Mindsets and Motivation as the Keys to Academic Success Center for Children & Families Spring Lecture Series March 27, 2015 How do we ensure children’s success in school? Motivation • Most of us tend to think of motivation in terms of encouragement. Intentions don’t always work out: Intentions don’t always work out: • Responding to successes with praise, or to failures with comfort or offers of help can be harmful. Intentions don’t always work out: • Responding to successes with praise, or to failures with comfort or offers of help can be harmful. • Responding to failures with anger, blame, or by withholding help can encourage students to try harder. Intentions don’t always work out: • Building confidence doesn’t help. • Giving students opportunities success doesn’t help. for Intentions don’t always work out: • Telling students to try harder is only helpful if they didn’t try. Intentions don’t always work out: • Telling students to try harder is only helpful if they didn’t try. • Understanding how students interpret our messages is the key. Teacher behaviors give students indirect cues about cause Teacher’s attribution for student’s outcome Student infers attribution of teacher Pity vs. Anger Praise vs. Blame Help vs. Neglect Self-ascription to ability or effort Expectancy, Esteem-related affect, Performance, etc. Children’s understanding of ability and effort changes with development • Children understand the concept of effort as controllable and unstable before they understand the concept of ability as uncontrollable and stable. Children’s understanding of ability and effort changes with development • Children understand the concept of effort as controllable and unstable before they understand the concept of ability as uncontrollable and stable. • Before middle childhood, children tend to view ability as fluctuating and modifiable through practice and effort (i.e., controllable and unstable). Pity vs. Anger • Sixth-graders were given impossible puzzles to solve, and the “teacher” responded with either pity, anger, or no emotional reaction. Pity vs. Anger • Sixth-graders were given impossible puzzles to solve, and the “teacher” responded with either pity, anger, or no emotional reaction. – Children whose teacher showed pity said they did poorly because they’re just not good at puzzles, and tended to think they would not do well in the future. Pity vs. Anger • Sixth-graders were given impossible puzzles to solve, and the “teacher” responded with either pity, anger, or no emotional reaction. – Children whose teacher showed pity said they did poorly because they’re just not good at puzzles, and tended to think they would not do well in the future. – Children whose teacher showed anger said they did poorly because they didn’t try hard enough. Praise vs. Blame Praise vs. Blame • 4-5yr olds and 11-12yr olds viewed a tape of two boys completing easy tasks (solving math problems or playing basketball) • Success: • Failure: Praise vs. Blame • 4-5yr olds and 11-12yr olds viewed a tape of two boys completing easy tasks (solving math problems or playing basketball) • Success: One boy was praised (“Good job”) and one received neutral feedback (“Correct”) • Failure: Praise vs. Blame • 4-5yr olds and 11-12yr olds viewed a tape of two boys completing easy tasks (solving math problems or playing basketball) • Success: One boy was praised (“Good job”) and one received neutral feedback (“Correct”) • Failure: One boy was criticized (“What’s the matter with you? That’s not right”) and one received neutral feedback (“No, not quite”) Praise vs. Blame • Children rated the boys’ math ability or throwing ability on a 5-point scale Praise vs. Blame Saw boy Saw boy praised for criticized success for failure 4- to 5-yearHigher Lower olds ability ability 11- to 12Lower Higher year-olds ability ability What does this tell us? • As children get older, they become more likely to interpret praise for easy success as an indication of low ability and blame for failure on an easy task as an indication of high ability. What does this tell us? • As children get older, they become more likely to interpret praise for easy success as an indication of low ability and blame for failure on an easy task as an indication of high ability. Younger children do not yet see high effort as an indicator of low ability. How does feedback like this affect children’s motivation and sense of self? Teacher’s attribution for student’s outcome Student infers attribution of teacher Pity vs. Anger Praise vs. Blame Help vs. Neglect Self-ascription to ability or effort Expectancy, Esteem-related affect, Performance, etc. • 3-week unit on cursive writing in a 2nd-grade classroom, with a new letter introduced each day with time for students to practice writing letters • Two types of feedback for success: – Praise (control) group: teacher gave a happy face token and praise – Attribution group: teacher gave a happy face token and an explanation for their success – Corrective feedback was given for unsuccessful attempts Outcome? • Children in the “praise” only group were disappointed and anxious • Children in the “attribution” group enjoyed cursive writing more, thought it was easier, and used it more • Students are eager for information that tells them they are doing well AND for information that affirms their sense of self-worth more generally What kind of feedback do they need? • To answer this question, we need to consider Carol Dweck’s ideas about mindset. – Growth mindset: incremental theory of intelligence – Fixed mindset: entity theory of intelligence Do you mostly agree or mostly disagree with each of these questions? 1. Your intelligence is something very basic about you that you can’t change very much. 2. You can learn new things, but you can’t really change how intelligent you are. 3. No matter how much intelligence you have, you can always change it quite a bit. Does understanding mindsets hold the key? • A student’s mindset is what we are changing in the ways we respond to success and failures. – Person praise promotes a fixed mindset – Process praise promotes a growth mindset What about confidence? • In one of Carol Dweck’s early studies, she gave grade-school students deliberately confusing material at the start of a new psychology unit. What about confidence? • In one of Carol Dweck’s early studies, she gave grade-school students deliberately confusing material at the start of a new psychology unit. – For boys, the higher their IQ, the more likely they would successfully learn the difficult material. – For girls, the opposite was true. Girls with higher IQs did even more poorly than girls with lower IQs. What about confidence? • In one of Carol Dweck’s early studies, she gave grade-school students deliberately confusing material at the start of a new psychology unit. – For boys, the higher their IQ, the more likely they would successfully learn the difficult material. – For girls, the opposite was true. – Even though the girls were confident, their fixed mindset caused them to give up easily. Success training • Dweck divided grade-schoolers who already tended to respond to challenge with helplessness into two groups. – Success training group: 15 success trials in which students completed difficult math problems within a time limit – Attribution retraining group: students were set up to fail 2-3 of the 15 trials, and were told “You should have tried harder.” They then were successful on later tasks. Comfort isn’t kind • Instructors with an entity theory of math ability (Rattan, Good, & Dweck, 2012) – Assumptions based on a single low score – More likely to comfort students, advise dropping out of math classes, choose another major, assign less homework – Students interpreted kindness as indicator of low expectations Intervention studies • Is it possible to teach students to adopt an incremental, growth mindset rather than an entity perspective, fixed mindset? Intervention studies • College students – Brain functioning and potential malleability – Wrote letters teaching this message to a struggling middle school student • Control group – Different people have different intellectual strengths – Also wrote letters • .23 increase in overall GPA Intervention studies • 7th-graders received weekly mentoring in person and through email for one year • Explained incremental theory of intelligence • Higher math and verbal scores on statewide achievement tests compared to students in control group who received antidrug emails – Especially girls in math Intervention studies • 7th-graders received 8 sessions of either incremental theory and study skills intervention or study skills intervention alone – Math scores declined for students in study skills group, as is common in transition to middle school – Math scores did not decline during intervention and increased afterward, .30 difference between groups, back to where they started – Learning study skills isn’t enough Intervention studies • Community college students received 8 sessions of incremental theory intervention emphasizing malleability of adult brains: “When people learn and practice new ways of doing algebra or statistics, it can grow their brains—even if they haven’t done well in math in the past.” A Formula for Growing Your Brain: Effort+Good Strategies+Help From Others “It’s not just about effort. You also need to learn skills that let you use your brain in a smarter way…You actually have to practice the right way…to get better at something. In fact, scientists have found that the brain grows more when you learn something new, and less when you practice things you already know.” • Intervention delivered via Internet to more than 200 community college students in developmental math classes. • Half received information about incremental theory of intelligence, half only learned about the brain; all wrote letters to future students. • Intervention delivered via Internet to more than 200 community college students in developmental math classes. • Half received information about incremental theory of intelligence, half only learned about the brain; all wrote letters to future students. • Students in the intervention condition were far more resilient months later. What made the difference? • The intervention changed the meaning of challenge for the students. Instead of making students feel “dumb,” the challenges offered a way to get smarter. The power of “yet” • I can’t do it. • I don’t know how. • This doesn’t make sense. The power of “yet” • I can’t do it…yet. • I don’t know how…yet. • This doesn’t make sense…yet. Two dangers 1. Blaming students 2. Trying to teach mindset as if it’s just another worksheet It’s not just magic words. • Can it really be this simple? Yes, because it only seems simple, but it’s not magic. • Interventions that target mindset or other psychological aspects of how students experience get at just one small aspect of motivation • This does not replace attention to how we teach, what we teach • Increasing students’ motivation to learn only helps when learning opportunities exist Thank you! But wait… • Once students have a growth mindset the next step is not as simple as just telling them to try harder • Students need to know HOW to learn effectively, and most don’t • Effort alone is not enough What comes next? • Metacognition – To learn effectively, students need to understand how to learn. – What does it mean to “study”? Mistaken beliefs about learning What doesn’t work: • Highlighting • Underlining • Rereading or reviewing previously learned material • Memorization of facts • Summarizing What works: • Practice tests for repeated retrieval • Distributed practice • Interleaved practice • Self-explanation • Elaborative interrogation Beliefs about stability • Kidd, Palmeri, & Aslin, 2013 • Rational response to marshmallow task • Interventions must be stealthy to be effective. Growth mindset and the sense of self • We’ve been talking about students’ thinking about intelligence, but can the same understanding be applied to students’ thinking about personality? From Joël Franusic on the book "Art & Fear" by David Bayles & Ted Orland 1 Mindsets and Motivation as the Keys to Academic Success March 27, 2015 Karen Huxtable-Jester, Ph.D. School of Behavioral and Brain Sciences, The University of Texas at Dallas Email: drkarenhj@utdallas.edu, Twitter: @drkarenhj References Aronson, J., Fried, C. B., & Good, C. (2002). Reducing the effects of stereotype threat on African American college students by shaping theories of intelligence. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 38, 113-125. Barker, G. P., & Graham, S. (1987). Developmental study of praise and blame as attributional cues. Journal of Educational Psychology, 79(1), 62-66. Butler, R. Teacher communications and student interpretations: Effects of teacher responses to failing students on attributional inferences in two age groups. British Journal of Educational Psychology, 64, 277-294. Blackwell, L. S., Trzesniewski, K. H., & Dweck, C. S. (2007). Implicit theories of intelligence predict achievement across an adolescent transition: A longitudinal study and an intervention. Child Development, 78(1), 246-263. Carey, B. (2014). How we learn: The surprising truth about when, where, and why it happens. New York, NY: Random House. Dweck, C. S. (2000). Self-theories: Their role in motivation, personality, and development. Philadelphia, PA: Taylor & Francis. Dweck, C. S. (2006). Mindset: The new psychology of success. New York, NY: Ballantine. Good, C., Aronson, J., & Inzlicht, M. (2003). Improving adolescents’ standardized test performance: An intervention to reduce the effects of stereotype threat. Applied Developmental Psychology, 24, 645-662. Graham, S. (2014). Communicating low ability in the classroom: Bad things good teachers sometimes do. In S. Graham, & V. S. Folkes (Eds.), Attribution theory: Application to achievement, mental health, and interpersonal conflict. Hoboken, NJ: Taylor and Francis. Kegan, R., & Lahey, L. L. (2001). From the language of prizes and praising to the language of ongoing regard (Chapter 5). In How the way we talk can change the way we work: Seven languages for transformation. San Francisco, CA: Jossey-Bass. 2 Kidd, C., Palmeri, H., & Aslin, R. N. (2013). Rational snacking: Young children’s decision-making on the marshmallow task is moderated by beliefs about environmental reliability. Cognition, 126, 109114. Kirschner, P. A., & van Merriënboer, J. J. G. (2013). Do learners really know best? Urban legends in education. Educational Psychologist, 48(3), 169-183. Mueller, C. M., & Dweck, C. S. (1998). Praise for intelligence can undermine children’s motivation and performance. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 75(1), 33-52. Ormrod, J. E. (2008). Interpersonal attributions. Supplementary reading in the Companion Website accompanying Human Learning, 5th ed. Upper Saddle River, NJ: Merrill/Prentice Hall. Rattan, A., Good, C., & Dweck, C. S. (2012). “It’s ok—Not everyone can be good at math”: Instructors with an entity theory comfort (and demotivate) students. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 48, 731-737. Wiggins, G. (2012). Seven keys to effective feedback. Educational Leadership, 70(1), 10-16. Yeager, D. S., & Dweck, C. S. (2012). Mindsets that promote resilience: When students believe that personal characteristics can be developed. Educational Psychologist, 47(4), 302-314. Yeager, D. S., Henderson, M. D., Paunesku, D., Walton, G. M., D’Mello, S., Spitzer, B. J., & Duckworth, A. L. (2014). Boring but important: A self-transcendent purpose for learning fosters academic self-regulation. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 107(4), 559-580. Yeager, D. S., Johnson, R., Spitzer, B. J., Trzesniewski, K. H., Powers, J., & Dweck, C. S. (2014). The farreaching effects of believing people can change: Implicit theories of personality shape stress, health, and achievement during adolescence. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 106(6), 867-884. Yeager, D. S., Miu, A. S., Powers, J., & Dweck, C. S. (2013). Implicit theories of personality and attributions of hostile intent: A meta-analysis, an experiment, and a longitudinal intervention. Child Development, 84(5), 1651-1667. Yeager, D. S., Trzesniewski, K. H., & Dweck, C. S. (2012). An implicit theories of personality intervention reduces adolescent aggression in response to victimization and exclusion. Child Development, 84(3), 970-988. Yeager, D. S., & Walton, G. M. (2011). Social-psychological interventions in education: They’re not magic. Review of Educational Research, 81(2), 267-301.
© Copyright 2024