INT’L. J. AGING AND HUMAN DEVELOPMENT, Vol. 57(2) 181-201, 2003 STRENGTHS AND SATISFACTION ACROSS THE ADULT LIFESPAN* DEREK M. ISAACOWITZ Brandeis University GEORGE E. VAILLANT Brigham and Women’s Hospital MARTIN E. P. SELIGMAN University of Pennsylvania ABSTRACT Positive psychology has recently developed a classification of human strengths (Peterson & Seligman, in press). We aimed to evaluate these strengths by investigating the strengths and life satisfaction in three adult samples recruited from the community (young adult, middle-aged, and older adult), as well as in the surviving men of the Grant study of Harvard graduates. In general, older adults had higher levels of interpersonal and self-regulatory strengths, whereas younger adults reported higher levels of strengths related to exploring the world. Grant study men tended to report lower strength levels than older adults from the community. Among the young adults, only hope significantly predicted life satisfaction, whereas among the middle-aged individuals, the capacity for loving relationships was the only predictor. Among community-dwelling older adults, hope, citizenship, and loving relationships all positively and uniquely predicted life satisfaction, compared with loving relationships and appreciation of beauty in the Grant sample. *This work was supported by grants from the Positive Psychology Network, the Max Planck Society, and the Society for a Science of Clinical Psychology to the first author. 181 Ó 2003, Baywood Publishing Co., Inc. 182 / ISAACOWITZ, VAILLANT AND SELIGMAN STRENGTHS AND SATISFACTION ACROSS THE ADULT LIFESPAN The burgeoning field of positive psychology has sought recently to identify a measurable set of human strengths or virtues (Peterson & Seligman, in press). The goal of such a classification is presumably the development of assessment tools for each strength, followed by clarification of the precursors and consequences of each strength. An eventual goal is intervention to build strengths. We believe that human strengths can only be fully understood when studied for changes and continuity across the lifespan. Just as it would appear ridiculous to try to measure and foster cognitive skills similarly in 6-year-olds and 86-year-olds, positive psychologists should not consider curiosity, or self-control, static across the adult lifespan. The most obvious reason for this is that strengths reveal themselves in behaviors, and behaviors may be seen differently when performed in individuals of different ages. For example, the same actions may be viewed by others as reflecting curiosity when performed by a young adult, but as reflecting forgetfulness and senility when performed by an older individual (see Erber, Szuchman, & Prager, 2001 for an example of differences in how people view behaviors of individuals of various ages). On a more complex level, theories of adult development may suggest differences in how much of a particular strength adults of different ages should possess, and how helpful it should be for them to possess that strength. Erikson’s psychosocial stage theory of development (Erikson, 1950), for example, suggests that generative skills increase from youngto middle-adulthood; furthermore, generativity should be more adaptive to the middle-aged than to the younger or older adult if indeed it is the core psychosocial task for that age group. Thus, there are two main lifespan developmental issues to be addressed. The first is the issue of levels: do individuals of different ages have different mean levels of each particular strength? While cross-sectional data cannot distinguish between age and cohort effects to explain mean differences between age groups, they are nonetheless an important first step in establishing a lifespan framework for understanding strengths. The second issue is life satisfaction and well-being. While strengths are valued in their own right (see Peterson & Seligman, in press), it is likely that some strengths relate positively to life satisfaction and well-being. Furthermore, a strength may relate strongly to well-being at one life stage (when it matches the developmental tasks of that time), but relate less strongly or not at all to well-being at other times in the adult lifespan. An Example: Hope and Optimism Looking at optimism in more detail highlights important lifespan issues relevant to the study of strengths. There are two main ways of defining and measuring optimism. According to Scheier and Carver (1985), dispositional optimism involves an individual’s general expectancy that good things will happen to them AGING AND HUMAN STRENGTHS / 183 in the future. While studies have investigated the relationship of dispositional optimism and well-being in groups ranging from college students (Vickers & Vogeltanz, 2000) to middle-aged women (Bromberger & Matthews, 1996) to older caregivers (Robinson-Whelen, Kim, MacCallum, & Kiecolt-Glaser, 1997), age differences in levels of dispositional optimism have been studied only very recently. Isaacowitz (2001) assessed dispositional optimism and pessimism in young, middle-aged, and older adults. Older adults scored higher on both dispositional optimism and pessimism, which are independent (Mroczek, Spiro, Aldwin, Ozer, & Bossé, 1993), than did younger individuals, and this finding remained significant even after controlling for self-reported health. The gist of the well-being findings is that either more dispositional optimism or less dispositional pessimism usually predicts positive psychological and physical outcomes; however, some studies find only effects of optimism, while others find effects only with pessimism (e.g., Robinson-Whelen et al., 1997; Vickers & Vogeltanz, 2000). What has not varied, however, is that more dispositional optimism and/or less dispositional pessimism have always predicted positive outcomes, when they predicted any outcomes at all. According to explanatory style theory, the second optimism construct in the literature, an optimist tends to explain negative events as being caused by external, temporary, and specific factors. In contrast, a pessimist attributes the negative events to internal, stable and global causes (Abramson, Seligman, & Teasdale, 1978; Peterson & Seligman, 1984; Seligman, 1990). Older individuals tend to report more optimistic explanatory style in some domains than do younger people. For example, older individuals have a more optimistic explanatory style for affiliation events than do younger adults (Isaacowitz, 2001). However, whereas clear evidence exists linking optimistic explanatory styles and well-being in young people (see, for example, Metalsky, Abramson, Seligman, Semmel, & Peterson, 1982), the evidence from older individuals is mixed. Optimistic explanatory style may even predict declining well-being in older individuals experiencing several stressful life events in a short period of time (Isaacowitz & Seligman, 2001; Isaacowitz & Seligman, 2002; cf. Kamen-Siegel, Rodin, Seligman, & Dwyer, 1991; see Isaacowitz & Seligman, 2003, for a discussion). Research on optimism in adulthood and old age therefore illustrates how there may be shifts in both the mean levels of a strength and the strength’s relationship to well-being across the adult lifespan, and even these may differ depending on how the strength is measured. Classifying and Measuring Strengths The current attempt by the positive psychology movement to measure human strengths has gone through several iterations in its first few years. The original classification, referred to as the Wellsprings of a Positive Life, consisted of 16 potential strengths, a list which has evolved into the current 24 strengths included 184 / ISAACOWITZ, VAILLANT AND SELIGMAN in the VIA classification (Peterson & Seligman, in press; see Appendix A for a comparison of the 16- and 24-strength lists). Early in this process, the Gallup Organization, in collaboration with two of the current study authors, developed “The Wellsprings Questionnaire,” a self-report measure based on the original list of 16 strengths. The questionnaire contained five items per strength, and individuals responded on a Likert scale from 1 to 5 concerning how much each strength-relevant statement applied to them. The data reported in the current study was collected using the 16-strength questionnaire in two different samples of adults. In order to keep up with the evolving classification of strengths, we attempted to map the items on the 16-strength questionnaire to the 24-strength VIA classification (Peterson & Seligman, in press). Two very different groups of adults completed this questionnaire: the first group included the surviving members of the Grant Sample of Harvard graduates (see Vaillant, 1977; Vaillant & Mukamal, 2001; Vaillant & Vaillant, 1990), a group of men selected from the sophomore class at Harvard between 1940 and 1942 because they seemed healthy and likely to succeed in life. At the time they completed the questionnaire, most of the Grant Study men were in their late 70s and early 80s. The second sample was a community sample of young, middle-aged and older adults, primarily from the Philadelphia area. This sample was obviously different than the Grant Study men in more ways than simply being younger. Sample participants were also less educated, more likely to be female, and more ethnically diverse than the Grant Study participants. Goals of the Current Study With these two very different samples, we hoped to begin to unravel the story of human strengths in adulthood and old age. While our analyses were primarily exploratory in nature, we did have several more specified hypotheses, in line with current theory on adult development and aging: 1. Young adulthood is a time when individuals explore their interpersonal environment and try to find their socioemotional niche in the world (e.g. Erikson, 1950). Therefore, we expect young adults to report the highest levels of strengths that have to do with exploring the world, such as originality and appreciation of beauty. Strengths that allow individuals to navigate the world and protect them in the face of setbacks, such as optimism and loving relationships, should also loom large in the well-being of young adults. 2. Middle age is a time when individuals focus on career and family (Lachman, 2001), so strengths relevant to those domains, including capacity for loving relationships and self-control, should be most centrally related to well-being during that life stage. It may be that the time and energy demands of focusing on these areas makes it harder for middle-aged individuals to use AGING AND HUMAN STRENGTHS / 185 and benefit from the use of less-applied strengths, such as appreciation of beauty. Furthermore, they may score lower on levels of strengths across the board due to the many constraints on their time. 3. In old age, individuals appear to focus on close social partners and familiar emotional relationships (Carstensen, Isaacowitz, & Charles, 1999). Therefore, we expected older individuals to have the highest levels of strengths that reflect and relate to socioemotional functioning, such as capacity for love and kindness. Additionally, older individuals appear better able to regulate their emotions than younger individuals (Gross et al., 1997), suggesting they may be higher on strengths of temperance and control. In terms of the relationship of strengths to well-being, we hypothesized that high levels of socioemotional strengths would particularly relate to well-being in older adults. Furthermore, older adults are largely free from the constraints of work and raising children, and may have more opportunity to use strengths (or not) than other age groups. Therefore, we expected to see a particularly strong relationship between report of strengths and wellbeing in this age group. METHOD Participants The first group of participants in the current study were those men of the Grant Study of Harvard graduates still living and in contact with the study during 1999 (for more information on the sample, see Vaillant, 1977; Vaillant & Mukamal, 2001, Vaillant & Vaillant, 1990). Eighty-nine participants returned the questionnaire, though there was quite a bit of missing data on the returned forms. These Caucasian men were, on average, 78 years of age (plus/minus two years) at the time they received the questionnaire. One hundred fifty-eight Grant men were alive at that time; the rate of questionnaire response in the sample was therefore a modest 56%. We recruited participants for the second, community sample from three adult age groups: young adults (age 18-25), middle-aged adults (age 36-59) and older adults (age 60 and above). Older adults could only participate if they were community-dwelling, did not utilize any regular skilled nursing care in their housing, and did not show any clear evidence of cognitive impairment. Young adults were recruited primarily through written and electronic advertisements in the Philadelphia, Pennsylvania area, and many were students in local colleges and universities. We recruited the majority of the middle-aged participants from church and synagogue groups in the Philadelphia area, and the older adults primarily from senior centers in the region. One hundred young adults, 86 middle-aged adults, and 94 older adults comprised the community sample. The young adults (mean age = 20.56, SD = 1.75) 186 / ISAACOWITZ, VAILLANT AND SELIGMAN consisted of 27 male and 73 female participants; of these, 67 identified themselves as Caucasian, eight as African-American, 19 as Asian or Asian-American, and six as members of other ethnic groups. The middle-aged group (mean age = 48.74, SD = 4.90) had 30 men and 56 women, with 74 Caucasians, eight African-Americans, and three Asian-Americans. One middle-aged adult did not report ethnicity. Finally, the older adult group (mean age = 76.71, SD = 8.70) consisted of 31 men and 63 women. This group included 83 Caucasians and 10 African-Americans. One older adult participant did not report ethnicity. Chi-square analyses revealed that the ethnic distribution of the three age groups was significantly different, P2 (6, N = 278) = 37.62, p < .001. Not surprisingly, group differences also emerged on educational attainment and self-reported health at baseline: education: F(2, 265) = 81.38, p < .0001; health: F(2, 275) = 34.48, p < .0001. The middle-aged group had the most total years of education completed (M = 17.67, SD = 3.32), followed by the young adults (M = 14.77, SD = 1.63) and the older adults (M = 12.34, SD = 3.07). For self-reported health, young adults reported the highest levels (M = 3.45, SD = .89), followed by middle-aged (M = 3.01, SD = .76) and older adults (M = 2.29, SD = .94) on a self-report scale going from 0 (poor) to 4 (excellent). All pairwise comparisons between the age groups revealed significant group differences using Tukey contrasts. Procedure Participants from the Grant Study were sent the self-report measure of strengths, which contained a measure of life satisfaction embedded within it. They were provided with instructions concerning how to complete the measure, and were asked to mail it back. Participants from the community study completed the measure as part of a larger packet of questionnaires, either one-on-one or in a small group with an interviewer to answer questions and transcribe responses in case of any vision or reading problems. Measures 16-Strength Questionnaire: A Measure of Human Strengths The original 100-item Wellsprings Questionnaire was developed by the Gallup Organization in 1999 as a face-valid self-report measure of human strengths, based on the classification of strengths developed earlier that year by a small group of social scientists convened in the Cayman Islands for such purpose. This group proposed the following 16 “characteristics” associated with a good life: capacity for love and intimacy, satisfying work, helping others/altruism, being a good citizen, spirituality, leadership, aesthetic appreciation/pleasures of the mind, knowledge and understanding of areas of life larger than one’s self (depth and AGING AND HUMAN STRENGTHS / 187 breadth), integrity (principles, ethics), creativity/originality, play, courage, purposive future-mindedness, individuality, self-regulation, and wisdom. The classification system has changed several times since then (for the most recent version, see Peterson & Seligman, in press). In the fall of 2000, one of us mapped the most up-to-date classification on to the existing items of the Wellsprings questionnaire. Some new strengths were well-represented (love, citizenship, spirituality, wisdom), while others had too few items to warrant further analysis (such as personal intelligence and love of life). If a particular strength had at least four associated items it was retained for further analysis, thereby leaving originality, appreciation of beauty, hope, courage, humor, capacity for loving relationships, kindness, citizenship, industry, humane leadership, self-control, wisdom, and spirituality. A second level of exclusion for further analysis was that each strength scale with enough items had a coefficient alpha of at least .6 in both the Grant study and adult development samples. This left, with coefficient alpha from the community adult sample in parentheses: originality (.64), hope (.80), citizenship (.75), capacity to love and be loved (.61), wisdom (.75), spirituality (.62), appreciation of beauty (.71), kindness (.64), and selfcontrol/self-regulation (.67). Appendix B lists items from the questionnaire representing each strength. Satisfaction with Life Scale The Satisfaction with Life Scale (SWLS: Pavot, Diener, Colvin, & Sandvik, 1991) is a five-item self-report measure assessing an individual’s cognitive assessment of their life. Higher scores indicate higher levels of satisfaction with current life. Previous research has found either no age differences on life satisfaction, or slightly positive age trajectories (Diener, Suh, Lucas, & Smith, 1999). Life satisfaction is considered to be a central component of subjective well-being (Diener et al., 1999). RESULTS While age differences emerged on mean levels of several strengths, the primary result was the differential unique predictors of life satisfaction in the different age groups and samples. Only hope predicted satisfaction in young adults, and only capacity for loving predicted satisfaction in middle-aged adults. Hope, citizenship and capacity for loving relationships all emerged as unique predictors among the community sample of older adults. In contrast, only capacity for loving relationships and appreciation of beauty predicted life satisfaction among the men of the Grant sample. Below, we first present descriptive statistics on mean levels of each strength and life satisfaction within the Grant Sample men and within the three adult age groups. While we present means and standard deviations for all four groups, we present group comparisons among the three groups from the adult sample, as 188 / ISAACOWITZ, VAILLANT AND SELIGMAN well as between the older adult group from the community and the similar-aged men of the Grant study. We then describe the pattern of correlations between each strength and life satisfaction in each of the samples, and finally we report analyses aimed at determining unique strength-based predictors of life satisfaction within each of the groups. Age Differences in Strengths As shown in Table 1, age differences among the three age groups from the adult development study emerged on all the strengths except for hope and wisdom. For each strength that showed an omnibus group difference, Tukey post-hoc pairwise comparisons were performed to test for differences between pairs of age groups. Older adults scored higher on self-control than did young or middle-aged individuals. Large group differences also emerged on spirituality: the older, the more spiritual within the community sample, but the men of the Grant study scored much lower than their community agemates. On appreciation of beauty, young adults scored higher than older adults, whereas for kindness, middle-aged individuals scored higher than did younger individuals, and Grant study men scored lower than their community older agemates on this strength. For Table 1. Mean Levels of Strengths in Three Adult Age Groups, As Well As in Members of the Grant Study Sample Strength: Mean (SD) Originality Hope Citizenship Loving relationships Wisdom Spirituality Beauty Kindness Self-control Young Middle Older adults aged adults 18.93a (2.97) 26.97 (4.41) 26.77a (4.02) 15.14 (2.78) 15.64 (2.60) 23.64a (4.44) 24.47a (3.50) 23.36a (3.39) 21.29a (3.74) 18.59a (4.24) 27.33 (3.47) 29.29b (4.96) 15.69 (2.55) 15.71 (2.20) 25.46b (4.17) 23.78 (3.48) 25.20b (6.10) 21.23a (3.32) 16.91b (3.49) 26.42 (3.95) 30.33b (4.68) 16.03 (2.69) 15.19 (2.85) 27.1c (3.26) 23.17b (3.91) 24.02 (4.13) 22.82b (3.96) Group difference F(2, 276) = 8.68** n.s. F(2, 276) =15.68*** F(2, 277) = 2.73# n.s. F(2, 273) =17.74*** F(2, 275) = 3.04* F(2, 277) = 3.74* F(2, 276) = 5.49** Grant sample (t vs. older) 15.62 (3.44) t(170) = 2.46* 25.37 (3.04) t(166) = 1.96# 27.46 (6.08) t(145) = 3.42*** 15.53 (2.56) t(179) = 1.28 14.59 (2.13) t(168) = 1.53 20.50 (5.01) t(169) = 11.56*** 22.38 (3.48) t(175) = 1.42 22.86 (2.86) t(170) = 2.10* 22.47 (2.90) t(179) = .67 Note: Variables with different superscripts differed significantly from each other on post-hoc Tukey comparisons. # p < .10; *p < .05; **p < .01; ***p < .001. AGING AND HUMAN STRENGTHS / 189 originality, older adults scored lower than younger adults, but Grant study men scored even lower than young adults. Middle-aged and older adults scored higher on citizenship than did young adults, but Grant sample men scored lower than their community agemates. We also used age as a continuous measure in the adult community sample, even though the sample lacks individuals in the 26-36 age range. Higher age correlated with higher levels of citizenship (r = .29, p < .0001), more self-control (r = .15, p < .05), and more spirituality (r = .32, p < .0001), but with lower levels of originality (r = –.23, p < .001) and lower appreciation of beauty (r = –.18, p < .01). Gender and Education Analyses Within the Adult Community Sample Because there were differences in education levels among the three age groups, we re-ran the age analyses in the adult community sample controlling for years of education. Age-group differences favoring middle-aged over younger adults were eliminated for kindness. Significant age-group differences in which older individuals scored higher than younger adults on self-control but lower on appreciation of beauty became only marginally significant after controlling for education. Significant age-group differences remained for originality, citizenship, and spirituality. We also tested for gender differences within the community sample, as well for Age group × Sex interaction differences in the strengths. Mean differences are shown in Table 2. Women scored higher than men on citizenship, loving relationships, and appreciation of beauty. Age group × Sex interactions were significant for two strengths. In general, men appeared to show larger positive age differences in citizenship and self-control. Table 2. Gender Differences in Mean Levels of Each Strength in the Adult Community Sample Strength Men (n = 88) Women (n = 192) Originality Hope Citizenshipa Loving relationships Wisdom Spirituality Beauty Kindness Self-controla 18.31 (3.34) 26.92 (3.75) 27.78 (4.70) 14.91 (2.91) 15.21 (2.69) 24.61 (4.75) 23.08 (4.01) 24.05 (6.36) 21.89 (3.69) 18.08 (3.82) 26.89 (4.10) 29.17 (4.77) 15.93 (2.53) 15.59 (2.59) 25.67 (3.95) 24.17 (3.44) 24.19 (3.63) 21.73 (3.79) # Gender difference t(277) = t(276) = t(277) = t(278) = t(277) = t(143) = t(276) = t(114) = t(277) = .47 .06 –2.26* –2.97** –1.13 –1.82#b –2.31* –.18b .33 p < .10; *p < .05; **p < .01; ***p < .001. Indicates variable with a significant sex * age group interaction. b Indicates variable in which group had unequal variance. In these cases, Satterthwaite method used for t-test. a 190 / ISAACOWITZ, VAILLANT AND SELIGMAN For self-control, women’s scores varied little over adulthood (means for young, middle-aged, and older: 21.67, 21.16, 22.28, respectively), whereas older men scored much higher on self-control than younger men (means 20.24, 21.35, and 24, respectively). On citizenship, middle-aged and older women scored higher than younger women (27.21, 30.38, 30.33, respectively), whereas each age group of older men scored higher than the younger group (25.55, 27.35, 30.30, respectively). These interactions are shown in Figures 1 and 2. Life Satisfaction There appeared to be group differences in life satisfaction, with young and middle-aged adults from the community sample scoring lowest (young: M = 16.84, SD = 3.39; middle aged: M = 16.85, SD = 4.08), followed by the older Figure 1. Age × sex interaction for mean levels of self-control. AGING AND HUMAN STRENGTHS / 191 adults from the community sample (M = 17.81, SD = 4.38), with the men of the Grant sample scoring highest on life satisfaction (M = 18.75, SD = 3.9). However, neither the omnibus ANOVA testing for group differences within the three community adult samples [F(2, 274) = 1.98, n.s.] nor the independent samples t-test comparing the older adults from the community with the men of the Grant sample were significant [t(181) = .94, n.s.]. Men and women in the community sample did not differ in their mean level of life satisfaction. The lack of significant group differences made life satisfaction especially desirable as an outcome measure, as it was especially sensitive to different human strength correlates and unique predictors. In this section, we first report on correlations between strengths and life satisfaction with each age group. We then report on unique prediction of life satisfaction, testing each strength within each group while holding all other strengths constant. Figure 2. Age × sex interaction for mean levels of citizenship. 192 / ISAACOWITZ, VAILLANT AND SELIGMAN Table 3. Correlations between Strengths and Life Satisfaction (As Measured by the Satisfaction with Life Scale) in the Three Adult Sample Age Groups and the Grant Sample Strength (r with SWLS) Young adults Middle aged Older adults Grant sample Originality Hope Citizenship Loving relationships Wisdom Spirituality Beauty Kindness Self-control .26** .46*** .24* .17# .33*** .14 .11 .30** .41*** –.13 .35*** .20# .46*** .04 .17 –.05 .07 .24* .17# .47*** .52*** .51*** .43*** .30** .26* .40*** .41*** .18# .41*** .26* .48*** .23* .10 .38*** .39*** .22* # p < .10; *p < .05; **p < .01; ***p < .001 Correlations of Strengths with Life Satisfaction Simple correlations between level of each strength and life satisfaction within each of the four groups are provided in Table 3. Not surprisingly, higher levels of most strengths correlated strongly and positively with higher levels of self-reported life satisfaction across the four groups. Notably, however, the pattern of significant correlations was much weaker in the middle-aged adult sample than in any of the other three samples. For example, higher levels of wisdom and kindness correlated with greater life satisfaction in young adults, older adults, and among the men of the Grant study, but these strengths did not correlate with life satisfaction in the middle-aged sample. Interestingly, higher levels of spirituality correlated with greater life satisfaction only within the community older sample, but not within any of the other samples. Controlling for education produced several interesting differences. It did not change the relationships among the young adults at all, but among middle-aged individuals kindness became significantly related to life satisfaction once education was controlled (pr = .22, p < .05). Controlling for education produced the most dramatic changes in the older sample. Originality and appreciation of beauty no longer correlated with life satisfaction, and the magnitude of the relationships between life satisfaction and kindness, citizenship, love, and wisdom were attenuated but remained significant. Unique Strength Predictors of Life Satisfaction Table 4 shows more conservative analyses linking the strengths with life satisfaction within each of the four groups, evaluating the contribution of each variable to a model predicting life satisfaction after all other strengths have been AGING AND HUMAN STRENGTHS / 193 Table 4. Unique Predictors of Life Satisfaction in Each Sample Young adult Strength b t Middle aged b t Older adult b t Grant sample b t –.29 –.03 –1.17 .17 –.18 .03 –.32 Originality –.05 1.56 .23 1.27 2.09* .24 .23 2.54* Hope .43 .65 .07 1.20 .52 .09 .05 2.79** Citizenship .40 Loving 4.22*** .44 2.43* .15 .47 .02 2.04* relationships .41 –1.82# –.39 –.83 .12 –.23 .02 .02 Wisdom .01 .05 –1.48 –1.24 –.13 –.10 .32 Spirituality .05 .38 –.23 2.36* –.46 .33 –.05 –.62 Beauty –.09 –1.68# .01 .85 1.49 .16 .19 Kindness –.27 –1.39 .15 .05 .94 1.73 .13 .19 –.89 Self-control –.16 .38 F for model F(9,90) = 4.05*** F(9,72) = 4.52*** F(9,76) = 4.86*** F(9,70) = 4.25*** # p < .10; *p < .05; **p < .01; ***p < .001 Each b represents the unique effect of each predictor while holding all others constant. entered into the regression model. Within the young adult sample, only hope significantly predicted life satisfaction. Only capacity for loving relationships significantly predicted life satisfaction among middle-aged adults. Both of these strengths positively predicted life satisfaction; higher strength levels predicted more life satisfaction. Interestingly, the two other strengths that reached the level of nonsignificant trends in the prediction of life satisfaction in the middle-aged sample were negative predictors; higher levels of wisdom and appreciation of beauty related to lower levels of life satisfaction in the middle-aged sample. Within the community older sample, hope, citizenship and loving all emerged as unique positive predictors of life satisfaction. Higher levels of loving also predicted more life satisfaction in the Grant sample; however, hope, and citizenship did not. Instead, higher levels of appreciation of beauty predicted higher life satisfaction in the sample of Harvard graduates. DISCUSSION We compared both the levels of different human strengths as well as the relationship between the strengths and life satisfaction across four adult samples. The four samples included three community-based samples of adults ranging in age from 18 to 93, as well as the surviving members of the Grant study of Harvard graduates, who were mostly about 78 years of age at the time they completed the measures for this study. The most interesting findings emerged when we tested for unique predictors between each strength and life satisfaction, above and beyond the effects of all other strengths. A sole strength emerged as a significant unique predictor in the young and middle-aged samples; for the young adults it was hope, and for the 194 / ISAACOWITZ, VAILLANT AND SELIGMAN middle-aged adults it was loving relationships. Hope, citizenship and loving relationships all emerged as unique predictors of life satisfaction in the community older adult sample, whereas among the older men of the Grant sample, only loving relationships and appreciation of beauty significantly predicted life satisfaction. All the significant predictive relationships were positive; higher levels of strengths uniquely predicted more life satisfaction. Interestingly, two trends emerged in the middle-aged sample in the opposite direction where there were trends for higher levels of wisdom and appreciation of beauty to predict lower levels of life satisfaction among these midlife adults. Age-group differences were also found for mean levels of all strengths with the exceptions of wisdom and hope (though the comparison of the two late-life samples was marginally significant for hope, with the Grant study men scoring lower than the community older adults). We hypothesized that younger adults focus more on exploring the world (e.g., originality), whereas older individuals possess more self-regulatory strengths; our findings generally supported these hypotheses. Controlling for education attenuated some of these age-group differences; additionally, modest Age × Gender interactions emerged for self-control and citizenship, such that older men reported particularly high levels of both strengths compared to younger men. Generally, higher levels of strengths correlated with more life satisfaction across the four groups. In the older community sample, all strengths correlated positively with life satisfaction (except for originality, which was only marginally significant). Interestingly, spirituality correlated with life satisfaction only among the community-dwelling older adults, but not in any of the other groups. While only spirituality and appreciation of beauty failed to correlate with life satisfaction in young adults, only self-control, capacity for loving relationships, and hope significantly correlated with life satisfaction in middle-aged adults. This supported our hypothesis that middle age is a time when adults are too busy with work and family demands to truly benefit from their strengths; their self-reports of strengths suggest that they simply do not have the opportunity to exercise, and benefit from, the use of their strengths due to constraints on their time. Nonetheless, these differences between age groups may reflect cohort effects rather than differences between age groups in life context; this cross-sectional data can not distinguish between these two plausible explanations for the findings. Age Differences Age differences in the levels and relationship to satisfaction of the various strengths to the developmental tasks of each time in the adult lifespan make theoretical sense (see Erikson, 1950; Heckhausen, 1999). Young adults reported the highest levels of strengths associated with exploring the world creatively (such as originality and appreciation of beauty). Middle-aged adults rated high on AGING AND HUMAN STRENGTHS / 195 generativity-relevant strengths such as citizenship and kindness, just as Erikson predicts. And finally, older adults rated high on interpersonal and self-regulatory strengths, consistent with socioemotional selectivity theory’s claim that older adults proactively regulate their emotions by being choosy about their social interactions (Carstensen et al., 1999). Unique predictors based on hierarchical regression did distinguish the three community age groups, and matched the developmental tasks for the young and middle-aged groups. Hope, for young adults, and loving, for middleaged individuals emerged as the sole unique predictors of life satisfaction. Younger adults need to make choices that will bank resources for the future (Carstensen et al., 1999), and they benefit from optimistic perspectives that make for continued perseverance even in the face of temporary setbacks (Seligman, 1990). Middle-aged individuals are occupied with family: raising children, keeping a marriage going—making their capacity to nurture close relationships especially critical. Older individuals have fewer constraints forcing them to channel their energies in any particular direction. They are thereby able to benefit from a wider range of strengths. For them, both the strengths important to the younger age groups, as well as citizenship, emerged as unique predictors of life satisfaction. Thus, older adults can exhibit strengths on intrapsychic, interpersonal, and social-cultural levels. Freed from the need to bank resources for the future (Carstensen et al., 1999), older individuals may select and utilize whatever strengths give them the most effective reward in the present. Community vs. Grant Sample The men of the Grant study differed from age-matched adults from the community sample in intriguing ways. The Grant study men reported lower levels of most strengths than their older community agemates, and their unique predictors of life satisfaction differed. While capacity for loving relationships was similar across community middle-aged, community older, and Grant samples, appreciation of beauty uniquely predicted life satisfaction only among the Grant men. This difference suggests one interpretation of the Grant study findings: that men put more value on the intellectual and aesthetic aspects of life than the prosocial, interpersonal, or self-regulatory aspects. Combining the finding of lower levels of spirituality among the Grant men than their community peers with the unique relationship between appreciation of beauty and life satisfaction only found among the Grant men suggests that this group of men may be using aesthetic appreciation as a form of spirituality. However, this interpretation is purely speculative and ignores the obvious demographic differences between the Grant sample and the community adult samples. The majority of community participants were female, while all of the Grant participants were male. Unfortunately, there are too few men in the community sample to determine whether sample 196 / ISAACOWITZ, VAILLANT AND SELIGMAN differences merely reflect gender differences. However, it seems unlikely that all older men would have such a strong link between appreciation of beauty and life satisfaction, suggesting this difference is due to something other than gender. As a preliminary attempt to evaluate whether sample differences arose simply from the major gender differences between the sample participants, we conducted post-hoc analyses comparing the men of the community sample (across age groups) with the Grant men and found that men in the community sample were, in general, more similar to the community women than to the Grant men. It is important to note, however, that the Grant men were also slightly older than the community olderadult sample, and they were also better-educated and of a higher socioeconomic status. Additionally, some of the community participants (particularly from the middle-aged sample) were recruited from religious institutions, thus confounding the Grant vs. community comparisons in an additional way. A plausible explanation is that these differences in socioeconomic status and education led to life trajectories in which aesthetics were valued more, and spirituality was valued less for these Harvard men than they were in the adult lives of the majority of individuals in their cohort. In his work with the Grant study men at age 47, Vaillant (1977) noted that spirituality did not appear to play a central role in their lives. This still held true at age 75 (Vaillant, 2002). These findings provide some multi-method support for our results. Limitations and Future Directions While it has been convenient to develop a self-report measure of various strengths, there are obvious drawbacks to such a measure. Individuals may not be accurate at reporting on their own strengths. Recent work has sought to validate the self-report measures using behavioral indices of the various strengths (see Peterson & Seligman, in press). For the purposes of the present study, this is a marked limitation because we are using one type of self-report measure (strengths) to predict simultaneous scores on a separate self-report measure (life satisfaction). Longitudinal analyses, using strengths as an independent variable and change over time in life satisfaction (as well as other subjective well-being measures) as the dependent measure, will illuminate whether strengths may serve a protective or buffering function in the face of negative life events. Of course, a brief longitudinal component will not change the inherently cross-sectional nature of the age comparisons used in the study. Differences between the age groups may reflect either developmental trajectories or cohort effects, or a combination of both. The Grant sample findings suggest that life context factors can play a role in the relationship of strengths to well-being, supporting the possible role of cohort factors. However, the close match of levels and relation to well-being of the various strengths to hypothesized developmental tasks of the adult lifespan support a more developmental interpretation. AGING AND HUMAN STRENGTHS / 197 CONCLUSIONS While lifespan developmental psychologists have consistently been concerned with “the good life” in old age, insofar as it could be measured by life satisfaction or by zest, positive psychology’s recent attempts to measure various human strengths and virtues as indicators of a good life have ignored the implications of aging for these constructs. This study explored how human strengths might unfold in adulthood and old age, in both community-dwelling adults of various ages as well as in a highly selected group of older men. Our primary finding is that age differences match up nicely with the developmental tasks of the various age groups. Younger adults explore the world and benefit most from hope of a good future, whereas middle-aged individuals are most focused on their relationships. Older adults possess more self-control, but also more opportunity to benefit from the use of a variety of strengths. The deployment of strengths therefore appears fundamentally related to development across the lifespan. Thus, developmental tasks, and the context of these tasks across the lifespan, must be central to any attempt to understand human strengths. The best qualities in individuals may depend not just on their inner beings, but also on their time in life and their place on earth as well. APPENDIX A Comparison of Lists of 16-Strengths and 24-Strengths 16-Strength List Creativity/originality Wisdom Integrity Capacity for love and intimacy Helping others/altruism Being a good citizen Leadership 24-Strength List Curiosity/interest Love of learning Active open-mindedness Creativity/ingenuity Perspective/wisdom Valor/courage Industry/perseverance Integrity/honesty/authenticity Vitality/zest/enthusiasm Intimacy/attachment Kindness/generosity Social intelligence Citizenship/teamwork Equity/fairness Leadership Forgiveness/mercy 198 / ISAACOWITZ, VAILLANT AND SELIGMAN APPENDIX A (Cont'd.) 16-Strength List Self-control/self regulation Aesthetic appreciation Purposive future-mindedness Play Spirituality Satisfying work Knowledge and understanding of areas larger than one's self Individuality 24-Strength List Modesty/humility Prudence/caution Self-control/self-regulation Awe/appreciation of beauty Gratitude Hope/optimism Playfulness Spirituality APPENDIX B Items Used to Measure Strengths Originality My imagination stretches far beyond that of my associates. In the last month I have found an original solution to a problem in my life. In the last week I have had the opportunity to use my imagination to make something better. I have a powerful urge to do something original during this next year. When someone tells me how to do something, I spontaneously think of alternative ways to get the same thing done. Hope I have clear pictures in my mind about what i want to happen in the future. I have what it takes to succeed. I have confidence in the choices I make. I have a plan for what I want to be doing five years from now. I will succeed with the goals I set for myself. I have confidence in my intuitive ways of doing things. Citizenship I volunteer five or more hours of community or professional service each week. Volunteering to help others makes me feel good. I generously contribute money to worthwhile causes. AGING AND HUMAN STRENGTHS / 199 I currently know over 100 persons whom I could rally to achieve a goal I believed in. I feel a responsibility to use my time and energy to make the world a better place to live. I frequently go out of my way to help people in need. I do many helpful things for people that go unrecognized. Others trust me to keep their secrets. Capacity for loving relationships There are people in my life who care as much about my feelings and well-being as they do about their own. I believe I am the most important person in someone else's life. My family and close friends can not do anything that would make me not love them. I always feel the presence of love in my life. Wisdom Others come to me for advice. I intentionally seek conversations with people whom I believe to be wise. I consider myself to be a wise person. Overall, others consider me to be a wise person. Spirituality In the last 24 hours, I have personally spent 30 minutes in prayer, meditation or contemplation. I have a set of principles that govern my life. I believe that each person has a soul. I believe there is a spirit that survives after the death of the body that can communicate with persons still living. I believe in a universal power, a god. My beliefs make me feel my life is important. I have had dreams that foretold what was going to happen. Appreciation of beauty I sometimes have a craving for experiencing great art, such as music, drama, or paintings. I have created something of beauty in the last year. I make it a point to tell people about what they do well. In the last month I have enjoyed classical music, art, drama, science or mathematics. I often experience emotions evoked by beauty. I often see beauty that other people pass by without noticing. Kindness I have a neighbor or someone at work who cares about me as a person. In the last two days, I have performed acts that were helpful to another person, but had no direct personal benefit for me. 200 / ISAACOWITZ, VAILLANT AND SELIGMAN APPENDIX B (Cont'd.) I have cared deeply about someone for ten or more years. I make other people happy. I have voluntarily helped a neighbor in the last month. I seem to have an uncanny sense for making others feel good. Self-control/self-regulation I am a goal-oriented person. I am a highly disciplined person. I always pay my bills on time. I can keep cool while others are freaking out. I have the capacity for concentrating on the goals i set for myself. "A place for everything and everything in its place" describes me perfectly. REFERENCES Abramson, L. Y., Seligman, M. E. P., & Teasdale, J. D. (1978). Learned helplessness: Critique and reformulation. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 87, 49–74. Bromberger, J. T., & Matthews, K. A. (1996). A longitudinal study of the effects of pessimism, trait anxiety, and life stress on depressive symptoms in middle-aged women. Psychology and Aging, 11, 207–213. Carstensen, L. L., Isaacowitz, D. M., & Charles, S. T. (1999). Taking time seriously: A theory of socioemotional selectivity. American Psychologist, 54, 165–181. Diener, E., Suh, E. M., Lucas, R. E., & Smith, H. L. (1999). Subjective well-being: Three decades of progress. Psychologist Bulletin, 125, 276-302. Erber, J. T., Szuchman, L. T., & Prager, I. G. (2001). Ain’t misbehavin’: The effects of age and intentionality on judgments about misconduct. Psychology and Aging, 16, 85-95. Erikson, E. H. (1950). Childhood and society. New York: W. W. Norton & Company. Gross, J. J., Carstensen, L. L., Pasupathi, M., Tsai, J., Goetestam Skorpen, C., & Hsu, A. Y. C. (1997). Emotion and aging: Experience, expression, and control. Psychology and Aging, 12, 590-599. Heckhausen, J. (1999). Developmental regulation in adulthood: Age-normative and sociocultural constraints as adaptive challenges. New York: Cambridge University Press. Isaacowitz, D. M. (2001). Optimism and subjective well-being in adulthood and old age. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, University of Pennsylvania, Philadelphia, PA. Isaacowitz, D. M., & Seligman, M. E. P. (2001). Is pessimistic explanatory style a risk factor for depressive mood among community-dwelling older adults? Behaviour Research and Therapy, 39, 255–273. Isaacowitz, D. M., & Seligman, M. E. P. (2002). Cognitive style predictors of affect change in older adults. International Journal of Aging and Human Development, 54, 233–253. Isaacowitz, D. M., & Seligman, M. E. P. (2003). Cognitive styles and psychological well-being in adulthood and old age. In M. Bornstein, L. Davidson, C. L. M. Keyes, & K. Moore (Eds.), Well-Being: Positive development across the lifespan (pp. 449-475). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. AGING AND HUMAN STRENGTHS / 201 Kamen-Siegel, L., Rodin, J., Seligman, M. E. P., & Dwyer, J. (1991). Explanatory style and cell-mediated immunity. Health Psychology, 10, 229–235. Lachman, M. E. (2001). Handbook of midlife development. New York: John Wiley & Sons, Inc. Metalsky, G. I., Abramson, L. Y., Seligman, M. E. P., Semmel, A., & Peterson, C. R. (1982). Attributional styles and life events in the classroom: Vulnerability and invulnerability to depressive mood reactions. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 43, 612–617. Mroczek, D. K., Spiro, A. III, Aldwin, C. M., Ozer, D. J., & Bossé, R. (1993). Construct validation of optimism and pessimism in older men: Findings from the Normative Aging Study. Health Psychology, 12, 406–409. Pavot, W. G., Diener, E., Colvin, C. R., & Sandvik, E. (1991). Further validation of the satisfaction with life scale: Evidence for the cross-method convergence of well-being measures. Journal of Personality Assessment, 57, 149–161. Peterson, C. P., & Seligman, M. E. P. (1984). Causal explanations as a risk factor of depression. Psychological Review, 91, 347–374. Peterson, C., & Seligman, M. E. P. (in press). The VIA classification of strengths. Washington, D.C.: American Psychological Association. http://psych.upenn.edu/Seligman/viastrengthsinventory.htm. Robinson-Whelen, S., Kim, C., MacCallum, R. C., & Kiecolt-Glaser, J. C. (1997). Distinguishing optimism from pessimism in older adults: Is it more important to be optimistic or not to be pessimistic? Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 73, 1345–1353. Scheier, M. R., & Carver, C. S. (1985). Optimism, coping, and health: Assessment and implications of generalized outcome expectancies. Health Psychology, 4, 219–247. Seligman, M. E. P. (1990). Learned optimism. New York: Basic Books. Vaillant, G. E. (1977). Adaptation to life. New York: Basic Books. Vaillant, G. E. (2002). Aging well. New York: Little-Brown. Vaillant, G. E., & Mukamal, K. (2001). Successful aging. American Journal of Psychiatry, 158, 839–847. Vaillant, G. E., & Vaillant, C. O. (1990). Natural history of male psychological health, XII: A 45-year study of predictors of successful aging at age 65. American Journal of Psychiatry, 147, 31–37. Vickers, K. S., & Vogeltanz, N. D. (2000). Dispositional optimism as a predictor of depressive symptoms over time. Personality and Individual Differences, 28, 259–272. Direct reprint requests to: Derek M. Isaacowitz Department of Psychology Brandeis University MS 062 Waltham, MA 02454-9110 e-mail: dmi@brandeis.edu
© Copyright 2024