and discussion of research in Britain. Slough, Berks: NFER Publishing Co .• 1974. BLOOM. B.S. Stability and change in human characteristics. New York: Wiley, 1964. BRONFENBRENNER. U. Two worlds of childhood. U.S. and U.S.S.R. New York: Russell Sage Foundation, 1970. BRUNER, J.5. The cognitive consequences of early sensory deprivation. In P. Solomon et al (Eds.), Sensory deprivation. A symposium held at Harvard Medical School. Cambridge. Mass: Harvard University Press, 1961. BUTLER, A.L. Current research in early childhood education: A compilation and analysis for program planners. Washington, D. c.: American Association of Elementary~Kindergarten~Nursery Educators, National Education Association, 1970. ELVIN, L. (Ed.). The educational systems in the European Community: A guide. Windsor, Berks.: NFER-Nelsan Publishing Co.• 1981. EUROPEAN ECONOMIC COMMUNITY. COMMISSION OF. Pre-school education in the European Community. Studies Collection: Education Series No. 12. Brussels: Author, 1979. EVANS, E.D. Contemporary influences in early childhood education (2nded.). New York: Holt. Rinehart & Winston, 1975. FRANKLIN, M.B .• & BIBER, B. Psychological perspectives and early childhood education: Same relations between theory and practice. In L. G. Katz (Ed.), Current topics in early childhood education. Vol. 1. Norwood New Jersey: Ablex Publishing Corporation. 1977. GESELL. A.L.. AMATRUDA. C.S .. CASTNER. B.M .. & THOMPSON. H. Biographies of child development: The mental' growth careers of eighty-four infants and children. A ten-year study. New York: Hoeber. 1939. GOODLAD. J.I.. KLEIN. M.F .. & NOVOTNEY. J.M. Early schooling inthe United States. New York: McGraw Hill. 1973. GORDON. E.W .. & WILKENSON. D.A. Compensatory education for the disadvantaged. Programs and practices. Preschool through college. New York: College Entrance Examinations Board 1966. GRANT, N. Soviet education. Harmondsworth. Middlesex: Penguin Books. 1964. HEBB. D.O. The organization of behavior. New York: Wiley, 1949. HUNT, D. Parents and children in history. New York: Basic Books, 1970. HUNT, J. Mc V. Intelligence and experience. New York: Ronald Press, 1961. KAMII. C.K. An application of Piage(s theory to the conceptualization of a preschool curriculum. In R.K. Parker (Ed.). The preschool in action. Boston: Allyn & Bacon, 1972. KELLAGHAN, T. The evaluation of an intervention programme for disadvantaged children. Slough. Berks: NFER Publishing CD .. 1977. KOHLBERG, L. Early education: A cognitive developmental view. Child Development. 1968. 39. 1013-1062. LAVATTELLI, C.S. Piaget's theory applied to an early childhood curriculum. Boston: American Science and Engineering. 1970. LAVATTELLI, C.S. (Ed.) Language training in early childhood education. Urbana, III.: University of Illinois Press, 1972. LAWRENCE, E. lEd.). Friedrich Froebel and English education. Landon.' Routledge & Kegan Paul. 1952. LAZERXON. M. The historical antecedents of early childhood education. In I. J. Gordon (Ed.). Early childhood education. The Seventy-first Yearbook of the National Society for Study of Education. Part II. Chicago: NSSE. 1972. LUSCHER, K. Building ecologies for human development: Towards a social policy for the Child In Centre for Educational Research and Innovation. Children and society. Issues for pre-school reforms. Paris: Organisation for Economic Co-operation and Development. 1981. MADAUS. G.F .• AIRASIAN. P.W .. & KELLAGHAN. T. School effectiveness: A reassessment of the evidence. New York: McGraw· Hill, 1980. 20 McKENNA. A. Psychology and preschool education. Irish Journal of Psychology. 1979.4. 131-140. o CONCHOBHAIR. S. Are we serving the system instead of the scholar? Irish Broadcasting Review, Spring 1979, 4. 7.-12. PETERS. D.l. Social science and social policy and the care of young children: Head start and after. Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology, 1980. 1, 7.27. PSACHAROPOUlOS. G. The economics of early childhood services. Paris: CERIIOECD. 1980. ROBINSON. N.M .. ROBINSON. H.B .. OARLING. M.A .• & HOLM. G. A world of children. Daycare and preschool institutions. Monterey, California: Brooks/Cole Publishing Co.. 1979. ROBISON, H.F .• & SPODEK. B. New directions in the kindergarten. New York: Teachers College Press, 1965. SEARS, P.S .• & DOWlEY. E.M. Research on teaching in the nursery school. In N.L. Gage (Ed), Handbook of research on teaching. Chicago: Rand McNally, 1963. SIGEL, I.E. Developmental theory and pre,school education: Issues. problems and implications. In I.J. Gordon (Ed). Early childhood education. The Seventy-first Yearbook of the National Society for the Study of Education. Part II. Chicago: NSSE. 1972. STODOlSKY. S. Defining treatment and outcome in early childhood education. In H. J. Walberg & A. T. Kopan (Eds.). Rethinking urban education. San Francisco: Jossey-Bass. 1972. TAMBURRINI. J. New directions in nursery school education. In c. Richards (Ed), New directions in primary education. Lewes. Sussex: Falmer Press. 1982. TAYLOR. P.H .. EXON. G .. & HOLLEY. B. A study of nursery education. London: Evans/Methuen, 1972. VAN DER EYKEN, W. The education of three-to-eight year olds in Europe in the eighties. Windsor, Berks: NFER-Nelson Publishing Co. 1982. WEBER, l. The English infant school and informal education. Englewood Cliffs. New Jersey: Prentice-Hall. 1971. WEI KART. O.P .. ROGERS. L. ADCOCK. C .. & McCLELLAND. D. Thecognitively oriented curriculum. Washington. D.C.: ERIC/National Association for the Education of Young Children. 1971. WOODHEAD. M. Preschool education in Western Europe: Issues. policies and trends. London: Longman. 1979. 21 Education and the psychological and physical development of young children Anne T. McKenna Lecturer, Department of Psychology, University College Dublin In talking to an audience of teachers in Mary Immaculate College for Teachers I am conscious that my audience is both well informed and highly experienced in the topic under discussion - Education and the Psychological and Physical Development of Young Children. I am sure you all remember singing as children a song that went: "Who shaves the barber, the barber, the barber, Who shaves the barber, the barber shaves himself". This morning I have been given the task of shaving the barber. But as the organisers of this excellently conceived seminar know, it has to be done, and in holiday time even the best of barbers don't mind letting somebody else do it for them. Growth and Development If we think of the child's growth and development as beginning at conception and finishing somewhere around twenty years of age, it is obvious that the increase in growth is not uniform throughout this period. We all know that in the first year of life growth is rapid in comparison to the rest of the life span; that it slows down until the years between nine and twelve years, when there is another burst of growth before the adolescent reaches adult height. All systems and parts of the body, however, do not grow at the same rate. It can be seen from Figure I that by four years of age, the child's brain and head have reached 80 per cent of their ultimate growth, whereas their general growth status for the rest of the body has reached only 40 per cent of that at twenty years. In other words the brain and head are precociously developed, relative to the rest of the bodily systems. What does this mean for the development of function in the child, for the development of the child's competencies in motor development and motor control, and for cognitive development? The question of brain growth is highly relevant to our topic of psychological development and education, since we know that the 23 100 /'",; - Brain :nd head 80 "llc ·s ~ ~ / / / 60 I I I 40 I Q l .~ VI General 20 Reproductive -'-'-'-'-'0 8 2 4 6 / ! ! i I ! .I ./ 8 10 12 14 16 18 20 Agt (years) Fig. 1. Typical growth curves of three different parts or tissue of the body from birth to age 20 (Adapted from Mussen (Ed) Carmichael's Manual of Child Psychology 1970. p. 85) brain is where messages from the outside world finish up, where they are linked and coordinated, where the long-term memory traces are laid down, where logical thinking is situated and where language is located. How do all of these things relate to brain growth and ultimately to age; are they parallel to brain growth; do they emerge immediately after the growth takes place or years after; are they affected by the amount and kind of stimulation the child receives in these early years? These are questions to whlch neuro-physiologists, psychologists and teachers of young children seek answers, answers . of which at the moment we can only catch tantalising hints and clues. Language Where we have the greatest and the most secure knowledge is in the area of language development, and here the emerging functions appear to follow very closely the developing structures in the brain. Figure 2 shows the growth of vocabulary items in young children. It can be seen that this growth parallels the growth in brain and head size, with the greatest increase occurring between two and a half and three years, and the rate of increase slowing down thereafter. The number of words a child has in her vocabulary is a good indication of overall language development, although they are not the most characteristic aspect of child language: they are merely, as it were, the tip of the iceberg. As you know, we expect an infant of one year to have two orthree words in their vocabulary, but by the age of two years we expect the toddler to be ablp. to put two words together in a relationship. Between one year and ,VIIO years of age, the child has, as one psychologist put it, "stepped into the human race". This is in fact precisely and biologically accurate. Between two years and four to five years, the child in normal circumstances has acquired the necessary and intricate skills of learning to speak - using three, four or five word sentences, changing the end of words for number and tense, joining two sentences together, embedding one within 24 another. The child can be accurately called a linguistic genius between the years of two and four, the genius beginning to fade a little at five to six years, and at eight almost all traces of originality have gone. That is not to say that the child will not use ideas and have original thoughts, but she will not be able to invent new wordseffortlessly and spontaneously, nor acquire a complex linguistic system with such ease. 5000 2fiOO :'i 1000 ~ .".cE 500 m 250 "" 100 -• .< .9 ,>0 "~ 0 > ."-, ~ £ - 50 25 0 t E 10 z 5 " 0 - 2.5 0.1 1 2 3 4 5 6 Yean or age Fig. 2. Logarithmic growth curve of the acquisition of recognition vocabulary by children from infancy to six years of age. (After Smith. 1926.) Recently a three year old advised me "We'd better dothat, bettern't we?", and I reckoned, as I'm sure you will too, that such a creation was beyond my capability, althourJh we can all und"erstand'what was meant. I would be incapable of making up such an example to illustrate this point. But this should not be too surprising if we think of it. Our ancestors, creatures like ourselves, made up a language "out of their own head", as children so graphically put it, and young children repeat this creative ability for a short time. The only surprising thing is that we as adults do not respect it more, but seem to keep most of our attention for related inessential aspects of emerging speech like proper pronunciation, or remembering to say "Please" and "Thank" you". Relative to what the child has learned and is learning, these are minor skills: to ask a child to repeat her 25 utterance, but remembering to say it properly this time, is a little like rebuking a man who has just saved you from drowning for not waiting to be properly introduced first. There is much that the adult can do to enrich the child's language, but it does not include correction of gratuitous speech. Indeed such corrections often have the opposite effect to that desired, viz. inhibiting the child's precious flow of speech. What then is the parent's or teacher's role in fostering language development in the years from three to five? Some years ago in speaking to an audience of early educators on the subject of child language, I was at great pains to explain the importance of language in the school curriculum in the period just after language has been acquired, i.e. from four to six years. I entitled the lecture "Child Language: old shoe or magic slipper", to express the idea that most children will slip into language easily and comfortably as into an old shoe, because they belong to a species that is made to speak a language: they are as it were, pre-programmed to speak. True, they need to be exposed to Irish or English or Chinese to be able to speak Irish or English or Chinese, but they do not need someone standing over them to see that they learn it. In these linguistic years, they appear to pick out what they need in a remarkably quick time, and this short exposure sets off their own language into an empty head, but more like exposing the child to a little language which will then act as a trigger to set off the child's own biologically pre-existing language "programmes". Just as we cannot say that a child "learns" to walk, because walking comes with growth and without much help from anyone, so it is to some extent with language. Just as we might say she "took her first steps" at fourteen months, so too should we say she "put her first two words together" at twenty months. We are wired up, prepared to speak, and this happens between the ages of two and four years. This early biological thrust to speaking and listening, or to what are sometimes called the primary linguistic processes, does not apply to reading and writing, the secondary linguistic processes. We are not wired up to read and write, no more than we are to broadcast on radio or to make audio-visual cassettes. Reading and writing, like these, are man-made artificats. There is nothing inevitable about them, and some cultures do not even have a written language, no more than they have libraries or TV sets. Whereas we might say then, that the years from two to four are biologically controlled, the years from four to six may be said to be environmentally or socially controlled, or if the child is at school, educationally controlled. And it is at this point. beginning around three or four years of age, that the old shoe mayor may not become a magic slipper, a magic slipper to carry the child into the realms of literacy and human culture by means of books and reading. The language that the great bulk of four year olds possess on entering school is perfectly adequate to carry them through all the 26 normal home and play transactions. They are able to express their wants to their parents, be it for a drink, for sweets or to be taken to MacDona Ids. They can shout and protest if someone annoys them, or tell tales on another child, and report when they are feeling sick. They can ask where their bike or lost shoe is, and pester their parents for special treats. But all this is of a quite different quality to written language. I do not propose here to discuss all the differences between written and spoken language. The main difference is that most of our spoken language and almost all of a four year old's language is context-bound, whilst written language is not. One has to be in the presence ofthe child to get her message, whereas a book may be read, and usually is, in total isolation. For example, if you asked a five year old a question like: "Who told you to draw on the paper?", the answer would likely be "Her" or "She did", pointing at or just looking at the person concerned. Most of the message comes from the meaning of the gesture and the total situation, with the words carrying relatively little information. And why should it be otherwise? If that message were to stand on its own without gestures, as in a written passage it would have to be something like: "The woman with the white blouse" or "The woman that's minding me". Why should you say allthatwhen you can do it so much more economically with a gesture, throwing in perhaps one word for good measure. The gesture plus pared down speech can make for effective inter-personal communication but it is of little use in such non-personal communication as reading and writing. The structure of reading and writing is more elaborate because it has to carryall the information. As well as being selfsufficient it also has to be "rounded off", as half finished sentences are not tolerated in writing as they are in speaking. The difference between the language that the four year old brings to school, and the language of reading books or written language, is the difference we feel our selves between talking to someone and writing a letter to them conveying the same information. The reason that the latter gives us so much trouble does not reside in the motor act of writing so. much as in the fact that we have to do a translation job first: we have to translate our speech into written language, then write this written language on paper. The language of books, even of infant readers, is of necessity a written language and therefore unknown to the non-reading child. Perhaps it would be more accurate to say unknown to some children because it is here that we find one of the most important sources of individual diffElrences, differences which are due almost entirely to the home Jckground and socio-economic status of the family. The child who has a repertoire of songs and poems, who has had stories read to her, who has been encouraged to use speech for comment and discussion as well as for on-going transactions, is already initiated into written or "school" language. For the others, the teacher may be the Open Sesame. The teaching task is to build a bridge between the child's very impressive and effortlessly acquired speech, and his 27 ability to read written language, and to put his own speech in writing. The child will acquire this only with considerable difficulty, and with great effort on his part and on the part of his teacher. These findings -from recent experimental research in the psychology and sociology of language have far-reaching consequences for practice in the infant classes. There are a numberof ways in which the teacher might build them into classroom practice, depending on the time available for individualised instruction. 1. The child's own speech can be transformed into reading material. This is not the same thing as constructing an "experience" reader from the child's own background of experience. It is doing this but doing it in the child's own words; we might call them linguistic experience readers. There is only one way to do this accurately and that is to make a recording of the child's speech. If this permanent record is not present for evidence, the adult will gloss the child's speech and not even be aware of the fact that they have tidied it up. Such a written record would look something like this: "My Mammy is nice and she's called Mary. My Mammy a/ways makes my tea, so she does". The more usual written version found in books, even children's books, would not repeat Mammy the second time but say "she" instead, and would not contain the little bit of circularity at the end which we all use in some shape or form when we talk, but not when we write. These are just a few of the differences between the child's language and written language. However, we cannot gauge how a child might construct his sentence until we have it on record. We know only that it is likely to be very different from reading book language. 2. The child's existing speech can be improved upon and the child can learn how to transform this into a closer approximation of written language when the occasion demands. This is sometimes called teaching for "language lift" or promoting aracy. The term oracy is a useful one as it reminds us that we are not teaching the child to speak: he can already do that. Our task is (a) to get the child to put his language skills to work in the classroom by whatever means we can, and (b) to work on this freely expressed language to prepare for reading. Oracytraining has received great attention in the last few years in many countries. We might mention in passing the Bullock Report, subtitledA Language for Life, and the many practical programmes of oracy, the best known being probably those of Joan Tough and the Gahagans, all of the United Kingdom. 3. A third aid in transforming or lifting child's language in the infant class is both traditional and routine in the infant programme, viz. the reading of stories to children. There is a structure or format to all stories, more subtle and often more undetected than that of a beginning and an end. The "once upon a time" at the beginning and the "happy ever after" at the end are part of the 28 structure, but there are other equally predictable elements in between. There is a central character, usually a little boy or girl, with a family constellation and friends around her. When there is a journey, there will be a setting-off followed by an arrival. Children who are fortunate enough to come from homes where stories are frequently read to them will have a variety of story structures which will match all eventualities without necessarily having one word of "reading". How often have you witnessed such a non-reading child go through, for example, an entire Ladybird book, getting the sequences in their correct order and sometimes - and sometimes not - reciting the written words in parrot fashion. Such a child is half-way to reading, and is engaging in the best kind of pre-reading activity. Psychologists studying children's cognitive processes have interested themselves in the kind of story structures or "scripts" that children can call on from inside their memory. It is obvious that the greater amount of scripts that the child has, the more predictable will be the written material of the stories and the better able will he be to guess what.comes next. We have known for a long time that reading or telling stories to children helps their speech and reading, and now we know that it is because the repeated story structures are forming a scaffolding for future forward planning when the child faces a page of text. The other pre-reading skills such as letter discrimination, matching letters to sound and word building, are well recognised and catered for in the infant programme, butthe basic one of matching spoken and written language, or the teaching of oral skills is now seen to be the most effective because it is the most basic skill for embarking on reading. Returning then to the question of brain growth and language development, it follows that if the basic syntactical and communicative abilities are laid down by four years of age, then atthis juncture, and perhaps even earlier, all children will benefit from language enrichment and teaching of oral skills. Every month that passes is taking the child from those twenty or so months of optimal language acquisition in the linguistic genius period of development: every month that passes is wideni ng the gap between the competencies of the child from the language-stimulating home background and those of the child from the language-deprived home background, a gap that good infant teaching can hope to narrow. Cognitive Skills Closely tied up with the child's developing language skills is the growth of conceptual development. Concepts may be called the tools of thought and it is indeed with these that children build up their store of knowledge and skills t~rough primary school and thereafter. The primary teacher may take many of the basic concepts for granted: the infant teachF!f dare not. When the child enters school, he has an array 29 of practical concepts which, again, like language, he has picked up for the business of living, sleeping, eating, shopping and playing. The world of concepts to which he will now be initiated in school, are those which divide up our world mathematically and scientifically, concepts of colour, time, space, size, number, and logic. There is, accompanying the learning of these concepts, a vocabulary, a technical language so to speak, which the child must know. The difficulty in introducing these first, all-important concepts to a four and five year old is that the language accompanying them is deceptively easy. If it were a technically abstruse vocabulary like that of chemistry or astronomy, we might do it better. The words for the concepts we are teaching are every-day words like more, less, bigger, in front of. later and soon. Because they are parts of our every-day speech, we are tempted to take them for granted, not fully appreciating the abstract structures they represent. Table 3 shows some of the basic scientific and social concepts that must be learned by a child. Those on the left hand side are abstract words showing the areas we are trying to teach: the words on the right hand side are the examples of these concepts we need toteach the child, and I think it is obvious from the table that they are in fact words in every-day use. Table 3. Typical Examples of Early Concepts SPACE nearlfar on top of TIME morning/night Saturday June SIZE bigger Ismallest NUMBER "none left" a pair of LOGIC same as/different if SOCIAL happy/sad myself daughter The familiarity of the words should not blunt an awareness ot their inherent difficulty. If we do not recognise this, w~ ~annot plan fortheir acquisition, and tick them off systematically when that has been done for each single child in our care. Such detailed ticking-off requires a level of detail in excess of that in the current Department of Education Curriculum. However, the great upsurge in interest in early childhood education over the last two decades has resulted in a plethora of excellent curricula and handbooks. These tend to converge on well agreed areas which include as well as language and cognitive skills, 30 manual and movement skills, self-awareness skills and social skills (see My World; A Handbook of Ideas). Just as most of the concepts that we are introducing to the child at the beginning of school are accompanied by words or labels that are used dozens of times a day by us, so too the activities that lead the child to understand these concepts may also be very ordinary and routine. The first and most important point to be made, of course, is that the activity of the child provides and lays the ground for all real cognitive development. This is the one most important insight that Piaget passed on to us, and one that has found its way into all good infant teaching. The young child's ability to snatch up a word and repeat it parrot-fashion can blind us to the fact that the possession of a word does not necessarily mean possession of the idea behind the word. So our four year old chanting the words "one, two, three, four, five" may signify no more and no less than his reciting "eenie, meenie, minee, mo". It has been said that a word is just a label on an empty bottle. Infant teachers certainly should treat it as such until they can investigate if the bottle has any contents. Secondly the activities that the child engages in, and the materials which he uses, may be of a very simple and ordinary nature indeed. The value lies in first what the child is doing with them and second how much the educator-observer is in association with the child. For example, a child slopping around in water with dishes might be imposing an order by his mental activity, noting that the little fat dish held more water than the big thin one, or that the lid of the small tea-pot fell inside the big tea-pot, but not vice versa. Another child, faced with an array of elaborate nesting cubes, might be doing nothing more than banging on the desk, or using them as pretend lorries. I am not, or course, suggesting that the child is wasting his time when he is engaged in pretend play. Pretending and make belief are as essential to the child's healthy development as food is to his physical development. The point is that the trained teacher knows one from the other and is unlikely to confuse them. Only thus can she extend the child's mental internal activities, presenting the right word, the provocative question, a new piece of equipment, introducing another child, or just leaving the first child alone. The importance and value of play are well known to the infant teacher: what she needs are the space and facilities to permit play its rightful place in the curriculum. Nor is it being suggested that good equipment in the classroom is not of great imporlance. On the contrary, we can say that the quality and quantity of material resourses of the classroom is more critical in these years than at any other period in school. The point I am stressing is that the most important equipment is the equipment inside the head of the teacher. Teachers are sometimes only too aware that they are expensively trained personnel, and feel that they have to justify their existence by constant activity on their part; questioning the child, planning guided discovery, or producing something tangible and visible at the end of the day. By quietly watching a child 31 manipulate material, by eavesdropping on children's conversations, the teacher's knowledge of the child and the child's competencies are being put to professional use, with the teacher noting, observing and planning for the next stage. Teaching, like beauty, is in the eye of the beholder, in so far as informed observation is at the heart of good infant teaching. For example, one of the impressive things about a child who has been at school for a mere two or three months is that they have learned the elements of time and know the difference between Saturday, Sunday and days of the week, between morning and evening. By the very act of attending the institution of school, these time elements, essential for learning to read the clock and calculate time, are borne in on the child through the structuring ofthe week and of the day. This is done by the teacher, more often than not, in an informal but highly effective manner. Just as in the other mathematical concepts, it comes through a heightened awareness on the part of the teacher as much as in any structured lessons on the concept of TIME. While the teacher is absorbed in watching and observing the child, she will be much less likely to push the child beyond its capacities, or fail to extend the child's capacities, the twin worries of the infant teacher. Whilst the teacher is using materials to build up concepts, whether these be purpose-built educational or teacher-made materials or even everyday objects, she should bear in mind that these will be novel to the child in varying degrees. Before a child can be asked to pe~form mental acts with the objects, like putting naturally corresponding objects together, putting one above another or arranging objects in order of size, he or she needs a degree of familiarity with the objects in the first instance. Children have a strong natural curiosity and are compelled to look at new things. They need time - and only they know how much time -to gaze and take in the new information from the object; with their eyes, hands and ears. When they have examined and 'got tothe bottom of' each object, then and only then are they able to perform mental actions on them. Atthe other end of the scale, children of this age can cease to find materials sufficiently attractive to want to do anything with them, apart from using them in play, in make-belief, or just throwing them around. Just as the period of getting to know them can take longerthan for an older child, their boredom with the same object can set in quick"" Social Thinking Finally let us consider an area of thinking and learning which is of paramount importance to the emotional and social wellbeing of our children, as well as affecting their reading, mathematical and other school skills. I am referring tothe child's thinking about himself, about his father, mother, his brothers and sisters, his perception of other people, his abilityto interpret his own emotions andtalkaboutthem, as well as interpret the emotions of those around him. The reason why it was not touched upon in this lecture was because it was too 32 important to be included, and would need another lecture. Rather than omit it altogether, perhap.s at the end of the lecture I could illustrate it with a story. The event took place on the very first day of a reception class, a reception class in a school which drew from all social groups. One miserable, poorly fed and carelessly clad little bot sat asfar away from the teacher as possible, at the back of the room. Like many children of this age, his behaviour under stress showed evidence of regression to an earlier stage in his thumb-sucking and in the fact that he hadjust wet his pants. Another new entrant of the same age confidently approached the teacher, and in a discreet low voice said: "Something very embarrassing has happened. A little boy has wet his pants". The social skill, poise and rich vacabulary separating the two children must serve for today to remind us of the magnitude and i'mportance of our role, and perhaps point the way our curriculum should be going in the area of social thinking and social skills. There are many areas of psychological development and education that have not been touched upon in this lecture, such as physical skills, aesthetic development, and in particular, the role of parents. The difficulty in selection serves to highlight the fact that there is so much to say in this newly expanding area of early education. However, one final thought on the critical nature of the infant teacher's work: a careful monitoring of each individual child's development and progress can be a critical factor for the whole educational future of the child. The infant educator, as well as being a teacher, is engaged in work of critical prevention, the prevention of remedial problems for the remainder of primary and possibly secondary education. In conclusion, might I say how impressive it is to observe how well infant teachers know their children, in spite of all the difficulties of large class size and inadequate equipment. The INTO organising committee have to be congratulated in helping them to get to know their children even better. References BATE, MARGARET, M. Smith and J. James 1981. Review of Tests and Assess· ments in Early Education. N.F.E.R. - Nelson. Windsor. BULLOCK, SIR AL.4N (Chairman) 1975. A language for life. (The Bullock Reportl H. M. S. O. London. CURTIS. AUDREY & 5. HILL. 1978. My World: A Handbook of Ideas. N.F.E.R. Windsor. GAHAGAN. D.M. & G.A .. 1970. Talk Reform: Exploration in Language for Infant School Children. London Routledge & Kegan Paul. McKEN NA. A. 1977. Child language: old shoe or magic slipper? Wychowanie w przedskolu. 11. 528·534. Warsaw. TOUGH. JOAN. 1976. Listening to children talking: A guide to the appraisal of children's use of language. Ward Lock Educational for Schools Council TOUGH, JOAN. 1977. Talking and learning: A guide to fostering communication skills. Ward Lock Educational for Schools Council. 33 Education for young children: infant classes in primary schools Siobhan M. Hurley Lecturer in Education Mary Immaculate College. Limerick In any treatment, however cursory, of the topic selected for discussion in this session of our seminar, some attention must be given both to the leading characters involved in the process being considered and to the main strategies which they adopt with a viewto achieving their objectives. In this paper, therefore, brief consideration will be given first of all to the two main characters whose interaction constitutes the focus of our attention, namely, the pupil and the teacher. From there we shall go on to examine the blue print which is meant to chart the course of this interaction, in other words, the curriculum. And we shall then turn our attention to what, from a practical point of view, is really the heart of the matter - the various problems which in one way or another ensure that actual outcomes are sometimes only a pale reflection of anticipated ones. It is the latter, I imagine, which will provide greatest food for thought in our subsequent workshop sessions. The Protagonists The Child Each year another set of new entrants is admitted into infant classes throughout the count .. · some are tearful, others smiling; some are shy, others self-assertive. But for all of them it is the beginning of a new era, and, whatever their temperament, it is a process that involves upheaval and readjustment. We must not lose sight of the fact that, whether it be at the age of four or five, and even for the best-adjusted child, starting school can be an intimidating experience. He is plunged from the small familiar world of home into the much bigger and less personal one of school. he is called upon to 35 r find his way around a new physical environment, to mix with new children, to take his instructions from strange adults. The sense of bewilderment at being left by his mother in these new surroundings can sometimes be very real. Naturally enough, the busy teacher is often tempted to assume that new children will quickly settle and to make light of the 'silliness' of those who seem to betaking longer than usual to do so. But the experiences of early school life can often play an important part in determining, not only a child's longer-term attitude to school, but also his attitude to new experiences in general. All infant-school children are young children, but not all who attend are the same age chronologically or in terms of maturity. Furthermore, there are tremendous individual differences in development even among children of the same chronological age. Una picks up the scissors and uses them matter-of-factly while Sean is hesitant and helpless in using such an instrument. Still, despite the wide range of individual differences so common at this stage, there are some characteristics infants share that are worth bearing in mind, the most significant, perhaps, being that their behaviour from one minute to the next can at times be marked by sudden and apparently unheralded contrasts. These children are no longer babies, as they will tell you in no uncertain terms; yet then need affection and support, and will break down under excessive stress as though they were. They are eager for information; yet they cannot concentrate for very long on anyone area. At this seminar our attention has already been drawn by Dr. McKenna to the complex pattern of psychological and physical development which characterises the young child at this stage. Atthe physical level alone, we cannot help noticing that they are always ready to run and climb and reach and grasp and shout. Watching them in the playground we marvel at their energy as they race at full speed, climb over self-imposed obstacles and screech with delight when propelled into the air. Their co-ordination is still to be refined. They therefore need a great deal of practice at such interesting skills as jumping from or over objects of moderate height, suspending themselves vertically by hands or by feet, or climbing swiftly up and slowly down. Infants often choose to do something the hard way just to give themselves interesting exercise. Linguistically their potential is no less obvious. Infants love to talk. With their keen ears, good memory and flexible tongues, four-, fiveand six-year-olds grow astonishingly in language power and in vocabulary. At this age they can pick up a second language in the same way in which they acquired their mother tongue. Current experience suggests that younger children are particularly responsive to informal opportunities for language acquisition. They can use accurate intonations and inflexions when portraying a certain character in dramatic play, they can learn many verses of a song, and they delight in acquiring new words. especially appealing ones like 'millions and trillions'. 36 Substantial intellectual activity is also in evidence at this time. One need only mention their constantly increasing power of reasoning, their deep and often unanswerable questions, their absorption in problem solving, fascination with a variety of mathematical concepts and spontaneous interest in symbols. The children need and want a chance to exercise their fast-growing minds. Yet they do not learn primarily by passive attention to the teacher or mere receptivity to information. Exercise of the mind at this stage comes about as partof the total activity of the child and is accompanied by a sense of urgency to find out now, on the spot. Significant development is also to be expected on the social front. The young child is small and frail. all around him, especially when he has taken the major step into the wider world by coming to school, he is confronted with greater size, strength and power. SOCially a fouryear-old is friendly and trusting. Even ill-treated and badly neglected children of this age have seldom enough experience of anything else with which to compare their lot, and anti-social attitudes, which they may develop later, have not yet had time to put down roots. Just occasionally a child is so accustomed to rough handling that he views the teacher's kindly concern with mistrust and suspicion. But, on the whole, almost all children of four, five or six years respond enthusiastically and willingly to someone who listens to them and takes an interest in what they are doing and thinking. This does not mean that four- and five-year-olds are not frequently self-centred and demanding. At school they have to learn to share and to take turns. Sometimes this is very hard for them, especially if their mother has always been at their beck and call. They usually have the capacity, though, to recognize fairness and to respond enthusiastically to it. While not as immediately obvious as certain other aspects of their personality development, perceptible advances are also being made in emotional growth and personality development. You can easily see the increases in height and weight. But if you look closely you will also see that a child in the infant school develops noticeably in personality as well. During this period, in fact, he may change from a youngster who seems to have no initiative and who only imitates what another child does, into a child who asserts his preferences, expresses his ideas and carries them out so that both teachers and other children have a genuine respect for him. This, then, in ,broad outline, is the main character in our plot. brimming over with enthusuasm to be allowed develop simultaneously along so many differer' 'rants. The essence of the task he constitutes for the infant school must surely be to find the most expeditious way possible of facilitating these spontaneous tendencies to development which have begun to manifest themselves. The task is undoubtedly a daunting one. To settle for anything less, however, would be seriously to betray the implicittrust placed in the school by this aspiring adult at perhaps the most crucial stage of his development. 37 The Teacher In coming to introduce our second protagonist, then, it must already be obvious that the task which confronts the teacher is in anybody's terms a formidable one. The work of the reception-class teacher is different in some important ways from that of any other kind of teacher. It used to be thought that she did not have to be particularly clever and that any motherly soul who could count up to ten, wipe noses and tie shoelaces could do the job competently. Fortunately, educationists do not take that view nowadays. The easing of children from home, where they have almost the undivided attention of their mother, to school, where they have to share the teacher with thirty or forty others, and where they are expected to become self-reliant and learn in specific ways, is a highly skilled job. Needless to mention, the reception-class teacher does more of the wiping-and-washing, tying-and-buttoning type of task than any other teacher, because her children are less capable of doing these things for themselves. But this is not the main burden of her work. On the reception-class teacher, for example, rests the weighty responsibility for forming the child's attitude towards school. If we can encourage him to enjoy coming to school, to enjoy learning, to have a positive attitude towards his own progress and to pecome a valued member of a group of children of his own age, then, whatever his capabilities may be, we have laid good foundations for his happiness and educational progress in school. This should be our primary aim. If we want the children to live happily in tomorrow's world, we must be aware that personal prejudices affect our judgement of them. One obvious example is that we teachers, being human, have to be on our guard against being prejudiced in favour of behaviour that makes life easier for us in the classroom. If we allow our personal preferences such matters as manners and social niceties, neatness and appearance, or the accents and social status of parents, to influence our professional judgements, we can make serious mistakes when assessing a child' ability. Assessment of potential for learning should be based on such indicators as level of curiosity, ability and determination in solving problems, increasing concentration span, sensitivity to experience, breadth of vocabulary, and on emotional factors like response to encouragement and reaction to new challenges. Such assessment can only be made by someone trained to know what to look for, who has observed a child carefully in learning situations for a considerable time. The effects of a teacher's judgement are more far-reaching and have more influence than many people realize. The burden of making accurate assessments lies heavily on every teacher, but especially on infant teachers who make the first ones. Succeeding teachers often, if only subconsciously, take earlier teachers' remarks into account. and so prejudice can build up, especially as the child himself by this time may be conforming to the official assessment of him. 3B The real test of the teacher's beneficial professional influence on the child, however, will be in the manner in which she exploits to the full the potential of the infant school curriculum, and it is to this aspect of her work that we shall now direct our attention, The Curriculum Building a curriculum that is sufficiently varied to be relevant to most if not all of the children must be the aim of every infant teacher. The possibilities for social and intellectual development in infants call for careful analysis of their capacities for achievement in these areas, and for just as careful an examination of which aspects of the environment should be built into the curriculum. The latter may well be different for different groups of children, since their previous experiences will differ as to familiarity with organizational requirements, receptivity to materials, facility in adjusting to new situations, and even familiarity with certain songs and stories. A lot of thought is required to plan a day's programme and organize the children's work and activities so as to cater for their all-round development at this level. A structured scheme for language and mathematics is clearly needed, due at least in part to the existence of individual differences. A child is a bundle of many parts - physical, intellectual, psychological, spiritual - and often there is no knowing where one ends and another begins. Each one of these facets of our human nature is itself fascinatingly complex and varied. one might liken the individual human being to a telephone exchange. It is not sufficient to have a limited number of lines in operation. In the one case as in the other the real challenge lies in keeping the entire system working at optimum capacity. Education, therefore, must help the child to grow in each and everyone of the facets that constitute his total make-up. To take an obvious example: the activity we used to know as 'Drill' has changed its name to 'Physical Education' because it is now recognized that the skills involved are as much a part of real education as the 3 R's. The name has changed, not because we think the new one sounds nicer than the old, but because the new one is a more adequate expression of what children are now doing in the periods on the timetable labelled Physical Education. Each child is born with abilities, powers, energies enfolded within him like a flower inside a bud. Education is the sunshine that coaxes the bud to open and the petals to unfold to their fullest size and perfection . . Children learn best when the things they learn about have their roots in their own interests and experiences. There are sound psychological reasons for this. Children feel secure and at ease with what they already know, and the teacher meeting them thus on their own ground wins their confidence and friendship. They are then willing and eager to set out with her on this educational treasure- 39 ", " hunt, following all the clues she has laid forthem along the trail to the rich prize of ever-growing knowledge and skill. All this means that the role of the teacher is no longer that of a dispenser of information in a haze of chalk-dust. She is a leader, a guide whose function it is to help her children to think forthemselves and to progress from one discovery to another and from concrete examples to general principles and abstract reasoning. This is delicate and demanding work and it presupposes a teacher who will have the patience to watch and listen to her children with a view to discovering from them the things they have at heart and the ways in which their minds work. Then she will provide opportunities and materials for these interests to be expressed in creative ways, through art, drama, reading, writing and mathematics. She leads her children along, step by step, as individuals or as members of a group,. but only rarely all together as a class. This is to ensure that each child can progress at his own rate, the brighter ones forging ahead as fast as they like, the duller ones plodding along without feeling that they have to try to catch up with the rest. In this way the latter are spared the humiliation and discouragement which are the fore-runners of failure throughout their school life. The starting point of all education is the child, at whatever stage of development we happen to find him. The aim of education is his maturity, which is simply the ability to apply his knowledge, skills and standards throughout his life in a way that is practical, persevering and of benefit both to himself and to the whole community. We have looked, then, at the nature of the child at infant level, atthe complexity of the child's development and at the problem offraming a curriculum suited to the needs and capacities of the infant. We recogn ize the tremendous challenge wh ich th is task constitutes forthe teacher. Perhaps the greatness of this challenge, however, arises not so much from the extent to which young children are alike but from the extent to which they differ. Who will deny that the level of individual differences in entrants to our primary school system today is greater than it has ever been before? Those infants who are fortunate, either through the provision for them by their parents of educational toys, colouring-books and pencils, story-readers, jig-saws and the like, or'through a selective and enlightened exposure to television, or perhaps through travel abroad, or even through the influence of effective pre-school preparation, will arrive at school already well advanced on the educational ladder. The result will bethatthosewho come from less fortunate or even disadvantaged homes arrive at school at an even lower starting-point, relatively speaking, than might have been the case in the past. The question now is not simply how this challenge can be met; for some teachers it has become a question of whether it can be met at all. The curriculum currently in operation in our schools allows the greatest degree of flexibility in selecting the programmes most suited to local circumstances, The school and its environment. the particular 40 aptitudes and interests of its pupils and teachers and the range of facilities available to it are all relevant considerations when making this selection. The emphasis which the curriculum places on the creative areas is of vital importance and has rightly been welcomed by teachers and children alike. It tends to correct the imbalance which has long been a feature of the educational system, and which has involved an over-emphasis on retention and recall and on waiting passively for information and direction at the expense of the more fundamentally important ability to question, to think independently, to put things together in new ways and to discover for oneself. The present curriculum seeks then to promote the growth of the child in his various dimensions, not through mere instruction in a variety of subject areas, but through a sequence of educative experiences .. The teaching of content knowledge is secondary to the development of a variety of skills. The basic skills of literacy and numeracy remain a foundation but are accompanied by a range of other skills - social skills and study skills, for example - all of which lend themselves to the development of that ability essential for survival in the world oftoday, and, we are told, even more essential for survival in the world of tomorrow, namely, the ability to find out for oneself. The Problems Incomplete Understanding of Curriculum With any curriculum there are problems. Our present curriculum, despite its considerable virtues, is hindered by many practical obstacles from really achieving the aims set for it. We as teachers should be aware of these difficulties. Admittedly, the current curriculum, by comparison with that which preceded it is still relatively new. Indeed we still tend unconsciously to refer to it as 'the New Curriculum'. But to what extent have we succeeded in exploiting its many-sided potential and in availing of the rich opportunities presented by its high degree of flexibility? To what extent have we introduced the informality in approaches to learning it explicitly encourages? To what extent have we escaped from the long tradition of formal work at the infant level or indeed throughout the primary school? The most fundamental difficulty with curriculum, then, is in relation to its implementation. To implement any curriculum successfully we must have a body of teachers, with adequate resources and backup facilities including in-service tr~.,ling, who understand the basis of the curriculum and who are prepared to implement it even in the face of some opposition. You may wonder where this opposition could come from. Occasionally it will come from fellow-teachers who either have only an imperfect conception of what the curriculum really offers, or who have become so inflexible in their own approaches that they are not prepared to put their trust in something new, whatever its 41 theoretical merits. Opposition, however, can more frequently be found to come from parents; and this brings us to our second point. Educating the Parents Despite efforts made through such publications as 'A.r nDaltaf Uile' and 'Your Child and Your School' to inform parents, there can be no doubt but that many parents have little understanding of the work of primary teachers today, and large numbers of them remain quite unconvinced of the value of approaches which are so radically different from those of former days. This is especially a problem at infant level. The infant teacher is concerned with developing competency in the basic skill areas through emphasizing number concepts and reading readiness, knowing how such a foundation will later enable the child to forge ahead. She will also be concerned with the creative aspects of the child's development as well as his social, spiritual and physical growth. The painstaking work of the teacher in carefully laying the foundation is not always appreciated by the parent who is anxious to see the building take some shape above ground level. In this area, too, we have ourfly-by-night builder who sets about erecting a structure on a foundation of sand. We have all met the children who arrive on the school door-step drilled in the rote recitation of numbers one to ten, and whose parents are convinced that they will be reading in weeks since they already have the alphabet by heart. If we are to avoid disappointing such parents, and if we are to win their confidence in us as professionals, it is essential that we establish the closest possible links between home and school. In so doing we will often be engaged in educating the parent as well as the child. The necessity for this approach was recognized by the Plowden Report where we read (par. 129): 'It has long been recognised that education is concerned with the whole man; hence forth it must be concerned with the whole family.' Teachers, however, are becoming increasingly conscious of the need for effective home-school links, and this fact is borne out by the recent INTO Congress policy decision in this matter. Finding Effective Admission Procedures A third problem manifests itself every year on the first schoolday in September, when upto forty new entrants, and sometimes more, take their places in the junior infant class. The problem is, in fact, twofold. Quite clearly it is a problem for the teacher. The frustration involved in attempting to develop an interpb. ~onal relationship with, and to act as guide, as mentor and as substitute mother for forty individuals is not to be underestimated. On the other hand, however, the problem for the child should not be underestimated either. Coming, as most children do, from a caring, secure relationship where the effective child-adult ratio is 1:1, he is expected to take in his stride an environment of apparent chaos, an environment of noise and 42 numbers, where a single caring adult is herself striving to operate within the limits imposed by a ratio of 1 :40. For years we have spoken of the pressures which large class sizes cause for teachers. it is my firm conviction that a greater emphasis on the pressures which they cause for children would bring speedier results in this area. After ali, how many parents would place their child in a creche of forty? What, in fact, is the largest number which a caring parent would tolerate in the care of a single adult in existing playgroups and pre-schools? Haphazard Pre-School Provision The teacher's problems in this regard are compounded by the great variety and lack of cohesion or organization to befound in the existing forms of pre-school child-care provision, which range from simple child-minding services through various kinds of playgroups and nursery groups to the more formally organized pre-schools which are quite specific in their effort to make at least some kind of structured educational provision for the children. Each of these can, of course, in its own way, quite successfully serve a particular need. Where the difficulty arises, however, is when one of them attempts to fulfil purposes which can only be catered for adequately by another. One might refer in this regard to the admittedly rare instances which occur of play-groups which sometimes set themselves the task of catering for specifically educational objectives. Such instances prompt one to ask: what is the point of having a carefully structured curriculum providing for a continuous graded development through the full eight years of primary school, and then permitting others, who may have only the haziest and most superficial knowledge of that curriculum and who may be totally lacking in anything approaching a profeSSional understanding of the developmental needs of young children, to set them objectives and to give them expectations which in no way relate to what may reasonably be expected of them when they eventually find themselves in the reception class in the infant school? Inadequacy of Two- Year Cycle Turning to problems of a different kind, one also notes with interest the resolution of the INTO Congress which advocates extending the present two-year infant cycle to a three-year one. Such a change would, in my view, be most welcome if it were used to extend the period of pre-formal preparation which all children should enjoy before commencing the more formal work of the higher standards. Given the relative shortness of the present two-year cycle, many infant teachers, recognizing the value of informal work and play-type activities, feel pressurized by the programme, by the parents, and on occasion even by other teachers, into embarking at too early a stage upon the more easily assessable, more obvious and more formal aspects of schoolwork. An extra year, provided it is put to the right use, and is not used to extend downwards still further th') commencement 43 of formal work, could be of tremendous advantage in laying a really sound foundation for all subsequent endeavours in the field offormal education. Shortness of Day At the risk of drawing upon myself the ire of the junior-infant teacher, let me highlight another related difficulty which sprang from the long tradition of formal work even at the junior-infant level. Here traditionally we have had a short day. But even with such a short day it was still felt that the necessary minimum of formal work had to be fitted in, thus leaving less timeforthe desirable informal activities. As we now move into an era of less emphasis on formal and more on informal work, is it not the case that the children will run less risk of becoming exhausted? Will the teachers not now be less pressurized? Will there not, accordingly, be reasonable grounds for arguing In favour of the extension of our relatively short junior-infant day? Deficient Background Services One must ask whether the burden of the infant teacher would not also be reduced by bringing support personnel into the classroom. Such ancillary personnel, or teacher aides, have proven their effectiveness in other countries. If they are needed and have been found useful in countries which have considerably more favourable pupil-teacher ratios, how much more essential are they in this country where the ratio is still so unfavourable. Much ofthe drudgery and many of the less professional aspects of the teacher's day, such as mixing paints, laying out newspapers, tidying up, paring pencils, perhaps even playground supervision, could be entrusted to an appropriately trained teacher's aide. Many parents also would welcome such involvement even on a rota basis. It is possible that special courses could be organized, perhaps through the colleges of education, for such personnel. The teacher of today, professional, caring and confident in her approaches, has nothing to fear and has much to gain from welcoming the purposeful presence of others in her classroom. Isolation of Classroom Teacher In speaking of the infant teacher as a professional person, we recognize the necessity for keeping up-to-date with new developments·in the field of early childhood education. A constant reevaluation of our ideas, our procedures and our practice is required. Such professional development is well-nigh impossible for the individual isolated in his or her own classroom. Each year brings new insights into this area for both the individual and the teaching profession as a whole. Such insights can be shared through meeting one's colleagues, through reading in this field and through a continuous process of inservice study. It is now patently obvious that specialist inservice courses on an ongoing basis should be available 44 to all infant teachers as one of the best means of advancing the frontiers of our professional knowledge. In this regard one might ask: when do infant teachers have an opportunity for such re-evaluation? and how many infant teachers ever get together to discuss the availability of resources, the preparation of apparatus, the grading of materials or the structure and organization of specific programmes? If at present many of them experience difficulty in making such arrangements, surely the matter is sufficiently urgent to demand that acceptable solutions to these difficulties be sought actively without delay. Inequitable Official Discrimination My final point has to do with points! To be more precise, it relates to the points system. You will have noticed that throughout our discussion of schooling at infant level it has been necessary to emphasize that the infant is a unique individual, that the infant level has its unique problems, and that we have never seriously faced up to the challenges or grappled with the problems of this level in a comprehensive or organized manner. In other words, we have failed to pay sufficient attention to the particular problems of the youngest children in our system. Indeed it is worth noting that that great arbiter of relative merit, the points system, gives to every child under nine years of age a value of 1.5 points as opposed to a full 2 points for his older brother or sister who is between nine and thirteen years of age. The child in infant classes, in fact, is conceded a mere quarter of the significance ascribed to a sixteen-year-old, who merits a full 6 points. Since it is the points system which decides such significant questions as the level of a principal's remuneration and the level of promotional opportunity in general within the school, and since these are matters which are by no means unrelated to the important question of how to attract enthusiastic and enterprising teachers into that area of the educational system where most can be done to alleviate the damaging effects of social inequality, it is surely not unreasonable to ask, with the best interests of the child at heart, whether in present circumstances we can truthfully be said to cherish all the children of the nation equally. References COHEN. DOROTHY H .. AND RUDOLPH. MARGUERITA. Kindergarten and Early Schooling. New Jersey: Prentice-Hall. Inc .. 1977. Curaclam na Bunscoile: Lamhleabhar an Oide. Baile Atha Cliath: an Roinn Oideachais, 1971. MITCHELL. CYNTHIA. Time for School. Harmondsworth Middx.: Penguin Books, 1973. SPODEK. BERNARD. Early Childhood Education. New Jersey: Prentice-Hall, Inc.. 1973. United Kingdom. Report of the Central AdVisory Council lor Education. (The Plowden Report.) Vol. /, London: HMSO. 1967. 45 Early education for children with special needs Anne O'Sullivan Lecturer in Special Education, St. Patrick's College of Education Dublin. Rationale of Early Intervention for Children with Special Needs Historically there have been two views on the impact of early intervention on cognitive abilities. One view is that heredity sets the limits of cognitive growth and intelligence, consequently home and school training cannot develop the child's intelligence beyond the limits set by inherited biological characteristics. Arthur Jensen was one of the most outspoken proponentsofthe hereditary viewpoint. The other viewpoint takes an enviromental perspective. It suggests that school success is at least partly the product of early home and school training. Consequently environmental experiences can accelerate mental growth. Some of the influential work giving support to this viewpoint included Hunt's famous book Intelligence and Experience (1961) and Bloom's Stability and Change in Human Characteristics (1964), the Skeels & Dye Iowa studies (1939),1942,1966), theKirkwork (1958, 1965), and the work of Heber et al (1972). In the Skeels & Dye study (1939) in Iowa 13 children of less than 3 years were removed from an orphanage and placed in an institution for mental defectives. These infants were placed only one to awardwith adolescent mentally retarded girls who gave them attention and training. Therewas also a comparison group that remained in the orphanage and received no special training or care. Two years later the first group was found on testing to have increased their IQ scores by an average of 27.5 pOints, while the comparison group experienced an average decrease of 26.2 points in their scores. Follow-up studies 3 and 20 years later showed that the differences between the groups were maintained in favour of the experimental group. In the Kirk study, two preschool groups were organised for young mentally handicapped children, one of which received preschool education and one of which did not. Children who received two 47 years preschool education increased in both mental and social development and retained the increase to age eight. Those who did not receive preschool education dropped in both their IQ and SQ. The results of these and other studies led to the conclusion (Kirk, Kliebhan & Lerner, 1978) that 'intensive and appropriate education at an early age can account for a 20-30 point increase in IQ'. The later Headstart findings provide support for the preventive function of early intervention. According to Lerner et al (1981) 'the belief that interventional strategies can enhance cognitive growth in children provides the common cord binding together the fields of early childhood and special education'. There is, moreover, evidence that for children with severe learning difficulties, the years before 6 may be critical for their grasp of language (Mittler, 1979). It does seem that the arguments for early intervention in the case of children with special needs are overwhelmingly in favour. According to Professor Peter Mittler (1979), 'there is now general acceptance of the principle that one cannot begin early enough to work with a handicapped child and his family. Services have to be provided right from the start; it is certainly no use waiting for the child to go to school before providing skilled professional help'. The functions of early intervention are two-fold. 1. Preventive By the time the child reaches school age precious learning time has passed and developmental delays may have led to academic failure. Early intervention may reduce the number of children requiring special education. 2. Developmental Early intervention will serve to increase the potential of severely handicapped children. In the U.S. the early childhood movement for the handicapped has received support from both State and national sources. At the federal level the Office of Special Education and Rehabilitation Services within the Department of Education has placed early childhood special education programmes as a 'top priority' item. PL94-142 (The Education of An Handicapped Children), which is considered a landmark in special education legislation, mandates free appropriate public education for all children aged 3 to 21. Thefederal mandate for children in the 3 to 21 age range is subject to the provisions in State law for this age group. PL94-142 does not include the birth-to-three population.' However as of 1977 ten states had legislation covering children from birth onward. In 1972 the Headstart legislation was revised. The revised legislation required all Headstart programmes to include a minumum of 10% of their enrolment to be identified as handicapped. In the U.K. in 1977, according to the Warnock report, roughly only one in six 3-5 year old children was attendin~ maintained nursery 48 school or nursery class in maintained primary schools. Within these restricted numbers of children receiving nursery education, the proportion of children with speCial needs appeared to be very limited and more of these children were found (in one study at least) to be attending playgroups and day nurseries. The Warnock Report recommended that the provision of nursery education for all children should be increased, so that opportunities for nursery education for young children with special needs could be correspondingly extended. In Ireland, with a few exceptions, children with special needs do not receive a state education below the 4yr level. The exceptions that I am aware of are the Sean McDermott Street Preschool Centre and young visually-impaired and hearing-impaired children who have the services of peripatetic teachers and a preschool diagnostic class in Temple St. hospital for children with language disorders. In Ireland, according to Faughnan & O'Connor's NESC report the number of children attending special schools or special classes (1.4% of the school population) is similar to comparable figures in England and Wales (1.8%) and Scotland (1.4%). This figure does not represent the total number of children with special needs. It has been estimated in 8ritain that up to one child in five will have some sort of special need. These are the children who in the Warnock Report's words 'are likely to require some form of special educational provision at some time during their school career'. At present in. Ireland, according to Faughnan & O'Connor, special provision of some type is available for just over 5% of the primary school population, 3% being in the form of remedial teaching. It would appear then that at present in Ireland the majority of children with special needs are being educated in ordinary schools without any formal special provision, either at pre-primary or primary level. Identification In order to provide for childen with special needs at an early stage, one has to identify these children. The more obviously handicapped children are easily identified. Children with Spina Bifida, Down's Syndrome, severe Cerebral Palsy and total deafness or blindness can be identified in the first hours or days of life. It becomes more difficult to identify children whose handicaps are not so obvious, especially those who fall into the mild category of handicap. in the U.S. a federal programme 'Child Find' mandates States to 'actively" seek out those handicapped children who are currently unserved, underserved, or inappropriately served'. The steps involved are four: 1. Definirlg the target population 2. Increasing the public's awareness 3. Encouraging referrals 4. Canvassing the community for children in need of services. 49 1 . ) Screening follows. A comprehensive list of medical and educational conditions are included in screening programmes. There are very grave problems, methodological and otherwise, involved in screening and assessing children at an early age. For instance, Lerner et al report that of forty-four preschool assessment tests available nationally in the U.S. only five met the American Psychological Association guidelines for educational and psychological tests. A start would have to be made to develop a reliable screening process. At present, the identification of children with special educational needs is donebyHealth Boards and probably serves only to isolate those children whose handicap is moderate or severe rather than to pick out those with mild degree of handicap, or those who will experience learning problems in the ordinary school. There exists at present no proper structure for identifying children with special educational needs. The absence of a preschool system, the absence of a Schools' Psychological Service makes the task impossible. Even at present in infant classes no formalised attempt at assessment to identify children with special needs is made. Some of the reason for this gap is lack of skill in formal assessment techniques on the part of infant teachers. It is due also to lack of a structure or tradition of assessment within the primary school. No small part of it is due to lack of time and resources. In the last few years in Britain there has been a move towards developing and trying out criterion-referenced assessment schedules which can identify and pinpoint areas of developmental delay and/or signs of handicap. Criterion-referenced assessment is to be distinguished from norm-referenced assessment. Norm referenced assessment is used to compare one individual's test performance with another individual's on the same test. Criterion-referenced assessment is used to compare the individual's performance with some standard or criterion and is concerned with the individual's ability to perform certain tasks. Examples include 1. The Behaviour Assessment Battery (Kiernan & Jones, 1978) a battery primarily aimed at the young severely handicapped child. 2. The parent Involvement Project Developmental Charts (Jeffrey & McConkey 1976). These charts are designed for mentally handicapped children but useful for pinpointing specific problem areas for other groups. 3. A Develop(TIental Schedule (Berry & Ives 1976) These schedules cover a wide range of developmental processes in gross motor, fine motor, social, personality and behaviour traits areas. There are several other examples, both British and American, for use at pre-school and early primary levels. The existence of a preschool system would in principle, at any rate, permit the identification of children with special needs at an early stage and the possible diagnosis and programming for spec'al needs 50 at this stage. The suggestion in the Warnock Report of special education advisors would seem to be a good one. That and/or a Schools' Psychological Service would appea'to be a necessary adjunct to a preschool system which would attempt to help children with special needs. Characteristics of Early Intervention for Children with Special Needs The chief ways by which a programme of early intervention for children with special needs would be characterised are: 1. 2. 3. 4. A major emphasis on individual differences. The intensive examination of tasks to be learned, or task analysis. High level of parent involvement. Flexibility the programme, and the setting will vary according to the needs ascertained. 1. Individual Differences The curriculum for the child with special needs at the early stages would probably focus on the following areas: 1. 2. 3. 4. Motor and Perceptual Skills Cognitive Skills Communication Skills Social and Affective Skills. Following assessment of the child's needs, an individual programme would be worked out for each child in all of these areas. For example, the child with special needs may have difficulty in the last area above. There is some evidence to suggest that handicapped children have psychosocial problems in addition to their other handicaps. This aspect of their problem may be the most debilitating. They may be rejected by parents, peers and teachers, and disliked or ignored by others. They may be poor in perceiving social cues. These children may need to be taught how to play. some of the research done, for instance in St. Michael's House, would indicate that young Down's Syndrome children develop spontaneous play more slowly than normal children; and that they can be helped here by parents. Wehman outlines four categories of play (1) exploratory play, (2) toy play, (3) social play and (4) structured game play. The handicapped child may need to be helped in any or all of these types. The example of play illustrates the necessity for detailed observation of the young child's behaviour, and the desirability for an understanding and working knowledge of the teacher's part in reinforcement theory, modelling, shaping behaviour, and methods of behaviour recording and monitoring. It illustrates also the fact that the young handicapped child will not learn many of the life tasks which in other children we take for granted, and that education for this child will involve teaching of these skills of living. 51 2. Task-Analysis The rationale underlying this emphasis in teaching young handicapped children, is that unlike the normal child for whom learning will occur naturally and spontaneously, the young handicapped child will not learn many tasks unless he is taught and, moreover, will not learn unless the task to be learned is broken down into successive steps, which are sequentially mastered. Another aspect of task analysis relates to the child's learning style - to how best the task should be presented. Thetask needs to be analysed from the point of view of the demands it makes on the child's ability to process information e.g. does it demand a visual, auditory or tactual/kinaesthetic response - is the response at receptive or expressive level. 3, Parent Involvement Lerner et allist a number of reasons why parents should be involved in early education of children with special needs. 1. 2. 3. 4. Parents are in strategic positions: they know their children better than anyone else and spend more time with them over an extended period. Parents can compensate for shortages of one-to-one services. Parents can reduce the cost of instruction and other services. Parents can solve time and distance problems, particularly in rural areas. They further outline four dimensions of parent programmes 1. Developing Parent Participation. 2. Supporting parents emotionally. 3. Exchanging information with parents 4. Improving Parent/Child interactions. Programmes of parent participation are varied, both in the U.S. and the U.K. some of them are worth mentioning: 1 . The Portage Projects The Portage Project was developed in Wisconsin, U.S.A. and used on both sides of.the Atlantic. It is a means of home-based teaching of of skills to preschool children with a variety of developmental handicaps or delays. The system is a structured method involving trained home advisors who on a weekly basis supervise and monitor the parents' handling and teaching of their own children, using schedules of training skills and behaviours which cover six areas - infant stimulation, self-help, motor, socialisation, cognitive and language. A developmental checklist containing 580skills in these six areas is accompanied by a set of cards, one for each skill on the checklist. and outlining suggestions for teaching the skills. The use of the Portage 52 system was evaluated in Wales and England, and results were positive in terms of mothers' responses and children's skills acquisition. According to Mittler the system seems to provide an effective and flexible approach to early intervention which helps the child and supports the family. It is also remarkably cheap; the training of home visitors can be complete in about a week, butthe system calls for a series of weekly control meetings between all the home teachers working in a particular area. 2. Putting-Two-Words-Together This course, designed and evaluated by Dr. Roy McConkey of St. Michael's House, uses 5 video-tape programmes. It aims to impart to parents and teachers, techniques which have been found to be effective in helping the early language development of mentally handicapped children, particularly those around the 'two-word' stage of expressive language. 3,Handbooks For parents who cannot attend courses, or avail of a home<based programmes, Souvenir Press have published some useful handbooks. These include: Starting Off, Kiernan, Jordan & Sounders, 1979 Let Me Speak, Jeffree & McConkey, 1976 Let Me Play, Jeffree, McConkey & Hewson, 1977. These are just some examples of ways of helping parents. There are others - self-help groups of parents for instance. Organisation of Early Intervention for Children with Special Needs It is clear that no blanket system exists which would meet the needs of all handicapped children. A number of aspects pertinent to organisation are: (11 Intervention at preschool level will need to commence shortly after birth for some children . .(21 Any system which is evolved will need to be flexible so that a range of options would exist, which could be adapted to a particular child's needs. -:)tions might include: (11 Home-based tuition. (21 Attendance at a regular preschool either whole or part-time. (31 Attendance at a special preschool class attached to an ordinary preschool/school. (41 Attendance at a special preschool attached to a special school. (51 A combination of any of the above. 53 It is particularly important that an openness be maintained as one important aim would be integration. An optional balance would have to be achieved between striving for integration and attempting to meet the child's special needs. for instance, at present some playgroups make a point of accepting handicapped children. this is a good thing, from the point of view both of providing the young children with social experience and the stimulation of normal children's company, and of introducing the other children early to the notion of handicap as a normal part of life. The danger is that specific educational and developmental needs which might call for a more specialist approach may be neglected and at a crucial period for learning. Training of Teachers Much of what has gone before has implications for the role of the teacher and training of teachers. Already in this country teachers are working in homes with infants and their families (e.g. the Visiting Teachers of the Visually and Hearing Impaired). Some beginnings are being made in viewing the teacher as a professional whose skills are useful in settings other than the classroom, and applicable to very, very young children. The question has to be faced, however, that if we are to meet the needs of those 20% of children, the ordinary teacher probably needs some training. We cannot hope that preschool education will do away with special needs later or that the Special Education Diploma will do it - at 40 per year, wewouldwait a longtime! some more economical system will have to be employed. Training is needed at both perservice and in-service levels. The preservice courses certainly would appearto be a useful starting point. In-service training might be organised along the lines of the pyramid model, where each person who receives training passes on skills learned to a group of others. The implications are far reaching. I hand over to the Conference for consideration this issue which is ultimately about teachers' own professional development. To conclude, I would like to quote this passage from the Warnock Report; it expresses very well my own feelings on this issue. 'Whatever else may come of our report. we hope that one thing will be clear. Special education is a challenging and intellectually demanding field for those engaged in it. More researc.h is needed, more expeflments in teaching techniques, in curriculum development, and in cooperation between different professions. "Those who work with children with special educational needs should regard themselves as having a crucial and developing role in a society which is now committed, not merely, to tending arId caring for its handicapped members, as a matter of charity, but to educating them as a matter of right and to developing their potential to the full.' 54 References SLUMS. S. ET AL The Portage Project: A Parent's Guide to Early Education, Cooperative Educational Service Agency 12. Portage, Wis.. U.S.A. DES Special Education needs (Warnock Report). HMSO. 1978. JEFFREE. O. ET AL Let Me Speak. Souvenir Press. 1976JEFFREE. O. ET AL Let Me Play. Souvenir Press. 1977. KIERNAN. C. ET. AL. Starting Off. Souvenir Press. 1978. LERNER, J. ET. AL. Special Education for the Early Years. Prentice Hall 1981. McCONKEY. R. & O·CONNOR. M. Putting Two Words Together. St. Michael's House, Dublin. 1978. MITTLER, PETER People Not Patients: Problems & policies in Mental Handicap, Methuen. 1979. NESC (50) Major Issues in Planning Services for Mentally & Physically Handicapped Persons. Stationery Office. Dublin. WOLFENDALE. S. & BRYANS. I. Idenfitication of Learning Difficulties: A Model for Intervention MARE 1978. 55 Home and school in the education of the young child Elizabeth McGovern Principal Teacher, Rutland Street Preschool Introduction The past two decades have witnessed a rapid growth in interest in early childhood education. Much of the impetus for this growth has come from an appreciation of the importance of the early years for later cognitive development. This appreciation has led, inevitably, to a consideration of the role of the home. There is a growing recognition that to consider the child in isolation from his family and community is not sufficient if long term educational goals are to be realized. The emphasis in much recent theorizing and practice has shifted, therefore, from the sole consideration of the child to a concern forthe adults surrounding him and to the relationship between them and the child. Schools are awakening to the need to be more accessible and more responsive to the family. Increasingly, the importance of parents as active partners with the school in the education of the young child is being acknowledged. In this paper I will first look at evidence which supports this welcome development. Then, I will review current attempts at promoting liaison between home and school. Finally, I will consider some practical implications for those of us dealing with the education of the young child. Why involve parents? While there is undoubtedly a growing general interest among teachers, psychologists, community workers, sociologists and policymakers in home-school liaison and the education of the young child, much ignorance persists concerning its importance. There is remarkably little consensus about what is involved. Certainly 'parental involvement' is a paradoxical and ambiguous concept. The 57 rights and duties of parents are enshrined in the Constitution and for most of our history the family's involvement in the young child's education required no justification. Indeed, for many, the introduction of compulsory schooling was seen as a revolutionary invasion of parental rights. Nevertheless, the past century has witnessed a dramatic change in the relative roles played by the family and nonfamily institutions in the education and welfare olthe young child. It is ironic that now, when more adults than ever before are literate, fewer are directly involved inthe educational process.Atthesametimesocial changes affecting the family as an institution are forcing parents into greater consciousness oftheir child-rearing practices. Furthermore, although much of the impetus for home-school collaboration in early childhood education has come from that section of education catering for children with special needs the disadvantaged and the physically and mentally handicapped - it is recognised that all children can benefit from such collaboration. To create the best possible learning climate for their pupils is a common goal ofthe teaching profession. It is clearthatthis cannot be achieved if the most profound influences in the young child's life - his home, family and parents - especially his mother - are ignored. The increaSing awareness of the need for co-operation between home and school has been prompted mainly by a growing body of research that highlights the part children's home environment plays in determining their school progress. In investigating the relationship between environmental factors and sCholastic ability and attainment much attention has been paid to social class membership. Numerous studies have shown substantial correlations between parental social class and intelligence and scholastic attainment on the one hand, and various characteristics of the home on the other hand. (Chazan, 1976, Davie, 1972, Douglas, 1964, Plowden, 1967, Kellaghan, 1977). Among the material circumstances in the home which have attracted attention are such variables as income level, family size and overcrowding. Not surprisingly, these have been found to have a significant impact on school performance. More crucial, perhaps, are cultural elements such as the quality of language used in the home, or its level of literacy. (Fraser, 1959, Plowden, 1967). Furthermore, several studies indicate that parental attitudes, motivation and contact with the school are factors also related to children's school progress (Douglas 1964, Floud, 1957). This research however, only partly explains the mechanisms by which people's class membership predispose them to educational success or failure. More relevant to teachers anxious to understand their pupils' home gackground and the effect it exercises on performance at school are studies which attempt to explore the early environment of the child, especially aspects of socialization such as the language, teaching style and control techniques of the mother. Although providing descriptions rather than explanations such research does indicate 58 that homes differ in certain respects. On the basis of such differences some consequences for cognitive development have been identified. The language of the family has been seen as an important medium by which differences in cognition are effected (Bernstein, 1960, 1971, Woohon, 1974). The child's orientation towards using language is induced by his mother's expectation of him and the talk he hears from her. If a mother fails to extend her child's language beyond his individual needs by responding minimally to his questions and comments, or, indeed, by leaving him for long periods in the company of his peers, the child will come to school with no sense of being listened to, talked to, or thought of as an individual (Newson, 1976, 1977). His level of language ability will compare unfavourably with that of the child whose mother welcomes and encourages his attempts at verbal interaction (Robinson, 1972, Tough, 1977, Wells, 1979). Related to this is the importance mothers attach to other aspects of childrearing. The extent to which they recognize that this involves helping the child develop an accurate picture of the world and a sense of mastery of his environment can vary considerably. Some mothers view the task of childrearing prinCipally in terms of physical care, and of bringing up the child "properly". Hence the child is largely the recipient of adult instructions. He is less frequently exposed to the language through which teaching is carried out. Other mothers, while sharing this concern for their children's physical well-being, also advance their awareness by provoking them to explore, to question, to solve problems and to use language as a means of thinking (Newson, 1968, Robinson, 1972, Tough, 1977, Woohon, 1974). Differences in the way parents control their children have also been observed. It is noted that some use techniques which discourage discussion and offer few opportunities for choice. Such techniques, it is argued, hinder cognitive growth which is fostered, rather, by opportunities to extract general principles from situations, to link cause and effect and to make choices. (Bernstein, 1960, Hess, 1967, Woohon, 1974). In other ways too, homes promote a range of school-related skills. Through participation in a high proportion of indoor activities, including being read to, some children come to school with a range of attitudes and skills which ensure early and easy adjustment to their new situation. Moreover, once there, some parents are better able to monitor their progress (Newson 1968, 1976). This brief review of aspects of home-background leads to the conclusion that the hidden curriculum of the homes of the children we teach d" ,er, and may, in many instances, be in conflict with. that of the school. There would appear to be good theoretical and empirical grounds for regarding domestic factors as crucial for educational success. These exercise a formidable influence on learning readiness and predispose the interaction of the young child with the school to a degree, we should not underestimate. The implications for those of us involved in early 59 childhood education are clear. We must seek to actively collabClrate with parents so that possible conflict between home and school is reduced. The home must be seen as a focus for educational intervention, and the early school years are an optimum time for such efforts. Formal academic demands are at their lowest, and most parents, for whom this period is a time for optimism, feel they can cope with any tasks the school may ask them to undertake. To answer the initial question then - why involve parents? We have seen that some parents for a variety of socio-cultural reasons fail to develop in their children the habits, skills and attitudes which facilitate learning and easy adjustment to school. Many more are not fully conscious of the significant contribution their continuing support and encouragement could make to their children's school performance. These parents need to be made aware of the aims and objectives of the school and of how they can complement the work of the school at home. Some may merely need to be alerted to their responsibilities, others will need additional help to assist them develop new skills and improve their existing ones. Without such active cooperation between school and home many of our pupils may never reach their full potential. A review of current home-school links Strategies which link home and school in joint educational effort take many forms. They run the gamut from simple exchange of information to serious attempts at aligning the values of the two institutions by seeking to influence home factors such as the motherchild interaction patterns thought to be responsible for school failure. For the purposes of this paper parental involvement will be considered under two main headings: Home-based and School-based strategies. Home-based strategies Lest we consider that home intervention is irrelevant to those of us who teach in schools, it must be stated at the outset that welldocumented work with parents and children in the home has provided much relevant information applicable to the school situation. Home visiting involves a series of regular visits to the home by a teacher or other professional or paraprofessional person. Methods of interacting wit.h the child and of teaching school-related skills and concepts are demonstrated (to the mother alone, or to the mother with the child). The hope is that shewill not only become a more competent teacher of her own children but that she will be enabled to deal more confidently with that part of her child's life represented by the school. While home-visiting programmes are used with school-going children -we are all familiar With visiting teachers ofthc hearing and visually impaired child - this approach is more frequently used with younger children, especially in disadvantaged communities. Irish 60 examples of the latter type have been the Rutland Street Pre-School Home Programme (Holland, 1979) and the Kilkenny Home Visiting Programme (Archer and Kellaghan 1975). The approach is also fairly widely used in other countries such as the United States, Britain and some European countries (Bronfenbrenner, 1975, Davies, 19B2, Smith, 1975, Tizard, 1975). An alternative approach is to invite the parent - usually the mother - to come on a regular basis to the school or other centre. Here, activities aimed at promoting the child's educational progress are demonstrated and discussed, and the necessary teaching materials are prepared. Through such techniques the parent is prepared forthe task of "teaching" units at home; she bears full responsibility for implementation. The regular session at the school, in addition to helping develop teaching skills, boosts parental confidence. Results from this approach are encouraging both with pre-schoolers, and in programmes aimed at older children. With this latter group, such 'home programmes usually focus on aspects of reading. It seems appropriate here to make a brief reference to occasional visits by class teachers to homes. Whilewe in Ireland aretaking some tentative steps in this direction, it is interesting to note that in some countries, notably the Soviet Union, class teachers are expected to visit homes at least once a year. While not advocating compulsory home visiting, I believe in the value of such visits, both from personal experience and from talking to teachers who visited homes - and survived! Handled properly a home visit can be mutually informative for teachers and parents, and does much to improve the quality ofthe teacher-pupil and teacher-parent relationship. I do think that the full benefit of a visit can only be truly appreciated by a teacher after the event. In my own school teachers visit the homes of all newly-enrolled children either before, or soon after the child enters school. They also seek other opportunities to make brief visits, for example, to ask about a sick pupil, or to take home a child not collected on time. Teachers in the field of early childhood education are presented with a variety of suitable opportunities for initiating visits to homes. Perhaps we should seriously consider availing of these opportunities, in some instances. at least. School-based strategies Strategies that link home and school are varied and happen at a variety of levels, both educational and non-educational. The recent INTO policy statement on home-school links provides a comprehensive review of current practice. Therefore, rather than reviewing specific approaches I wish instead to consider such strategies from a different perspective, namely, in relation to the varying roles they confer on parents. Generally speaking these can be categorized as parents as supporters, as learners, as teachers of their own children, as classroom helpers, and as management. Before 61 turning to these categories however, it is necessary to consider the warmth or openness of our schools and ourselves, as teachers, to parents. I am referring here to the atmosphere of tolerance and warmth we create, indeed must create if we are serious about involving parents and sharing with them the task of educating their children. The first and most essential ingredient of satisfactory teacherparent relations is that parents should be able to sense that the teacher is genuinely concerned for their children's welfare. Through frequent casual contacts, which the infant teacher usually has, parents have the opportunity to get to know the teacher as a person, seeing her in a variety of moods and predicaments. The sheer informality of such encounters robs them of any threat and helps build a relationship between the two. It is this relationship which is the prerequisite of satisfactory and effective home-school cOllaboration. We might ask ourselves the following questions. Do we leave parents standing outside the school gate? Have we notices displayed or implicit rules which say "No parents beyond this pOint"? Can we greet parents by name? Are we readily available for consultation? Do' parents spend time in our classrooms? I believe that we could do much to improve our schools in this regard. (without creating mayhem). Parents as supporters: Most teachers will recognise the "parents as supporters" role. Indeed, few schools could function properly without encouraging parents in this role, for example, helping on fund-raising committees, on outings, at concerts; providing materials for arts and crafts, making tea, mending books and equipment etc. Such contacts, although primarily of a non-educational nature, do confer on parents a specific recognizable role and a real feeling of being involved in the life of the school. Moreover, involvement of this type is often a stepping stonetotheir wider involvement in community affairs, especially in newly developed residential areas. Parents as learners: This way of viewing parents is probably the most relevant to us here. The underlying assumption we make is that parents who are familiar with what is happening in school are better able to reinforce and extend this work at home. Consequently, schools communicate indirectly with parents through letters, reports, information' leaflets and booklets, newsletters, and notice board displays. We expect them to act upon the information received. In . addition, parents are invited to meetings, films, discussion groups, talks by experts and to open days, where discussion on their children's progress is used. However, telling parents is not always enough, some need a more active approach such as demonstrations, or classes (e.g. explaining new Maths)andschools in increasing numbers are providing these facilities. 62 Parents as teachers of their own children: Helping parents to recognize their 'teaching role' is usually seen as a crucial component in maximizing their children's potential. Some parents of schoolgoing children abdicate their responsibility in this regard, leaving the full burden to fall on the "teacher-expert". Schools often need to remind and where necessary, inform parents of their primary role. More importantly, they need to create opportunities which help some parents assume that role. Where necessary they should provide advice or material to facilitate parental efforts. Homework seems an ideal device for parent-teacher collaboration in this category, involving, as it does, the parent working alongside the child on education activities. Parents as aides and volunteers. This is a role parents are beginning to fill, if only in a very limited way, in Irish schools, and is in my opinion, one worth encouraging. It can involve the parent in assisting the teacher by mending books, preparing art materials, and in supervising groups of children at various activities within the classroom, while the teacher works with individuals or small groups. Teachers can also use parents very successfully to direct art activities and to hear reading. The other obvious way of using parents as volunteers is to capitalize on their own skills or talents in a variety of areas such as hand-crafts, music or sport. Since the financial and other resources of most schools are limited, we should not neglect external assistance which may be available in the community. As in the case of home-visiting, any fears we might have soon disappear, and from personal observation the benefits are well worth the effort. Parents as management. Until recently, parents playing a role in policy-making or in management and planning is not something of which we, in Ireland, have had much experience. In some countries it is a recognized and accepted role for parents. Here, apart from some isolated instances and the involvement of parents on management committees, it is most frequently seen at pre-school level. It is a type of involvement we will probably see more of in the future. For the present, it is worth noting in passing, that writers on disadvantage stress this as a vital way of giving people a sense of mastery of their environment. It is claimed that only through such experiences will parents change the way they interact with their children, thereby facilitating their better adjustment to, and success at, school. From this review it is evident. I think, that schools are already doing much, both formally and informally. to foster home-school cooperation. Many teachers undervalue their own efforts and frequently fai I to perceive the fu II extent, or benefit of their day-to-day contacts with parents. Yet it is equally clear that we could do more no matter how restricted our resources and facilities. If we are serious about linking home and school we must become more professional in our approach. By structuring our thinking on the matter we will be better able to create opportunities which allow and encourage all 63 parents to playa role in their children's schooling according to their needs and ability. Furthermore, we will become more adept at utilizing situations which present themselves to us, and at gradually coaxing parents into new and more complex levels of involvement. Is home-school cooperation effective? Having considered the various types of situations when home and school can cooperate in the education of the young child, it is opportune to ask how effective are such attempts? Even where no formal evaluation is carried out, evidence based on observations from parents, teachers and children is positive. Evaluation of individual small-scale programmes do point to significant, measurable gains in children's ability and attainment. Large scale longitudinal studies of early education programmes are generally positive. (Bronfenbrenner, 1975; Kellaghan, 1977; Smith, 1975; Tizard, 1975). To paraphrase the main findings we see: - Home involvement strategies can have long-term impact on ch ildren, in terms of sustai ned gains in 10 and on the parents, both in relation to interaction with the child, and at a personal level. It can also have an impact on siblings and even on neighbouring children. - The effects in terms of measured 10 are greatest for children under school age. For the school-going child parental involvement has been proven to be a crucial component of successful programmes. Its importance appears to lie in its being a catalyst for sustaining and enhancing the effects ofthe school programmethroughoutthe child's school career. - Effects are diminished when parents fail to see themselves as having a central role in their children's educational development. - Finally, results from many studies indicate that home intervention, without other supporting services, has limited applicability and effectiveness with severly deprived families. Some Guidelines for planning home-school contacts From such evidence above, and from a general review of the literature it is possible to abstract some guidelines to help teachers plan effective home-school contacts. Briefly, these are: - parents must be viewed as educators with a central role to play in their children'S educational development; - parental confidence in their own abilities and worth is a crucial component of their competence in implementing anything the school might ask them to do; schools need to help and encourage parents to assume their responsibilities through collaboration rather than by coercion; -the sort of activities through which we choose to promote cooperation should where possible, be closely related to the 64 curriculum the child is following in school; where possible too, they should involve the parent and child working together in situations which promote sustained verbal interaction; - guidelines and advice given should be specific ratherthan general, be individual to the child's needs and be presented actively if possible; - evaluation, however simple, must play an integral part; only inthis way can impressionistic evidence about effects be verified, but more importantly, it involves a continuous struggle to solve theoretical and practical problems. The role of the teacher In the time that remains it is appropriate to reconsider an issue central to the whole home-school liaison debate, that is, the role of the teacher. While there is general agreement that the early school years is an optimum time for such activity this does place a heavy burden on the already over-worked teacher. She is paramount in bringing parents to an early awareness of their crucial role in their children's educational development. She has to find ways of awakening, harnessing and directing parental interest and energy. Not only must she be able to give specific detailed information to parents on educational matters, but she is often expected to be an expert on physical, social and other problems as well. She also needs to be a good listener and be able to extract from and act upon information parents have about their own children. In order that teachers may begin totreatthe child and his family as a unit they may need additional skills. More initial and inservice training are required to acquaint both trainee and professional teacher of current trends and findings in sociology, psycholinguistics and psychology, so that they may develop a fuller awareness of, and empathy with, the life-style of the pupils in their care. Teachers need to develop skills which help them to motivate and communicate with adults of differing levels of education, to work with professionals from other disciplines, to diagnose problems and to select suitable stragegies for solving these problems. In addition, they need time to reflect on what they are doing and to share experiences with colleagues in similar situations. In particular, they merit the support and encouragement of principal teachers who must be prepared to facilitate their efforts at home-school liaison. Regarding this last point, it is obvious that leadership must come from principals and inspectors. The momentum within a school must be initiated and sustained by the principal. A whole-school policy needs to be adopted so that parents can experience consistency in home-school collaboration and, equally, so that the efforts of individual teachers are not to be dissipated when their pupils move on to other teachers. Perhaps school staffs should ~gree a minimum standard of involvement, e.g. (i) to promote a welcoming and tolerant 65 attitude towards parents, (ii) to open and maintain communication channels between school and home, (iii) to establish times when teachers are available for individual consultation, (iv) to hold open days - once a year etc. Teachers who wish to attempt additional forms of liaison or who are already doing so should be encouraged to do so. Conclusion In conclusion, this paper has been a plea for greater home-school liaison in Irish schools particularly for the younger children. It has reviewed research which underlines the important role home background factors play in determining the young child's response to learning. Practical steps which can be taken by teachers to improve parents' contribution to their children'S educational careers have been discussed and guidelines to help structure such efforts have been listed. We have also seen that one can be modestly optimistic about the outcome of any attempts made in this area. Our hope is that both teachers and parents will rise to the challenge. What seems certain is that parents whose role in their children's education has been high-lighted and nurtured by home-school cooperation in the early years will be unwilling to retreat for the rest of the children's schooling to as passive a role as they have formerly been accorded or have themselves adopted. Thus, time spent working with them now may not only pay current dividends but future ones, as well. References ARCHER. P. and KELLAGHAN. T. "A home Intervention Project for Pre-School Disadvantaged Children", Irish Journal of Education. IX (1975): 5-27. BERNSTEIN, B. "Language and Social CLass", British Journal of Sociology. XI 11960): 271·306. BERNSTEIN. B. Class. Codes and Control. London: Routledge & Kegan Paul, 1971 II), 1973 {/I). BRONFENBRENNER, U. "Is Early Intervention Effective?", In M. Guttentag and E.L. Struening rEds.) Handbook of Evaluation Research (Beverly H;tJs: Sage Publications, 1975. CHAZAN, M.: LAING. A.: COX. T.: JACKSON. S.: and LLOYD. G. Deprivation and School Progress. Oxford: Basil Blackwell for the Schools Council Publications, 1976. DAVIE. R.; BUTLER, N.; and GOLDSTEIN, H. From Birth to Seven: A report ofthe National Child Development Study. London: Longman 1972. DAVIES, A., ed. Language and Learning in Home and School. London: Heinemann Educational Books, 1982. DOUGLAS, J.W.B. The Home and the School. London: McGibbon and Kee, 1964. FLOUD. J. HALSEY, A.H.; and MARTIN, F.M, Social Class and Educational Opportunity. London: Heinemann, 1957. 66 FRASER, E. Home Environment and the School. London: University of London Press, 1959. GREAT BRITAIN, DEPARTMENT OF EDUCATION & SCIENCE, Children and their Primary Schools. A Report of the Central Advisory Council for Education (England). London: H.M.S. 0" 1967. HESS, R.D. and SHIPMAN, V.C. "Cognitive Elements in Maternal Behaviour". In J.P. Hill (Ed), Minnesota Symposia on Child Psychology, Vol. I Minneapolis: University of Minnesota Press, 1967. HOLLAND, S. Rutland Street. The Story of an Educational Experiment for Disadvantaged Children in Dublin. The Hague: Bernard van Leer Foundation. and Oxford' Pergamon Press, 1979. KELLAGHAN. T. The Evaluation of an Intervention Programme for Disadvantaged Children. Slough: NFER Publishing, 1977. KELLAGHAN. T.. "Relationships between Home Environment and Scholastic Behaviour in a Disadvantaged Population", Journal of Educational Psychology, LXIX (1977): 754-760. NEWSON, J. and NEWSON, E. Four Year Old in an Urban Community. London: Allen & Unwin, 1968. NEWSON, J. and NEWSON. E. Seven Years Old in the Home Environment. London: Allen & Unwin. 1976. NEWSON. J. and NEWSON. E. Perspectives on School at Seven Vears Old. London: Allen & Unwin. 1971ROBINSON, W.P. and RACKSTRAW, F.J. A Question of Answers, Volumes 1 and 2. London: Routledge & Kegan Paul, 1972. SMITH. G .• ED. Educational Priority. volume 4: The West Riding EPA. London: H.M.S.O" 1975. TIZARD, B. Early Childhood Education. A Review and Discussion of Research in Britain. Slough: NFER Publishing Co., 1975. TOUGH, J. "Children and Programmes: How Should We Educate the Young Child". In A. Davies (Ed), Language and Learning in Early Childhood. London: Heinemann. 1977. WELLS. G. "Describing Children's Linguistic Development at Home and at Schoor. British Educational Research Journal. V (1979): 75·98. WOOTtON, A,J, "Talk in the Homes of Young Children", Sociology, VIII (1974): 227-295. 67 Preschools, playschools, nursery schools and classes Kathleen Day Former Principal Teacher, Rutland Street Preschool The organisation of official educational provision for young children in the Republic of Ireland has been relatively uncomplicated because of: (i) the centralised control of education; (ii) the education of young children as part of primary education; (iii) the uniformity of teacher qualifications at primary level, and (iv) the age of entry to formal school. A look at our educational history shows that (iv) age of entry has been the subject of attention from time to time. Compulsory attendance at school for children from 6-14 was introduced in the Republic from 1 January 1927 under the provisions of the School Attendance Act, 1926. From Dr. J. Coolahan's article in the INTO journal (Vol. 26. No.3, 1982) we learn of an even older tradition - that of sending quite young children to school. Shortly after the issuing of the 1930 Rules for National Schools, the Minister for Education revised Rule 60(i) which had read: "No child under 3 years of age may be enrolled as a pupil in any national school." This was changed in 1934 and further amended in 1946 as follows: 1934 (March) Revision of Rules and Regulations, Rule 60(i) amended to read as follows: "No child under 4 years of age may be enrolled as a pupil in any national school. "* (1946) Revision of Rules and Regulations, amendment of Rule 60(i) "No child under 4 years of age may be allowedto attend a national school or to be enrolled as a pupil therein. .Unlike the general European pattern, we have a strong tradition of school attendance among the pre-compulsory age group, whereby about 85% of those aged 4 and 5 are in regular attendance in both urban and rural areas. (Coolahan, 1982). *No. of children involved, about 7,000. 69 Percentage of children attending a pre-school establishment 3. 4 and 5 years 70.00% Belgium 5 years 4 years 3 years 100.00% 97.00% 90.00% Denmark 6 years 5 and 6 years 3, 4, 5 and 6 years 87.30% 60.00% 35.00% Federal Republic of Germany France 5 4 3 2 years years years years 100.00% 97.00% 80.00% 26.00% Ireland 5 years 4 years 95.00% 65.00% 3, 4. and 5 years 70.38% Luxembourg 5 years 4 years 100.00% 90.00% Netherlands 5 years 4 years 97.00% 93.00% United Kingdom (England and 5 4 3 2 years years years years 5 4 3 2 years years years years Italy Wales) (Scot/and) Full-time 100.00% 39.80% 2.80% 0.30% Part-time 10.3% 7.6% 0.2% Nursery class Primary school 97.00% less than 1% 23.44% 12.00% 11.00% less than 1% (excluding playgroups) Extract from "Preschool Education in the European Community" Education Series No. 12. Brussels. September, 1979. If the term .preschool child is taken as a generic term to cover all children who have not yet enrolled in a formal school system, it applies to a different age range in different countries. In Denmark, children of 6 are not yet all in school - in Britain, Luxembourg, Belgium and France all children of 5 years attend an educational establishment. A strategy for dealing with the educational provision for young children in some European countries has been the gradual lowering of the age of compulsory school attendance. 70 Of the terms included in the title ofthis paper - and other speakers have shown us how imprecise the terminology is since the same term means different things in different countries - that with the oldest tradition would seem to be the nursery school. Until recent years it was a common assumption that the British nursery school as it evolved was archetypically the institution for the education of young children once they were old enough to leave home. Yet a glance at a history of education reveals that:(i) the demand for nursery schools was related tothe enforcement of rules for school attendance (ii) the growth of nursery school provision was limited and sporadic, and (iii) nursery schools were separate from primary schools. In Great Britain, the 1870 Act gave Boards of Education the power to introduce bye-laws to insist on attendance at school for children from 5-13. The 1920 Education Act made the schools the responsibility of the Local Education Authorities and by this time the tradition of attendance at school by children as young as 3 years was well established. Figures for 1900 show that 43% of all 3-5 year olds were in school - "a higher proportion than were attending nursery and reception classes today"' (Hughes, 1980). These children were predominantly working class. The great majority of middle and upper class children were being cared for by nannies and nurse maids in household nurseries. At the end of the last century the number of nannies in employment is quoted as half a million. At this time, in Ireland, girls read in their school text book that nursery education was defined "as consisting chiefly in the judicious management of diet, cleanliness, clothing, atmospherical temperature, respiration, muscular exercise, sleep and the animal passions" (HMSO, 1873)and nursery education was understood as taking place in the child's own home. In Britain in 1905 inspectors of the Boards of Education recommended the exclusion of children of under 5 from school and complete discretion in the matter was given to the local authorities without any stipulation that the change in the age of admission be dependent on alternative arrangements being made. By 1926 the number of under 5's attending school had fallen to 13%. In 1907 the consultative committee of the Boards of Education was asked to consider the need for making some provision for young children whose home conditions were poor, the ideal institution for such children and the advantages to be derived from attending at such institutions which were to be called nursery schools. As a result of its enquiry, the committee reported that though the ideal place for a child under 5 was at home, "the economic and social conditions of large numbers of children were such that special nursery schools were best for them .. ' Their recommendations that such schools should be attached to the public elementary schools 71 separate from them but nonetheless an integral part of them - was significant and farseeing. The fragmentation of educational systems may not always have been a good thing and indeed in the past decade much thought is being given in many countries to the integration of educational provision. "No profound impact is likely from any programme as long as it remains isolated from the mainstream of educational provision and is not planned as part of it." (Shipman). As previously stated the development of nursery schools has been inconsistent. By the Education Act 1918 local authorities were empowered to supply nursery school places for children over 2 and under 5 "whose attendance at such is necessary or desirable for their healthy, physical and mental development." Unfortunately this Act. while it gave recognition to the separate work and identity of the nursery school also coloured attitudes as to the role of nursery education. The result was that by 1921 an anomaly had been created within the education act of that year whereby the nursery school was classed as a special school out of the mainstream of the educational system. The situation has not improved with the passing of time because although the policy of the central government may have been to back the expansion of nursery education, local authorities were not obliged to adopt such a policy. During World War II the Ministry of Health encouraged local authorities to open day nurseries so that mothers could contribute to the war effort. Day nurseries in peace times were not run by education departments but by the Departments of Health and Social Services. Day nurseries were not schools. They kept children for the whole day and their personnel were trained with a bias towards health and nursing care rather than education. The few day nurseries which exist in this country, while not under the aegis of the Department of Education, include some educational activity appropriate for the age level of the children attending. In the immediate post war years there was a great deal of heated controversy, even in educational circles about whether the best place for a child under 5 was at home or in a nursery school. The World Health Organisation in 1951 stated "that the use of day nurseries and creches has a particularly serious and permanently deleterious effect on children". Certainly in the poorer urban areas in Britain "there was a fairly widely held feeling that there was a social stigma about trying to get one's child into a nursery. Nursery schools were regarded traditionally as necessary provisions for deprived children rather than a desirable educational provision." (Cave). Perhaps that ,cadition owed something to the fact that the concern shown by the McMillan sisters for deprived preschool children first brought nursery education to the fore in public consciousness A circular issued by the Ministry of Education in May 1960 stated "no resources can at present be spared for the expansion of nursery education and, in particular, no teachers can be spared who might 72 otherwise work with children of compulsory school age." It was not until 1967 with the publication of the report of the Plowden Committee that the demand for nursery education in Britain got a tremendous impetus. The report recommended that nursery education should be available to all those who wish for it". It is possible to learn a lot about the viscissitudes of the nursery school without getting a clear picture of what a nursery school is or of what its function is. 'The word 'school' is misleading. There are no formal lessons but children from 2-5 are supervised by trained staff and given a stimulating environment in which, through selfdiscovery, they can broaden their outlook" (Van der Eyken, 1967). "A nursery school is a small world run entirely for children, with furniture and fittings of the right size, in which the grown ups only business is that of providing such surroundings as will give every child a chance of becoming a happy, creative, independent and useful person" (Nursery Association of Britain and Northern Ireland). The most recently opened nursery schools are custom built with an outdoor playing area, open for the normal school year and staffed by a teacher who is supported by nursery assistants. Teachers get either a 3 or 4 year college training or a one-year post graduate qualification rather like a primary teacher's education in this country. Nursery assistants are usually trained by the National Nursery Examination Board which was set up for that purpose in 1944. The average sized nursery school has 2 or 3 nursery classes with about 52 places per school. The Department of Education and Science lays down standards for premises, staffing, equipment and curriculum. A staff child ratio of 1:13 is considered acceptable and one full time nursery nurse (or 2 students) is recommended. A strategy for circumventing the problem of scarce resources is the nursery class. This is a class mainly for 4 year olds attached to infant schools where children of preschool age are introduced to an educational environment. The nursery class was suggested in the 1944 Education Act as a cheaper way of increasing the provision of nursery education. Numbers attending must not exceed 30 for which is provided one teacher and one full-time nursery assistant. Another idea for the conservation of resources is the nursery annex whereby the nursery school opens 'branch' classes some distance from the main building in heavily populated areas and these are served by peripatetic teachers. The ethos of the nursery school was traditionally governed by the belief that a good environment in which the young child's needs can be satisfied under the care of understanding adults was sufficient in launching each child on the world to eventually achieve his fulf potential. The existing model available for some of the Head Start programmes was the BritiSh nursery school and as Dr. Kellaghan has pointed out in hi; paper this proved inadequate for the population at which Head Start was aimed. Social and emotional development was necp.ssary of course for all children who were preparing for formal 73 1 schooling but there were a lot of school relevant skills to be learned as well. These were hardly likely to 'unfold' in a setting, however benign, without the conscious intervention and direction of a teacher. By school relevant skills are meant the skills appropriate to a child's age and ability level which helps him to gain maximum benefit from school. Some of these could be listed as follows: the ability to use and understand language, the ability to listen and reflect, the ability to ask for help i.e. use adults as resources, and the ability to persist with a task. These are skills which are established at an early age. Bereiter and Engelmann were early critics of what it was felt was the almost laissez-faire approach of the nursery school ethos, and they set oullO introduce clearly stated goals and structure into what children were being taught. The Rutland Street Preschool Centre opened in Dublin in 1969 and was the only government funded model of a preschool in a disadvantaged area in the Republic. From the outset it adopted a structured approach. It had a curriculum modelled on what had been established as good preschool practice in intervention projects in the U.S.A. Its teachers were specially trained, their goals were clear and parental participation was considered essential. (Kellaghan, 1977, Holland, 1981). Undoubtedly the most extreme departure from tradition was that of Bereiter and Engelmann, and although many teachers of young children would reject it as beinga 'pressure cooker' technique, it caused many to question their long cherished assumptions about the nursery school and to look again at the characteristics and needs of the young child. The White Paper in Britain, (1972) announced the intention of expanding nursery provision and a plan to set up a research programme to monitor the development of the new provision. The Social Sc.ience Research Council then invited Dr. Barbara Tizard to review current research on preschool education in the U.K. and the Scottish Council for Research and Education commissioned Mrs. Jennifer Haystead to examine the demand for and supply of preschool education. The greater part of funds allocated by the S.S.R.C. was awarded to Professsor Bruner of the Oxford Preschool Research Unit in 1975. The Schools Council project on preschool education was an attempt to justify the traditional aspects of nursery schools and to define what was 'deemed good practice in 1970' and to study techniques used in different establishfT'°nts for the education of young children. For all of us, parents and teachers with a special interest in the education of the young, there is now much valuable documentation on public library shelves. Nursery schools in Ireland, where they exist, are privately run feepaying institutions which seem to be organised along Froebel or Montessori lines. There were in 1980, 34 schools affiliated to the Montessori Association. Some of these nurseries are attached to 74 private primary schools run by religious orders. It is not surprising that the lack of places in nursery schools for under fives in Britain sparked off a self-help effort amongst parents. A letter to the Guardian in 1961 began what has been called the 'miracle of the post-war years' - the growth of the playgroup. The playgroup is a group normally run by parents who charge a small fee for each child attending, to provide a degree of recreation and stimulus for local children. There is no formal teaching. Playgroup sessions usually last 2-3 hours on 4-5 mornings a week. The movement mushroomed and a national structure emerged, All those involved believed that the groups were a stop gap for state nurseries. Some see its roots in the co-operative nursery schools organised as far back as the 20s in the U.S. on university campuses. Professor Bruner remarks "No country in the world has achieved anything like it."ln Australia the playgroup movement was also brought into being by parents' initiative but was soon supported by the state, proportionately to the financial contribution of parents. In the U.S. parents' participation is largely restricted tofund raising by pressure groups like P.T.A. Some states provide tra i n ing for parents. A three year fu II-ti me tra in i ng is provided for playgroup leaders alongside teacher training in a training college. The playgroup movement is greatly influenced by the Play Centre Movement of New Zealand. Parents initiate the play centre and a remarkable spontaneity is maintained by a regular handing over of the groups to the oncoming generation of parents. In Britain the first national adviser to the playgroup movement was appointed with a grant from D.E.S. and since 1972 the Department of Social Services is responsible for the registration of playgroups. Who is entitled to run a playgroup in the Republic of Ireland? Under the existing statutory arrangements anyone can set up a playgroup without registration or any other form of statutory control. The Irish Preschool Playgroups Association was set up almost 20 years ago and it is affiliated to a similar group in Britain and Northern Ireland. It has consistently called for statutory registration of playgroups and in the absence of such legislation it offers its members advice, training, insurance, a news-sheet, suggestions as to activities and equipment together with voluntary registration. Some see the movement as a vehicle for parent power. The aims of the I.P.P.A. are as follows: to assist the promotion of preschool education in Ireland; to seek a nationally recognised training course; to set and maintain a code of standards for playgroups; to hold meetings, lectures, seminars and to servicE> the demand of members for training courses; to increase public awareness of the needs of the preschool child and the value of playgroups; and to cooperate with other organisations and persons engaged in similar activities. The Minister for Education has requested the V.E.Cs to assist in 75 , training playgroup leaders. "The foundation course in the North Strand vocational school has been awarded a certificate by Dublin City V.E.C. The proposed foundation course in Ballyfermot Senior Cycle College will be awarded a certificate from Maynooth College. Certification for these two courses comes from the academic body in association with LP.P.A. Limerick's foundation course is progressing well but certification has still to be finalised." (Extract from address of the chairman of LP.PA 1982). Playgroups held in private homes are normally fee-paying while community playgroups, generally staffed on a voluntary basis, charge nominal fees. Mobile playgroups in converted buses are operated by Dr. Barnardos, LS.P.C.C. and by a supermarket chain. The I.P.PA has 767 members representing 692 playgroups in every county in the country. The adult to child ratio in affiliated playgroups is 1:846.7% of playgroups have parent participation, 74% take handicapped children. The association has been given a government grant to enable it to run a Dublin office and employ a national advisor. The following is an extract from the Minister for Education's address to an O.M.E.P. (international preschool organisation) seminar in May 1982. "The involvement of the child's family as an active participant is critical to the success of any early childhood intervention programme. Therefore it would seem than any future developments in the education of young children should have as a major objective the development of the self confidence of parents". In a reference to empty classrooms in Dublin city schools he said "I would like to see groups of parents and community interests organise playgroups for young children in such classes." There are already a few playgroups in national schools, both in well established and developing areas. Their organisation and funding are separate, of course. An Comhchoiste l'Ieamhscolafochta was set up in March 1978. It is a joint committee for the promotion of schooling through Irish, between Bord na Gaeilge and the voluntary organisation Na naionrai Gaelacha. It is funded by Bord na Gaeilge. In April 1982 there were one hundred Irish speaking groups involved with over 20 in Gaeltacht areas. Practically all of the scoileanna lan-gaelacha have a naionra attached and the children come from 90% English speaking homes. Each naionra has an adult to child ratio of 8: 1 and with each there is a stiuirtheoir and 11 cuntoir (director and assistant). As well as instant . where mothers acting as individuals or as groups set l!P playgroups other caring associations such as LS.P.C.C. or Dr. Bernardos give help in certain areas in setting up playgroups for children 'at risk'. Dr. Bernardos employ three community playgroup advisors in the Dublin area. They have a playbus which is set up as a playgroup venue and also used to provide practical training for mothers. It has in the past year helped to start two community playgroups and to provide playing facilities for children of travellers. 76 These also have a toy library in a mother and toddler setting. In recent times Health Boards around the country have shown a willingness to help in the setting up of playgroups for children at risk. Both the playgroups and the naionrai insist that their chief aim is to provide play opportunities.. They do not teach and they make no claim to be a substitute for school. We, as teachers, may have something to learn from the institutions mentioned in this paper af"'d we certainly should have something to contribute to the debate on the educational needs of children who are not yet enrolled in school. It is worth noting again the stunted growth of nursery education because it catered for children below compulsory school age. Preschool provision of its nature will be random, disperse and selective - only within a global framework such as that of the national school system can a comprehensive facility of universal access be guaranteed. Our school system has failed to comprehend the implications of contemporary judgements on early childhood education. As Barbara Tizard says "The pedagogy of the early years is as yet undeveloped." It is good that researchers here are now turning their attention towhat is happening in the infant school. Recent work by Archer and O'Rourke concerns the educational experiences of four-to-six-year-olds at present in national schools. If the school system seeks to develop a broader view of educationwhich is part of the theme of the papers in this seminar -the system will have to change. Authority patterns will be altered in some way. There will be a danger of role confusion. What is really crucial is that the people within the system will have to change. There will have to be changes on the part of classroom teachers, principals, inspectors, training college personnel and parents. Everybody's expectations will have to alter. References ARCHER P. and O'ROURKE B. Teaching Practice and Infant Classrooms - a survey. Educational Research Centre, 1981. COOLAHAN J. Who Makes the Rules for National Schools? - article in An Mllimeoir Naisiunta. Vol. 26. No.3 pub. I.NTO 1982. COMMISSIONERS FOR NATIONAL EDUCATION. English Reader for Girls. 1873. SHIPMAN MARTIN. The Limits of Positive Discrimination. CARE R.G. Partnership for Change: Parents and Sc!1o,oJs Ward Lock EduC8.tiqn. VAN DER EYKEN W. The Pre-school Years, Pelican 1967. NURSERY SCHOOLS ASSOCIATION OF GREAT BRITAIN AND NORTHERN IRELAND, Oxford Branch Leaflet No.2. BEREITER C. and ENGELMANN S. Teaching Disadvantaged Children in the preschool. Englewood Cliffs. New Jersey: Prentice-Hall 1966. HOLLAND S. Rutland Street Project Pergammon Press 1981. KELLAGHAN T. The Evaluation of an Intervention Programme for Disadvantaged Children NFER Co. 1977. TIZARD B. Early Childhood Education NFER pub. Co. 1974 PARRY M. and ARCHER H. Two to Five - a handbook for students and teachers pub. Schools Council 1975. 77 Appendix EARLY CHILDHOOD EDUCATION SEMINAR: 11112 June 1982 Programme Friday 11th June: 3.00 - 3.45 p.m. 4.00 p.m. Lecture: "'Education and the Psychological and Chair: 5.30 p.m. 7.00 p.m. Registration. Physical Development of Young Children" Dr. Anne McKenna, Lecturer in Psychology. University College Dublin. Mr. Morgan O'Connell, Vice-President, INTO. Meal. Lecture: "Trends in Early Childhood Education". Dr. Thomas Kellaghan. Director, Educational Research Centre, St". Patrick's College, Drumcondra. Chair: 8.30 p.m. Discussion. 9.00 p.m. Adjourn. Saturday 12th June: 9.00 a.m. 9.15 a.m. 9.50 a.m. Assembly. Lecture: "Schooling for Young Children in Infant Classes in Primary Schools". Ms. Siobhan Hurley, Lecturer, Mary Immaculate College, Limerick. Chair: 78 Mr. Gerry Quigley, General Secretary, INTO. Mr. John White. INTO Central Executive Committee. Workshop Session. 10.20 a.m. Report and General Discussion. 10.50 a.m. Coffee. 11.10 a.m. Lecture "Home and School in the Education of the Young Child". Ms. Elizabeth McGovern, Principal Rutland Street Preschool, formerly teacher Chair: responsible for home/school liaison. Mr. Tom Gilmore. INTO Central Executive Committee. 11.45 a.m. Workshop Session. 12.20 p.m. Report and General Discussion. J2.45 p.m. Lunch. 2.00 p.m. Lecture: "Early Education for Children with Special Needs". Ms. Anne O'Sullivan, Lecturer in Special Education, St. Patrick's College, Drumcondra. Chair: Mr. Brendan Gilmore. INTO Central Executive Committee. 2.35 p.m. Workshop Session. 3.05 p.m. Report and General Discussion. 3.30 p.m. Lecture: "Preschools. Playschools, Nursery-schools and Classes". Mrs. Kathleen Day, former Principal of Rutland Street Preschool. Chair: Miss Raisin Carabine. INTO Central Executive Committee. 4.15 p.m. Coffee. 4.30 p.m. - Plenary "The Future Development of Education for Young Children". Panel 5.30 p.m. Session: Chair: Mr. Morgan INTO. O·Connell. Vice-President 79
© Copyright 2024