OMEGA, Vol. 61(4) 269-271, 2010 THE DUAL PROCESS MODEL OF COPING WITH BEREAVEMENT: A DECADE LATER VIRGINIA E. RICHARDSON, PH.D., Guest Editor The Ohio State University, Columbus PREFACE Almost exactly 1 decade ago, Dr. Shirley O’Bryant, professor emeritus in human ecology, asked me to oversee her data and students focusing on older bereaved men. Dr. O’Bryant had received funding from the AARP Andrus Foundation to interview 200 older widowers residing in the Central Ohio area who were in their second year of bereavement. Based on these data, Dr. Shantha Balaswamy and I published the first evaluation of Margaret Stroebe and Henk Schut’s newly proposed Dual Process Model of Bereavement (DPM) in the article, “Coping with Bereavement among Elderly Widowers,” published in Omega: Journal of Death and Dying in 2001. We had discovered Stroebe and Schut’s paper, “The Dual Process Model of Coping with Bereavement, Rationale and Description,” published in Death Studies in 1999, and we were intrigued with their challenge to “the grief work hypothesis,” which was the most widely accepted viewpoint on loss and bereavement in popular and scientific journals. The grief work hypothesis claimed that bereaved persons must focus on their feelings of loss or they will experience psychosomatic and other maladaptive symptoms and will never recover from their loss. Bereaved persons must confront their painful feelings and “work through them” in order to avoid developing disordered grief reactions, according to proponents of this perspective. Stroebe and Schut (1999) identified several problems with the grief work hypothesis, including lack of solid supporting evidence, inadequate clarity of the concept and processes, and inaccurate operationalization of concepts. By 269 Ó 2010, Baywood Publishing Co., Inc. doi: 10.2190/OM.61.4.a http://baywood.com 270 / RICHARDSON integrating precepts from chronic stress theory (Lazarus & Folkman, 1984) and Horowitz’s Stress Response Syndrome (Horowitz, 1986), Stroebe and Schut proposed the DPM as an alternative framework with three central constructs: loss-oriented coping, restoration-oriented coping, and oscillation. Dr. Balaswamy and I decided to test this approach using Dr. O’Bryant’s sample of older widowers. As reported in Omega: Journal of Death and Dying in 2001, we found that loss variables, such as death circumstances, influenced negative feelings and were especially critical during the early stages of bereavement, while restorationoriented factors, such as dating, were significantly associated with positive affect and were more relevant later. Although our findings supported the DPM, we recognized several limitations in our research: our data were cross-sectional, the operationalization of constructs was questionable, and our sample was limited to a self-selected group of older bereaved men. When the National Institute of Aging awarded Dr. Deborah Carr and her colleagues funds to organize a workshop on the Changing Lives of Older Couples (CLOC) data at the University of Michigan during the spring of 2002, I jumped at the opportunity to test the DPM using a longitudinal research design comprised of older widows and widowers. My analyses of the CLOC data, which were published in the Journal of Gerontological Social Work in 2007, corroborated our previous findings reported in Omega: Journal of Death and Dying in 2001. Meanwhile others were also testing the DPM. Dale Lund, Michael Caserta, Rebecca Utz, and Brian de Vries received funding from the National Institute of Aging to compare a DPM-intervention with a comparison group receiving traditional support (Lund, Caserta, de Vries, & Wright, 2004). Kate Bennett and her colleagues differentiated copers from noncopers (Bennett, Hughes, & Smith, 2005), and M. Katherine Shear, with support from the National Institute of Mental Health, used a randomized control trial to evaluate an intervention for complicated grief using DPM principles (Shear, Frank, Houck, & Reynolds, 2005). We organized a symposium on the DPM for the annual meeting of the Gerontological Society of America in 2008 and presented our most recent research. The papers from the symposium comprise this special issue of Omega. We are grateful to Ken Doka, editor of Omega: Journal of Death and Dying, for providing us with this opportunity to disseminate our findings on this important model. We launch the issue with a special contribution from Stroebe and Schut, who present the introductory article entitled, “The Dual Process Model of Coping with Bereavement: A Decade On.” This is followed by three empirical studies: Lund, Caserta, Utz, & de Vries’ article, “Experiences and Early Coping of Bereaved Spouses/Partners in an Intervention Based on the Dual Process Model (DPM)”; Bennett, Gibbons, & Mackenzie-Smith’s article, “Loss and Restoration in Later Life: An Examination of the Dual Process Model of Coping with Bereavement; and my article, “Length of Caregiving and Well-Being among Older Widowers: Implications for the Dual Process Model of Bereavement.” These are followed by Shear’s article, “Exploring the Role of Experiential DUAL PROCESS MODEL OF COPING WITH BEREAVEMENT / 271 Avoidance from the Perspective of Attachment Theory and the Dual Process Model,” and Carr’s concluding article, “ New Perspectives on the Dual Process Model (DPM): What Have We Learned? What Questions Remain?” Carr’s article is based on her comments as a reactor during the symposium. As we continue our research on the DPM, we look forward to receiving feedback from clinicians and academicians and from older bereaved persons who are most directly affected by our ideas. REFERENCES Bennett, K. L., Hughes, G. M., & Smith, P. T. (2005). Psychological response to later life widowhood: Coping and the effects of gender. Omega: Journal of Death and Dying, 51, 33-52. Horowitz, M. (1986). Stress response syndromes. Northvale, NJ: Aronson. Lazarus, R., & Folkman, S. (1984). Stress, appraisal, and coping. New York: Springer. Lund, D. A., & Caserta, M. S., de Vries, B., & Wright, S. (2004). Restoration during bereavement. Generations Review, 14, 9-15. Richardson, V. E., & Balaswamy, S. (2001). Coping with bereavement among elderly widowers. Omega: Journal of Death and Dying, 43, 129-144. Richardson, V. E. (2007). A dual process model of grief counseling: Findings from the changing lives of older couples (CLOC) study. Journal of Gerontological Social Work, 48, 311-329. Shear, K., Frank, E., Houck, P., & Reynolds, C. (2005). Treatment of complicated grief: A randomized controlled trial. Journal of the American Medical Association, 293, 2601-2608. Stroebe, M. S., & Schut, H. (1999). The Dual Process Model of coping with bereavement: Rationale and description. Death Studies, 23, 197-224. Direct reprint requests to: Virginia E. Richardson The Ohio State University College of Social Work 1947 College Road Columbus, OH 43210 e-mail: Richardson.2@osu.edu DEATH, VALUE AND MEANING SERIES John D. Morgan, Series Editor Editors: Gerry R. Cox, Robert A. Bendiksen, and Robert G. Stevenson Spirituality is clearly related to identity. It is a search to discover our personal make-up: what we are all about, our innate identity. Letting go of who we are, and what we have acquired for ourselves, has become such an enterprise that, in the face of death, deriving meaning out of what is left becomes an essential project. It is a venture into terra incognita. Yet, this apparent void may prove to be the beginning of a very creative process. When confronted by death, the believer finds that religion can offer a profound sense of direction. The editors of Making Sense of Death: Spiritual, Pastoral, and Personal Aspects of Death, Dying and Bereavement provide stimulating discussions as they ponder the meaning of life and death. This anthology explores the process of meaning-making in the face of death and the roles of religion and spirituality at times of loss; the profound and devastating experience of loss in the death of a spouse or a child; a psychological model of spirituality; the dimensions of spirituality; humor in client-caregiver relationships; the worldview of modernity in contrast to postmodern assumptions; the Buddhist perspective of death, dying, and pastoral care; meaning-making in the virtual reality of cyberspace; individualism and death; the historical context of Native Americans, the concept of disenfranchised grief, and its detailed application to the Native American experience; a qualitative survey on the impact of the shooting deaths of students in Colorado; a team approach with physicians, nursing, social services, and pastoral care; a study of health care professionals, comparing clergy with other health professionals; marginality in spiritual and pastoral care for the dying; a qualitative research study of registered nurses in the northeast United States; and loss and growth in the seasons of life. 6" × 9", 260 Pages, Cloth, ISBN 0-89503-249-X $57.95 + $7.00 p/h in U.S. (please inquire for postage rates outside of U.S.) BAYWOOD PUBLISHING COMPANY, INC. 26 Austin Avenue, P.O. Box 337, Amityville, NY 11701 phone (631) 691-1270 • fax (631) 691-1770 • toll-free orderline (800) 638-7819 e-mail baywood@baywood.com • website http://baywood.com OMEGA, Vol. 61(4) 273-289, 2010 THE DUAL PROCESS MODEL OF COPING WITH BEREAVEMENT: A DECADE ON* MARGARET STROEBE HENK SCHUT Utrecht University, The Netherlands ABSTRACT The Dual Process Model of Coping with Bereavement (DPM; Stroebe & Schut, 1999) is described in this article. The rationale is given as to why this model was deemed necessary and how it was designed to overcome limitations of earlier models of adaptive coping with loss. Although building on earlier theoretical formulations, it contrasts with other models along a number of dimensions which are outlined. In addition to describing the basic parameters of the DPM, theoretical and empirical developments that have taken place since the original publication of the model are summarized. Guidelines for future research are given focusing on principles that should be followed to put the model to stringent empirical test. It is gratifying to know that precisely a decade after the publication of our Dual Process Model of Coping with Bereavement (DPM; Stroebe & Schut, 1999), interest in this model has grown to the extent that a Special Issue in Omega: Journal of Death and Dying is deemed appropriate. Within the past decade various research teams have taken up the challenge both to apply and to test the model, as represented in the following articles in this Special Issue. A scientific model *Parts of this article have been adapted and updated (with permission) from a more detailed review of the DPM by Stroebe and Schut in Grief Matters: The Australian Journal of Grief and Bereavement, 2008, 11, 1-4. 273 Ó 2010, Baywood Publishing Co., Inc. doi: 10.2190/OM.61.4.b http://baywood.com 274 / STROEBE AND SCHUT of this kind is no use unless it can be implemented, and being firm empiricists ourselves, we have always considered it essential to put the model to the test. In our view, this should be undertaken first and foremost by research teams independent of our own, and this, fortunately, is what has occurred. The articles in this volume represent an excellent variety of different types of investigation of the model’s parameters. To understand the significance of these contributions, some background information about the DPM is necessary. Thus, in this article, we first describe the rationale for the DPM and summarize its main parameters. We compare and contrast it with other models along a number of dimensions. Throughout, we indicate where further developments and empirical examinations have taken place during the past decade. We end with suggestions for future research directions, focusing on principles that should be followed to put the model to stringent empirical test. RATIONALE FOR DEVELOPMENT OF THE DPM Our purpose in developing the DPM was to provide a model that would better describe coping and predict good versus poor adaptation to this stressful life event, and by doing so, to better understand individual differences in the ways that people come to terms with bereavement. It is a model, then, of coping with loss, not a generic model aimed at explaining the broad range of phenomena and manifestations associated with bereavement. Coping refers to processes, strategies, or styles of managing (reducing, mastering, tolerating) the situation in which bereavement places the individual. Coping is assumed to impact on adaptation to bereavement. If coping is effective, then not only the suffering, but also the mental and physical ill health difficulties that are associated with bereavement (Stroebe, Schut, & Stroebe, 2007) should be reduced (in time, usually after quite some struggle and turmoil). To understand its impact on outcome, coping must be considered a separate entity from the consequences of bereavement: the former is a process, the latter an outcome variable. Thus (and we return to this later) it becomes essential to differentiate coping (process) from consequences (outcomes) in our empirical investigations. Overall, then, in constructing the DPM, the aim was to postulate regularities in coping processes that are predictive of (non)adaptive outcomes. At the time when the DPM was developed, there were a number of models available that addressed how people go about coming to terms with bereavement (for a review, see Stroebe, in press). Each of these provided guidelines to understand what needs to be done for successful outcome to occur. Among these, the most influential and appealing coping models in the bereavement area during the latter part of the 20th century were the Phase Model (e.g., Bowlby, 1980, see also Parkes, 1972/1996), which was fundamental to attachment theory, and the Task Model (Worden, 1982, 1991, 2002, 2009), which became prominent DUAL PROCESS MODEL: A DECADE ON / 275 in the planning of counseling and therapy programs for bereaved people in need of help. Working through grief (known as “grief work,” following Freud, 1917/1957) was a fundamental notion underlying the development of both the phases and tasks that are integral to these models (see Figure 1, left hand and middle columns). Grief work is understood to refer to the cognitive process of confronting the reality of a loss through death, of going over events that occurred before and at the time of death, and of focusing on memories and working toward detachment from (or relocating) the deceased (Stroebe, 1992). Following this, it was understood that one has to confront the experience of bereavement in order to come to terms with loss and avoid detrimental health consequences. It was in large part due to consideration of these grief work models—with respect to their major strengths but also their perceived limitations—that led us to develop the DPM. As such, and as will become evident, the DPM built on and extended these earlier conceptualizations. Despite the useful guidelines that these prior models evidently offered, we had major concerns about the adequacy of their central construct of grief work in explaining adaptive ways of coping with bereavement. First, there are alternative ways of coming to terms with bereavement. As examinations of certain nonWestern cultural patterns of grieving show, the types of confrontation involved in grief work are not universal, nor is non-confrontation systematically linked with mal-adaptation. Second, the process itself as described in the Phase Model (far less so in the Task Model) seems somewhat passive (as though the person is being put through, rather than actively dealing with), neglecting the effortful struggle that is so much part of grieving. Third, there is no acknowledgment of the need for “dosage” of grief. It is arduous and exhausting to grieve, respite at times is recuperative. Fourth, the benefits of denial have not been taken into account (cf. Bonanno, 2001). Fifth, the grief work notion focuses on the loss of the loved person him- or herself, neglecting the possibility that there may be other sources of stress that arise indirectly following a bereavement (e.g., concerns with finances, legal matters, or upbringing of children as a single parent). Furthermore, our own research had failed to show evidence that persons who were doing grief work adjusted better (W. Stroebe, Schut, & Stroebe, 2005). Finally, different types of “working through” appeared to help different subgroups (Schut, Stroebe, de Keijser, & van den Bout, 1997), suggesting the need for a more nuanced approach to understanding effective coping. Our conclusion was that the grief work model needed revision to define when and for whom—and in what way— working through is efficacious. 1 This reasoning led to the DPM. 1 Bonanno and Kaltman’s (1999) integrative perspective on bereavement also replaced what they viewed as a too-narrow focus on the grief work hypothesis (highlighting a lack of empirical evidence). For a critical appraisal of their approach in comparison with the DPM, see Archer (2001). Figure 1. The Dual Process Model of Coping with Bereavement (Stroebe & Schut, 1999). 276 / STROEBE AND SCHUT DUAL PROCESS MODEL: A DECADE ON / 277 DESCRIPTION OF THE DPM: MAIN PARAMETERS The DPM can be understood as a taxonomy to describe ways that people come to terms with the loss of a loved one (for detailed accounts, see Stroebe & Schut, 1999, 2001). The structure of the DPM parameters owes much to Cognitive Stress Theory (see Folkman, 2001; Lazarus & Folkman, 1984), particularly insofar as it defines a number of key components related to coping. These are: stressors (the nature of the events leading to stress, i.e., the cause, reason for the coping process to be set in motion); appraisal processes (assessment of threat); coping processes (ways of dealing with threat); and outcome variables (e.g., mental and physical health indices). A fundamental contrast with earlier models is that the DPM defines two categories of stressors associated with bereavement, namely, those that are loss- versus restoration-oriented. Loss-orientation refers to the bereaved person’s concentration on, appraising and processing of some aspect of the loss experience itself and as such, incorporates grief work. It involves a painful dwelling on, even searching for the lost person, a phenomenon that lies at the heart of grieving. Restoration-orientation refers to the focus on secondary stressors that are also consequences of bereavement, reflecting a struggle to reorient oneself in a changed world without the deceased person. Rethinking and replanning one’s life in the face of bereavement (a part of restoration orientation) can also be regarded an essential component of grieving (cf. Parkes’s psychosocial transition theory, e.g., 2006). Caserta and Lund (2007) were able to demonstrate that attention was paid to both types of stressors among a sample of widowed persons, and to indicate that these were related to bereavement outcomes, as did Wijngaards, Stroebe, Stroebe, Schut, van den Heijden, et al. (2008). It is important to note that loss- and restoration-oriented coping are not equivalent to the Cognitive Stress Theory concepts of emotion- and problem-focused coping (cf. Billings & Moos, 1981, 1984; Folkman, 2001), although at first sight, one might think that emotion-focused coping seems more loss-oriented, problemfocused coping more restoration-oriented. Emotion-focused coping is directed at managing the emotion that results from stress, problem-focused coping is directed at managing and changing the problem causing the distress (Folkman, 2001; Lazarus & Folkman, 1984). Indeed, some aspects to do with loss orientation may be better dealt with in an emotion-focused manner (e.g., unchangeable things, such as relating to the fact that the deceased cannot be brought back), but other loss-related experiences can also be dealt with in a problem-focused manner (e.g., to keep the deceased close, one can plant and nurture a tree in his/her memory). Likewise, both emotion- and problem-focused strategies can be employed in coping with restoration stressors. For example, consider the need to repair the financial situation following loss of a spouse’s income: Either one can deal with this in an emotion-focused way by worrying and feeling anxious but doing nothing about it, or one can take steps to solve the problem by earning 278 / STROEBE AND SCHUT money oneself. It becomes evident that many loss- and restoration-stressors can be dealt with either in an emotion- or a problem-focused manner (and in fact, both types are typically used: sometimes the former, sometimes the latter will be more deemed appropriate when dealing with loss- or restoration-stressors). Both orientations are sources of stress and can be associated with outcomes such as distress and anxiety. Both are also involved in the coping process, for example, they are attended to (confronted versus avoided) in varying degrees (according to individual and cultural variations). The process of attending to or avoiding these two types of stressor is dynamic and fluctuating, and it also changes over time. Therefore, the DPM specifies a dynamic coping process, namely, a regulatory process labeled oscillation, which distinguishes it from the earlier bereavement models (and also from the more generic cognitive stress theory). The principle underlying oscillation is that at times the bereaved will confront aspects of loss, at other times avoid them, and the same applies to the tasks of restoration. Sometimes, too, there will be “time out,” when the person is not grieving. Coping with bereavement according to the DPM is thus a complex regulatory process of confrontation and avoidance. An important postulation of the model is that oscillation between the two types of stressors is necessary for adaptive coping. The structural components described above are depicted in Figure 1. Table 1 compares the DPM with the previous models, illustrating restoration stressors that need to be dealt with in addition to those postulated in the Task Model. At this point in the development of the model, the primary strategy of coping with the loss and restoration stressors was understood to relate to emotion Table 1. Comparison of Models Phase Model (Bowlby, 1980) Task Model (Worden, 1991) DPM (Stroebe & Schut, 1999) Shock Accept reality of loss Accept reality of loss . . . and accept reality of changed world. Yearning/protest Experience pain of grief Experience pain of grief . . . and take time off from pain of grief. Despair Adjust to life without deceased Adjust to life without deceased . . . and master the changed (subjective) environment. Restitution Relocate deceased emotionally and move on Relocate deceased emotionally and move on . . . and develop new roles, identities, relationships. DUAL PROCESS MODEL: A DECADE ON / 279 regulation, or more precisely, to confrontation versus avoidance (a major coping dimension in Cognitive Stress Theory). We extended the original DPM model to include further analysis of types of cognitive processing in a subsequent paper (Stroebe & Schut, 2001), as depicted in Figure 2. Oscillation between positive and negative affect/(re)appraisal is understood to be an integral part of the coping process, and to be a component of both loss- and restoration-oriented coping. Persistent negative effect enhances grief, yet working through grief, which includes rumination, has been identified as important in coming to terms with loss. On the other hand, positive reappraisals sustain the coping effort. Yet if positive states are maintained relentlessly, grieving is neglected. We drew on the work of Folkman (2001) on positive meaning states, and of Nolen-Hoeksema (2001) on negative appraisals to introduce cognitive pathways into the model. We continue to believe that processes of confrontation-avoidance are central mechanisms in adjustment to bereavement. One line of our current research is directed toward gaining finer-grained understanding of the types of loss- and restoration-oriented cognitions which are associated with normal versus complicated forms of grieving, focusing on rumination as an avoidance process (see Stroebe, Boelen, van den Hout, Stroebe, Salemink, & van den Bout, 2007). Relatedly, Boelen and van den Bout (in press) have used their CognitiveBehavioural Model of Complicated Grief (Boelen, van den Hout, & van den Bout, 2006) to examine assumptions about the role of two types of avoidance in complicated grief, ones which they see as comparable with avoidance of lossand restoration-orientations in our own conceptualization. They described and empirically-tested the role of “anxious avoidance” (avoidance of confrontation with the reality of the loss) and “depressive avoidance” (avoiding engagement in activities that could foster adjustment), dimensions that are clearly compatible with the DPM constructs. Anxious and depressive avoidance emerged as distinct factors and accounted for unique parts of explained variance in grief symptomatology. In our view, their results provide indirect support for our proposition that the two types of stressor are distinct and relevant to adjustment: difficulty in dealing with them is associated with poor outcome. Boelen and van den Bout (in press) provided a fine-grained analysis of pathways between process and outcome variables, to which we return later. THE DPM IN COMPARISON WITH OTHER MODELS Already above, it has become evident that—rather unusually in bereavement research—the DPM draws heavily on pre-existing generic as well as bereavement-specific theories for derivation of its parameters. We consider it a strength that the DPM integrates major theoretical perspectives such as attachment and cognitive stress theories. But does the DPM add substantially to previous models, notably, the Phase and Task Models? Worden (2009), in the 4th edition Figure 2. Appraisal processes in the Dual Process Model (cf. Stroebe & Schut, 2001). 280 / STROEBE AND SCHUT DUAL PROCESS MODEL: A DECADE ON / 281 of his monograph, actually argued that there is little difference between his Task Model and the DPM, noting that the tasks are “almost identical” with our stressors. This is more the case for his revised, post-DPM Task Model. Worden’s tasks have been reformulated over the years since the DPM was first published, and they are now more explicitly inclusive of restoration stressors, and more in line with our additions in Table 1. Worden’s (2009) Task 2 is now: “To process the pain of grief”; Task 3: “To adjust to a world without the deceased”; Task 4: “To find an enduring connection with the deceased in the midst of embarking on a new life” (p. 50). Even though, as evident in these new tasks, there is now more similarity with the DPM, we consider our explicit distinction of the two categories of loss- and restoration-stressors to be unique, to reflect the reality that bereaved people experience, and to be useful for both clinical application and research investigation. There are a number of other distinguishing features between the models. Worden does not distinguish stressors from coping processes, as we do, or make predictions about (mal)adjustment in relationship specifically to these different component parts. Furthermore, Worden argued that there is similarity in that, according to his model, people go back and forth between his tasks as needed, like our principle of oscillation. Consider oscillation: Although shifting back and forth between the tasks is implicit in his model, merely stating this fails to postulate emotion regulation (confrontation and avoidance) as a fundamental parameter of coping, as we do in our model: The principle of oscillation captures and highlights the necessity for attention to the different categories of stressors. Finally, contrary to what Worden claims, our model posits flexibility (it is fundamental to oscillation), certainly not the “fixed patterns” that he mentions in considering our model, and it caters for individual and subgroup differences (see below). There are a number of additional respects in which we consider the DPM to add to other formulations, the major ones are briefly described next (for more details, see Stroebe & Schut, 1999, 2008). First, the DPM provides a framework for understanding forms of complicated grief, such as chronic, or absent, delayed, inhibited grief (cf. Lindemann, 1944; Parkes & Weiss, 1983) in a way that was not nearly so differentiated or explicit in the previous models, with chronic grievers focusing on loss, absent grievers on restoration-oriented activities, while those who suffer a complicated form of traumatic bereavement might be expected to have trouble alternating smoothly between loss- and restoration-orientation, manifesting extreme symptoms of intrusion and avoidance (for details, see Stroebe & Schut, 2008). It is important to note that in both loss-oriented (e.g., chronic) and restoration-oriented (e.g., absent) types of complicated grief, reactions are extreme, with extensive focus on the one orientation and avoidance of the other. Such patterns are associated with an absence of the type of confrontation-avoidance (oscillation) that we have described as characteristic of “normal” coping with bereavement. In general, there are substantial individual 282 / STROEBE AND SCHUT differences in the extent to which (normally) bereaved persons focus on lossor restoration-orientation: only in extreme cases of confrontation of the one, and avoidance of the other are complications in grieving and poor adaptation likely to occur. In recent publications, the relationship between complicated grief and patterns of attachment has been explored within the context of the DPM (e.g., Mikulincer & Shaver, 2008; Parkes, 2006; M. Stroebe, Schut, & W. Stroebe, 2005). In fact, one of the most important developments of the DPM has emerged from exploration of its links with attachment theory constructs (e.g., Mikulincer & Shaver, 2008; Parkes, 2006; Stroebe, Stroebe, & Schut, 2005; Stroebe, Schut, & Stroebe, 2005a), and some empirical evidence has accumulated in support of these connections in the meantime (see Stroebe & Schut, 2008). We have also explored how attachment style differences influence patterns of disclosure in coping with bereavement (see Stroebe, Schut, & Stroebe, 2005b). Turning next to (sub)group differences: As described in Stroebe and Schut (1999), the DPM also accommodated male and female differences in ways of grieving better than the previous models described above and, more recently, we have explored gender differences in coping with bereavement in relationship to health outcomes, using the DPM framework (see Stroebe, Stroebe, & Schut, 2001). Women appear to be more loss-oriented following bereavement, feeling and expressing their distress at their loss; men more restoration-oriented, actively engaging with the problems and practical issues associated with loss (Wijngaards et al., 2008; cf. Parkes, 2006). Again it is important to note that focusing on lossorientation among women and restoration-orientation among men may generally work well, unless one or other orientation is adhered to in the extreme (indicated by an absence of oscillation to the other orientation). Contrary to subsequent criticism that the DPM is an intra-personal model (like the preceding models), in our 1999 paper we already described interpersonal coping processes that the model has the potential to incorporate, acknowledging that one person’s way of grieving impacts on that of another. The gender differences described above provide an illustration: if, say, a bereaved father is more restoration-oriented, a mother more loss-oriented, attributions may be made in terms of differences in extremity of grief, for example, a mother might assume “he is grieving less than I am” rather than what may actually be the case, that “he is grieving differently.” Making the former attribution could negatively impact the couple’s adjustment to bereavement. More recent evidence regarding the influence of such interpersonal coping processes from a DPM perspective has been found: Wijngaards et al. (2008) used the DPM framework to examine the relationship between a bereaved parent’s own and their partner’s way of coping in relationship to their adjustment to the death of their child. Interpersonal factors were indeed found to play a part in coping and adjustment. For example, one of the main findings was that, for fathers, having a wife who was high in restoration-oriented coping was related to positive adjustment. DUAL PROCESS MODEL: A DECADE ON / 283 We mentioned earlier that cultural differences in ways of working through grief were a major reason for the need to revise earlier conceptions. Cultures vary according to the norms/belief systems which govern manifestations and expressions of grief. These can be understood according to loss- versus restorationoriented coping. A clear example has been provided by the anthropologist Wikan (e.g., 1988). The Muslim community on the island of Bali would be described as restoration-oriented, showing little or no overt sign of grief and outwardly continuing daily life as though nothing untoward had happened. By contrast, the Muslim community in Egypt expresses their grief openly, gathering together to reminisce and share anguish over their loss. Other vivid examples of such cultural differences that are compatible with our DPM formulation can be found in Rosenblatt (2008). Although we also mentioned changes in patterns of coping over time, in our 1999 article we did not elaborate much on this aspect. It is important to note that, like the Phase and Task Models, changes are expected across the duration of bereavement according to the DPM. There will gradually (and unevenly) be less attention to loss-oriented and more to restoration-oriented tasks. For example, early in bereavement there is generally comparatively little attention to forming a new identity and far more to going over the events to do with the death, while over time a gradual reversal in attention to these different aspects is likely to take place. Furthermore, as time goes on, the total amount of time spent on coping with loss and restoration tasks will diminish. Some evidence for these patterns is now available (e.g., Caserta & Lund, 2007; Richardson & Balaswamy, 2001; Stroebe & Schut, 2008). Of course, both the Phase and Task Models incorporate changes over time too, but without specifying these so explicitly in relationship to different types of stressors. We originally formulated the DPM to address coping after partner loss, since this had been the focus of our empirical research before that time. By contrast, the other models were not limited to any particular type of loss (of a child, parent, spouse, etc.), and we have come to realize that this is probably also the case for application of the DPM. Indeed, more recently we have explored application of the model to partners coping with the death of their child (e.g., Wijngaards et al., 2008) and to bereavement specifically among the elderly (Hansson &. Stroebe, 2007). Other teams of researchers have addressed additional types of bereavement, for example, Stokes, Pennington, Monroe, Papadatou, and Relf (1999) have applied the DPM to children and other family members. We have also suggested its application to the phenomenon of homesickness, which can be regarded as a “mini-grief” experience (Stroebe, van Vliet, Hewstone, & Willis, 2002). We have sometimes been asked about the applicability of the DPM to other stressful life events, such as divorce or dealing with the chronic illness of a loved one. There are certain parallels across such different types of events in loss and restoration domains, but it remains for researchers and clinicians to explore the usefulness of the DPM to them empirically. 284 / STROEBE AND SCHUT GUIDELINES FOR TESTING THE DPM Although the DPM looks quite straightforward as presented in Figure 1, it is difficult to test its parameters and/or empirically examine the relationship of the postulated coping processes to bereavement outcomes. The guidelines are summarized in Table 2. Differentiating Stressors, Coping, and Outcome Given that we have postulated two different categories of stressors, loss- and restoration-oriented, a useful direction for research is simply first to show that bereaved people actually have to deal with aspects that fall within these two categories: here, the focus is not on coping strategies or on the outcomes of dealing with them, but on the (range of) experiences that have to be dealt with, per se, the stressors. Thus, in doing such research, it is important to keep clear that the loss or restoration stressors are not equivalent to “coping with” or “being restored.” An example hopefully makes this distinction clearer. Consider one restoration stressor, namely, the problem of changed identity from wife to widow: the stressor Table 2. Recommended Guidelines for Testing the DPM: Summary •Separate stressors from process from outcome variables (e.g., make sure no symptoms are included among coping items). •Best test of relationship between DPM coping and outcome involves maladjusted versus adjusted bereaved persons. •Observe scientific design principles: (e.g., control groups—e.g., non-intervention control groups in intervention studies; longitudinal investigation: before, after, and follow-up). •Extend beyond questionnaire measures (mobile phones, mobile internet, PDA’s for monitoring; diaries; intervention principles, etc.). •Specify (in definitions and operationalizations) the precise parameter under investigation (the two types of stressors; coping processes; oscillation process; coping in relationship to outcome). •Integrate other theoretical perspectives to refine DPM predictions. •Keep in mind that: •Normal reactions can vary greatly between individuals and groups with respect to preferred focus on loss- versus restoration tasks: for some loss-orientation will dominate, for others, restoration-orientation. •Only in cases of extreme, unrelenting, exclusive adherence to (focus on) one or the other type of stressor, or in cases of disturbance of the oscillation process itself, will maladaptation occur. •Loss- and restoration-orientation are not equivalent to emotion- and problem-focused coping. DUAL PROCESS MODEL: A DECADE ON / 285 should be formulated in words such as “I have trouble finding a place in life without my spouse.” Coping with this stressor would be tapped with items such as “I avoid going on dates with potential new partners,” while “I have a new identity or role in life” indicates outcome. Assessing Oscillation It is not easy to investigate the process of oscillation since it is a dynamic process of confrontation and avoidance that can change not only from moment to moment, but also in relationship to the duration of bereavement. Methods other than questionnaire investigations are thus advisable. Questionnaire items asking about shifting attention from loss- to restoration-oriented stressors have sometimes been used, but rather these fail to capture the dynamics of oscillation. Some suggestions would be to use: • Cell phones, mobile internet, personal digital assistants (PDA’s); diaries, and time sampling (these may indeed usefully include—but not necessarily be limited to—questionnaires). • Laboratory techniques to induce shifting (e.g., by presenting stimuli to do with the loss, and then to do with restoration). Examining DPM Parameters in Relationship to Outcome It is important to note that there are individual differences in the amount of attention paid to LO and RO stressors within the normal range of reactions to bereavement: some will have a tendency/preference to focus more on their loss, others more on restoration tasks; some will spend much time, others little time on either or both of these dimensions. Within a moderate range, that is, one that does not exclude attention to either or both types of stressor, the prediction will be that persons adapt to loss in time. Only in extreme cases, such as focusing unremittingly and exclusively on loss, will there be poor outcome. It follows from this that the best test of the DPM will be to compare samples of persons experiencing poor outcomes such as complicated grief with those who are undergoing a normal grief process. The DPM postulates a number of patterns that will be predictive of poor outcome, and which could be used to formulate hypotheses: 1. extreme attention to (i.e., coping with) loss orientation, avoidance of restoration 2. extreme attention to (i.e., coping with) restoration orientation, avoidance of loss 3. high scores on 1 and 2 and no “time off”? 4. disturbance of oscillation (disturbed intrusion-avoidance) 5. high scores on the number of stressors (could be both LO and RO) 286 / STROEBE AND SCHUT The DPM coping-adaptation research paradigm described here can be conducted on an interpersonal level. For example, the Wijngaards et al. (2008) investigation cited earlier investigated differences in adaptation among couples as a function of the amount of loss- versus restoration-oriented coping of each member of the couple. Clearly, additional hypotheses need to be developed to make couple-level predictions as in this latter project. It is also important to remember that outcomes can include a variety of consequences besides normal and complicated grief. It is important to look beyond manifestations of grief and even other symptoms (depression, anxiety, physical health symptoms, etc.), to think in terms of broader aspects relating to personal functioning, such as outcome acceptance (i.e. …), sense of control, self-efficacy, relationship/marital satisfaction, attachment, and emotional equilibrium. Research Design Features for Testing the DPM We have already mentioned a number of features for the design of empirical tests of the DPM. In addition, it is important to stress that longitudinal, preferably prospective designs should be used in order to establish what is cause and what is effect. When it comes to testing the effects of intervention using the DPM, it goes without saying that randomized controlled trials are necessary. This brings us to our next point. Intervention Principles: A Test of the DPM Following DPM principles, if the bereaved person is suffering from complications in their grieving process, intervening to change his or her pattern of confronting versus avoiding loss- and restoration-stressors should lead to better adjustment. This is precisely what Shear, Frank, Houck, and Reynolds (2005) did. They used the DPM as a guideline for designing one intervention program (labeled Complicated Grief Treatment, CGT) and evaluated the efficacy of this program against an established one (Interpersonal Psychotherapy, IPT). The therapist described the DPM to clients with complicated grief and emphasized the need to focus on restoration as well as loss tasks, which were both addressed in the therapy sessions. The DPM-type CGT intervention was more effective (even) than IPT. This suggests that the processes identified in the DPM may indeed be central in coming to terms with bereavement. Again, the authors emphasize the need for follow-up investigation, and indeed, it is too early to conclude that the DPM base was the or even a success factor. Furthermore, in the absence of a non-bereaved control, one cannot be sure that either the CGT or IPT interventions were really effective for treating CG. Nevertheless, this study is an excellent start and good example of how DPM parameters can be included and tested in intervention. Testing DPM (see Table 2) using intervention should focus on high risk groups or those with complicated grief (see Schut et al., 1997; Schut & Stroebe, 2005). DUAL PROCESS MODEL: A DECADE ON / 287 CONCLUDING REMARKS There is enormous scope for further research on the DPM, and, in our view, the best way forward is to follow the sorts of guidelines that we have outlined above. Both theoretical and empirical contributions are needed, the former to refine and extend the DPM framework and further identify specific underlying cognitive processes, the latter to test the model’s parameters and their power in predicting good versus poor adjustment to bereavement. The input of different research teams—such as those illustrated in the following articles in this Special Issue— will be invaluable in this endeavor. REFERENCES Archer, J. (2001). Broad and narrow perspectives in grief theory: Comment on Bonanno and Kaltman (1999). Psychological Bulletin, 127, 554-560. Billings, A., & Moos, R. (1981). The role of coping resources and social resources in attenuating the stress of life events. Journal of Behavioral Medicine, 4, 139-157. Billings, A., & Moos, R. (1984). Coping, stress, and social resources among adults with unipolar depression. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 46, 877-891. Boelen, P. A., & van den Bout, J. (in press). Anxious and depressive avoidance and symptoms of prolonged grief, depression, and Post Traumatic Stress-Disorder. Psychologica Belgica. Boelen, P. A., van den Hout, M. A. & van den Bout, J. (2006). A cognitive-behavioral conceptualization of complicated grief. Clinical Psychology: Science and Practice, 13, 109-128. Bonanno, G. (2001). Grief and emotion: A social-functional perspective. In M. S. Stroebe, R. O. Hansson, W. Stroebe, & H. Schut (Eds.), Handbook of bereavement research: Consequences, coping and care (pp. 493-515). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association. Bonanno, G., & Kaltman, S. (1999). Toward an integrative perspective on bereavement. Psychological Bulletin, 125, 760-776. Bowlby, J. (1980). Attachment and loss; Vol. 3, Loss: Sadness and depression. London: Hogarth. Caserta, M., & Lund, D. (2007). Toward the development of an inventory of daily widowed life (IDWL): Guided by the Dual Process Model of coping with bereavement. Death Studies, 31, 505-535. Folkman, S. (2001). Revised coping theory and the process of bereavement. In M. S. Stroebe, R. O. Hansson, W. Stroebe, & H. Schut (Eds.), Handbook of bereavement research: Consequences, coping and care (pp. 563-584). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association. Freud, S. (1917/1957). Mourning and melancholia. In J. Strachey (Ed. & Trans.), The standard edition of the complete works of Sigmund Freud (Vol. 14, pp. 152-170). London: Hogarth Press. Hansson, R. O., & Stroebe, M. S. (2007). Bereavement in later life: Coping, adaptation, and developmental influences. Washington, DC: American Psychological Association. Lazarus, R., & Folkman, S. (1984). Stress, appraisal, and coping. New York: Springer. 288 / STROEBE AND SCHUT Lindemann, E. (1944). Symptomatology and management of acute grief. American Journal of Psychiatry, 101, 141-148. Mikulincer, M., & Shaver, P. (2008). An attachment perspective on bereavement. In M. S. Stroebe, R. O. Hansson, H. Schut, & W. Stroebe (Eds.), Handbook of bereavement research and practice: Advances in theory and intervention (pp. 87-112). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association. Nolen-Hoeksema, S. (2001). Ruminative coping and adjustment to bereavement. In M. Stroebe, R. O. Hansson, W. Stroebe, & H. Schut (Eds.), Handbook of bereavement research: Consequences, coping and care (pp. 545-562). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association. Parkes, C. M. (1972/1996). Bereavement: Studies of grief in adult life. London: Routledge. Parkes, C. M. (2006). Love and loss: The roots of grief and its complications. London: Routledge. Parkes, C. M., & Weiss, R. (1983). Recovery from bereavement. New York: Basic Books. Richardson, V., & Balaswamy, S. (2001). Coping with bereavement among elderly widowers. Omega, 43, 129-144. Rosenblatt, P. (2008). Grief across cultures: A review and research agenda. In M. S. Stroebe, R. O. Hansson, H. Schut, & W. Stroebe (Eds.), Handbook of bereavement research and practice: Advances in theory and intervention (pp. 207-222). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association. Schut, H. A. W., Stroebe, M. S., de Keijser, J., & van den Bout, J. (1997). Intervention for the bereaved: Gender differences in the efficacy of grief counselling. British Journal of Clinical Psychology, 36, 63-72. Shear, K., Frank, E., Houck, P., & Reynolds, C. (2005). Treatment of complicated grief: A randomized controlled trial. Journal of the American Medical Association, 293, 2601-2608. Stokes, J., Pennington, J., Monroe, B., Papadatou, D., & Relf, M. (1999). Developing services for bereaved children: A discussion of the theoretical and practical issues involved. Mortality, 4, 291-307. Stroebe, M. (in press). Coping with bereavement. In S. Folkman (Ed.), Handbook of stress, coping and health. Oxford: Oxford University Press. Stroebe, M. S. (1992). Coping with bereavement: A review of the grief work hypothesis. Omega: Journal of Death and Dying, 26, 19-42. Stroebe, M. S., Boelen, P., van der Hout, M., Stroebe, W., Salemink, E., & van den Bout, J. (2007). Ruminative coping as avoidance: A reinterpretation of its function in adjustment to bereavement. European Archives of Psychiatry and Clinical Neuroscience, 257, 462-472. Stroebe, M. S., & Schut, H. (1999). The Dual Process Model of coping with bereavement: Rationale and description. Death Studies, 23, 197-224. Stroebe, M. S., & Schut, H. (2001). Meaning making in the Dual Process Model. In R. Neimeyer (Ed.), Meaning reconstruction and the experience of loss (pp. 55-73). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association. Stroebe, M., & Schut, H. (2008). The Dual Process Model of Coping with Bereavement: Overview and update. Grief Matters: The Australian Journal of Grief and Bereavement, 11, 1-4. Stroebe, M., Schut, H., & Stroebe, W. (2005a). Attachment in coping with bereavement: A theoretical integration. Review of General Psychology, 9, 48-66. DUAL PROCESS MODEL: A DECADE ON / 289 Stroebe, W., Schut, H., & Stroebe, M. S. (2005b). Grief work, disclosure and counseling: Do they help the bereaved? Clinical Psychology Review, 25, 395-414. Stroebe, M., Schut, H., & Stroebe, W. (2007). Health consequences of bereavement: A review. The Lancet, 370, 1960-1973. Stroebe, M. S., Stroebe, W., & Schut, H. (2001). Gender differences in adjustment to bereavement: An empirical and theoretical review. Review of General Psychology, 5, 62-83. Stroebe, M., Stroebe, W., & Schut, H. (2005). Who benefits from disclosure? Exploration of attachment style differences in the effects of expressing emotions. Clinical Psychology Review, 26, 66-85. Stroebe, M., van Vliet, T., Hewstone, M., & Willis, H. (2002). Homesickness among students of two cultures: Antecedents and consequences. British Journal of Psychology, 93, 147-168. Wijngaards, L., Stroebe, M. S., Stroebe, W., Schut, H., van den Bout, J., van der Heijden, P., et al. (2008). Parents grieving the loss of their child: Interdependence in coping. British Journal of Clinical Psychology, 47, 31-42. Wikan, U. (1988). Bereavement and loss in two Muslim communities: Egypt and Bali compared. Social Science and Medicine, 27, 451-460. Worden, J. (1982/1991/2002/2009). Grief counselling and grief therapy: A handbook for the mental health practitioner (4th ed.). New York: Springer. Direct reprint requests to: Margaret Stroebe Department of Clinical & Health Psychology Utrecht University Box 80140 3508 TC Utrecht, The Netherlands e-mail: M.S.Stroebe@UU.NL OMEGA, Vol. 61(4) 291-313, 2010 EXPERIENCES AND EARLY COPING OF BEREAVED SPOUSES/PARTNERS IN AN INTERVENTION BASED ON THE DUAL PROCESS MODEL (DPM)* DALE LUND, PH.D. California State University San Bernardino MICHAEL CASERTA, PH.D. REBECCA UTZ, PH.D. University of Utah, Salt Lake City BRIAN DE VRIES, PH.D. San Francisco State University ABSTRACT This study was designed to test the effectiveness of the Dual Process Model (DPM) of coping with bereavement. The sample consisted of 298 recently widowed women (61%) and men age 50+ who participated in 14 weekly intervention sessions and also completed before (O1) and after (O2) selfadministered questionnaires. While the study also includes two additional follow-up assessments (O3 and O4) that cover up to 14-16 months bereaved, this article examines only O1 and O2 assessments. Based on random assignment, 128 persons attended traditional grief groups that focused on loss-orientation (LO) in the model and 170 persons participated in groups receiving both the LO and restoration-orientation (RO) coping (learning daily life skills). As expected, participants in DPM groups showed slightly higher *Funded by a grant from the National Institute on Aging (R01 AG023090). The content is solely the responsibility of the authors and does not necessarily represent the official views of the National Institute on Aging or the National Institutes of Health. 291 Ó 2010, Baywood Publishing Co., Inc. doi: 10.2190/OM.61.4.c http://baywood.com 292 / LUND ET AL. use of RO coping initially, but compared with LO group participants they improved at similar levels and reported similar high degrees of satisfaction with their participation (i.e., having their needs met and 98-100% indicating they were glad they participated. Even though DPM participants had six fewer LO sessions, they showed similar levels of LO improvement. Qualitative data indicate that the RO component of the DPM might be more effective if it is tailored and delivered individually. In this article we present and describe the preliminary results of our intervention study based on the Dual Process Model (DPM) of coping with bereavement (Stroebe & Schut, 1999). Our study compared the experiences of recently bereaved men and women age 50+ who received both of the DPM features (i.e., loss-orientation [LO] and restoration-orientation [RO]) with others in a comparison group who received only the LO features in their intervention. There are three primary purposes of this article. First, we describe and compare the experiences and evaluations of those recently bereaved spouses/partners who participated in the two group conditions described above. Second, we examine the extent to which the LO and RO coping processes were enhanced by participating in each of the two study conditions, DPM treatment versus the comparison group. Third, and based on the findings of the above, we identify the most valuable and promising features of the DPM intervention for future research and practice. While other articles in this special issue deal with different populations of bereaved persons, we focused exclusively with recently widowed men and women age 50+ regardless of the level of early coping difficulty. In other words, we did not select only those who were experiencing complicated or traumatic grief. Rather, we wanted to test the DPM with a broad representation of bereaved persons in mid and later life. Furthermore, although the larger study was designed to test the effectiveness of both study conditions in terms of various bereavement adjustment outcomes over time, these data will be analyzed in future manuscripts. Therefore, this report is focused on the early coping processes targeted by the intervention. The DPM describes loss-orientation and restoration-orientation as coping responses to two primary types of stressors that bereaved persons experience with each type of stress requiring specific types of coping behaviors and strategies. These processes, in turn, influence bereavement adjustment outcomes related to well-being. BACKGROUND ON SPOUSE/PARTNER BEREAVEMENT The consequences of spousal/partner loss in later life have been well documented (Bennett, Hughes, & Smith, 2005; Bisconti, Bergeman, & Boker, 2004; Carr, Nesse, & Wortman, 2006; Hansson & Stroebe, 2007; Lee & Carr, 2007; EXPERIENCES IN INTERVENTION BASED ON THE DPM / 293 Lund, 1989; Lund & Caserta, 2002; Stroebe, Stroebe, & Hansson, 1993). Although the long-term bereavement process is experienced with considerable variability, some common elements have included profound sadness, pining, depression, altered identity, negative health outcomes, loneliness, and the withdrawal of support networks. Additionally, there is evidence of considerable stress associated with the role changes that accompany widowhood, particularly those due to disruptions in life patterns and daily routine, taking on new unfamiliar tasks, and changes in social activities and relationships (Anderson & Dimond, 1995; Carr, 2004; Moss, Moss, & Hansson, 2001; Utz, Carr, Nesse, & Wortman, 2002; Utz, Reidy, Carr, Nesse, & Wortman, 2004). Persons overwhelmed or preoccupied with grief often neglect their own nutrition, fail to exercise regularly, discontinue physical and social activities that they previously did as a married couple, and become more accident prone from paying less attention to their personal safety (Johnson, 2002; Quandt, McDonald, Arcury, Bell, & Vitolins, 2000; Schone & Weinick 1998; Shahar, Schultz, Shahar, & Wing, 2001). The loss of a spouse or partner can be disruptive to existing health care practices, as well as interfere with the adoption of new healthy behaviors (Chen, Gill, & Prigerson, 2005; Pienta & Franks, 2006; Powers & Wampold, 1994; Rosenbloom & Whittington, 1993; Williams, 2004). Spouse/partner bereavement can adversely impact the performance of daily living tasks that are essential for health and independent functioning. For example, meal planning and preparation, household maintenance, managing finances, as well as other tasks often go unattended by the surviving spouse if these tasks were primarily the responsibility of his or her deceased partner. Those who fail to acquire new skills to accomplish these tasks are at increased risk for long-term mental and physical health problems following widowhood (Carr, House, Kessler, Nesse, Sonnega, & Wortman, 2000; Lund, Caserta, Dimond, & Shaffer, 1989b; Stroebe & Schut, 1999; Utz, 2006; Wells & Kendig, 1997). While spouse or partner loss is often associated with a variety of disruptive and negative outcomes, research and theory also has focused on successful adaptation, resiliency, and personal growth among the bereaved (Boerner, Wortman, & Bonanno, 2005; Bonanno, 2004; Calhoun & Tedeschi, 2006; Caserta, Lund, Utz, & de Vries, 2009; Dutton & Zisook, 2005; Lund, Utz, Caserta, & de Vries, 2008; Montpetit, Bergeman, Bisconti, & Rausch, 2006; Ong & Bergeman, 2004; Ong, Bergeman, & Bisconti, 2004; O’Rourke, 2004; Wilcox, Evenson, Aragaki, Wassertheil-Smoller, Mouton, & Loevinger, 2003; Znoj, 2006). Several conceptual models have emerged to describe these disruptive processes and outcomes—most notably Worden’s (2002) “tasks of grief” and other more general stress and coping models (Lazarus & Folkman, 1984). These approaches, however, pay little attention to other concurrent restorative processes (i.e., in addition to the LO processes) and potential positive consequences like opportunities for personal growth through learning, having new experiences and helping others (Doka & Martin, 2001; Lund, 1999; Lund et al., 2008). 294 / LUND ET AL. DUAL PROCESS MODEL OF COPING WITH BEREAVEMENT The Dual Process Model (DPM) of coping with bereavement has emerged as a response to the limitations associated with earlier conceptual frameworks. While the first article in this special issue of Omega (Stroebe & Schut, 2010) presents an overview of and the latest developments regarding the DPM (Stroebe & Schut, 1999), we provide a description of the key elements of the model, with particular attention to how it guided the development of our intervention and influenced the selection of our process and outcome measures. The DPM identifies two concurrent types of stressors and coping processes. First, loss-orientation (LO) refers to the coping processes (including grief work) directly focused on the stress attributed to the loss itself, encompassing many of the grief-related feelings and behaviors that tend to dominate early but can re-emerge later and sporadically in the bereavement process. Second, restoration-orientation (RO) refers to those processes the bereaved use to cope with the secondary stressors that accompany new roles, identities, and challenges related to the new status as a widow, widower, or bereaved partner. These often include the need to master new tasks, make important decisions, meet new role expectations, and take greater self-care initiative. If RO progresses effectively, self-efficacy beliefs emerge and help facilitate greater confidence, independence, and autonomy needed to manage their daily lives (Caserta, 2003; Lund & Caserta, 2002; Utz, 2006). Another desired outcome is a sense of personal growth as a result of becoming more independent and learning new skills (Calhoun & Tedeschi, 2006; Caserta et al., 2009; Dutton & Zisook, 2005; Schaefer & Moos, 2001). Another feature of the DPM is the need to take brief periods of respite from grieving itself, whether to address these new tasks or demands or to keep busy with diversionary or meaningful activities to help restore a sense of balance and well-being. There is evidence that engaging in physical activity, hobbies, volunteer projects, and other leisure activities, as well as socializing and being involved with others provides an important source of restoration and respite for the bereaved. As well, our previous research found a strong association between perceived competencies in tasks of daily living and more favorable adjustments to psycho-emotional aspects of grief (Lund, Caserta, & Dimond, 1989a) The RO coping process is amenable to intervention by focusing on self-efficacy, skills related to new unfamiliar tasks of daily living, self-care, and opportunities to engage in activities that provide brief periods of respite from grief. What distinguishes the DPM from the other more global stress and coping frameworks is the recognition that the bereaved will oscillate between the two coping processes (RO, LO) throughout the course of bereavement as demands arise in their daily lives, even on a moment to moment basis (Caserta & Lund, 2007; Richardson & Balaswamy, 2001; Stroebe & Schut, 1999). In previous EXPERIENCES IN INTERVENTION BASED ON THE DPM / 295 research, we identified and described several potentially important dimensions of “oscillation” (Caserta & Lund, 2007), including the degree of balance, frequency, awareness, control, and intent of oscillation. Although there have been exceptions (Caserta, Lund, & Obray, 2004; Caserta, Lund, & Rice, 1999), most of the previous and current bereavement interventions focus primarily on emotional impacts of the loss itself (LO) and have rarely addressed the concurrent RO issues that the bereaved confront in their daily lives. Our intervention, based on the DPM provides coping assistance directed at both the LO and RO components and encourages the oscillation between them by alternating attention to them in group sessions/meetings. THE DPM-BASED INTERVENTION We refer to this study as the “Living After Loss” (LAL) project; the name was chosen as a way to identify the program to the research participants and community professionals without sounding too academic or theoretical. We designed the LAL study specifically to compare a DPM-based intervention (with both the LO and RO components) with the traditional support group format as the comparison group (which provides attention primarily to LO coping processes). We hypothesized that those in the DPM condition would have more positive and broadly based coping processes and outcomes than those who were in the LO only comparison condition. Our present analyses are focused on the DPM coping processes and not on the adjustment outcomes as those data are not yet available. Both the DPM and comparison conditions consisted of 14 weekly sessions, each about 90 minutes in length. Those in the DPM group had seven of 14 sessions devoted to RO issues, whereas all 14 sessions within the comparison group were solely loss-oriented in focus. The RO-focused sessions of the DPM condition were alternated with the LO-focused sessions to simulate the oscillation between the LO and RO coping processes. For those in the DPM groups, they had fewer LO sessions but the content for these sessions was the same as those in the LO only comparison groups. Table 1 provides a detailed description of the content and theoretical principles addressed in each of the 14 weekly sessions of the DPM intervention. Additional details about the intervention content and format are available upon request to the first author and/or in another publication (Lund, Caserta, de Vries, & Wright, 2004).\ RESEARCH DESIGN AND SAMPLING PROCEDURES Eligibility for the LAL project was restricted to widowed persons age 50 and older, whose spouse or partner had died within the previous 2-6 months, who were English speaking, and who were cognitively and physically able to complete questionnaires and participate in a 14-week group meeting. Participants 296 / LUND ET AL. Table 1. Description of the 14-Week Dual Process Model (DPM) Intervention Session 1 LO Content • Introduction and overview of group and dual Link to model • Grief work processes of grief • Grief: What is it, typical reactions, how the groups might help • Journaling and writing one’s thoughts and feelings • Circumstances surrounding spouse’s death • Specific concerns of participants 2, 3 LO • Physical sensations, cognitions, behaviors • How grief affects daily functioning • How to express grief-related feelings • Grief work • Intrusion of grief 4 LO • New responsibilities: One’s unique situation and • Grief work • Intrusion of grief • Avoidance of common experiences • New roles: Welcomed, anger, frustration, feeling overwhelmed, etc. • Homework: What is one new responsibility you want to do better? 5 RO Goal Setting and Personal Priorities • How to take more control of the situation • Goal setting as a way to learn new skills and 6 LO restoration • Attending to life changes behaviors • Sharing ideas for success • How to “take a break” from grief; attending to one’s personal needs • Homework: Goal Setting—Do something nice for yourself. • Doing new things • Distraction from • How to put own needs first • Dealing with loneliness • Coping with critical time periods (birthdays, • Intrusion of grief • Avoidance of grief restoration anniversaries, holidays) 7 RO Self-Care and Health Care • Meeting one’s own health needs: To what extent was spouse a “partner” in health-related needs? • How to utilize the health care system and service network effectively (immunizations, screenings, medication management, communicating with providers, local services and programs available). 8 LO • What do participants miss and not miss about spouse? • Unfinished business • Attending to life changes • Grief work • Reframing ties with spouse EXPERIENCES IN INTERVENTION BASED ON THE DPM / 297 Table 1. (Cont’d.) Session 9 RO Content Finances and Legal Issues • Wills and trusts • Filing legal and important documents • Understanding statements • Developing and managing a household budget • Making decisions about property issues • Recognizing and avoiding scams 10 LO • Putting self first: Dealing with requests from others • Things that have interfered with the need to grieve • Things that have interfered with the need to be Link to model • New roles, identities • Doing new things • Attending to life changes • Intrusion of grief • Avoidance of restoration independent 11 RO Household and Vehicle Responsibilities • Breaking household duties into small, manageable steps • Following regular maintenance schedules for home and car • Identifying and remedying household hazards • Who to contact for help, and How to avoid being defrauded 12 RO 14 RO LORO changes • Doing new things • Distraction from grief Nutrition for One • Shopping for one and understanding labels • Preparing one- or two-step meals • Freezing portions for later use • Sharing recipes • Finding nutritional assistance in the community 13 • Attending to life • Attending to life changes • Doing new things • Distraction from grief Remaining Socially Connected • Functioning “comfortably” as a single person • Inexpensive and accessible entertainment or • Attending to life leisure activities • Engaging in non-threatening socialization experiences • Finding volunteer opportunities • New roles, identities • New relationships • Distraction from changes grief • Rediscovering sources of joy, fulfillment, and growth; • All of the above renewing old interests and exploring new ones • “A time to mourn—A time to dance”; taking the time to grieve and the time away from grieving • Having realistic expectations • Where to learn more LO = Loss-Oriented RO = Restoration-Oriented elements 298 / LUND ET AL. also had to reside in one of the two study areas (San Francisco or Salt Lake City) for the duration of the group intervention. Potential participants were identified from death certificate data filed with local and federal health agencies. Names and addresses of the surviving spouses or partners, as well as information on the decedent, were included on the death records. Potential participants received an invitation letter roughly 3 months prior to the commencement of the groups. Then, approximately 5-7 days after the letters were mailed, a trained research assistant contacted them by telephone. The purpose of the call was to verify that they received the letter, that they met the study criteria, to address any questions about the study, and to solicit their preliminary agreement to participate. Those who were ineligible or not interested in participating received a list of available community-based resources related to bereavement. If all the eligibility criteria were met and the potential participant agreed, the RA made an appointment to conduct a home visit, in which informed consent was obtained and the baseline (O1) questionnaire was left for the participant to complete. Participants were randomly assigned in equivalent proportions to one of the two group interventions (DPM or Comparison). All participants, regardless of their assigned study condition, completed selfadministered questionnaires largely consisting of background information and a variety of measures commonly used in bereavement research: Baseline (O1) data were collected prior to the intervention at 2-6 months of bereavement. Post-test (O2) occurred at the conclusion of the 14-week intervention period (at 5 to 9 months of bereavement) with the O3 assessment following 3 months later. The final data point (O4) was 6 months following O3, equivalent to 14-18 months after the loss. Participants received $25 per questionnaire, for a total of $100 if they completed all four waves of data collection. The average participant completed the O1 baseline questionnaire approximately 4 months (15.7 weeks) after the spouse’s death, with some completing it as early as 5 weeks post-loss and some as late as 24 weeks post-loss. The group intervention commenced approximately 4.7 months post-loss, with a range of 2 to 7.5 months bereaved. SAMPLE A total of 328 widowed persons completed O1 questionnaires, but the analytic sample for this report includes the 298 participants who completed both the O1 (pre-test) and O2 (first post-intervention assessment). The most common reasons for attrition between O1 and O2 included: “I decided that the group was not for me,” “My grief is too strong and I am unable to participate in group,” or the participant decided that they “no longer wanted to participate in LAL” without specifying a reason. One participant died after he completed the baseline questionnaire, but before the groups started. Two participants experienced significant health declines just prior to the start of the group intervention, and a few others had to withdraw in order to take care of unexpected family emergencies. EXPERIENCES IN INTERVENTION BASED ON THE DPM / 299 Finally, the group leaders discovered that a few of the enrolled participants were not suitable for the group intervention due to cognitive impairment or significant hearing loss; they were asked to withdraw from the study. Our analytic sample includes 61% women (n = 183) and 39% men (n = 115). The average age of our sample was 69.5 years (SD = 10.6), with a range of 50 to 93 years. Participants had been married or partnered for an average of 40.1 years (SD = 17.0). Our sample was quite educated: only 14% of the sample had a high school education or less; 41% had some college; and 45% had graduated from a college. The majority were Caucasian (87%), with 6% Asian, 4% African American, 2% Latino, and 1% other. Also, 10 participants (or 3% of the sample) had been in a same-sex union. A little more than half (58%) said they expected the spouse’s death. Regarding financial status, 69% reported that they were “comfortable,” 17% said “more than adequate,” and 14% said that it was “not very good.” A little more than half of the participants (n = 170 or 57%) were assigned to the DPM condition, while 128 (43%) were assigned to the LO condition. The number of weekly intervention sessions attended was the same for those in the LO and DPM conditions, with an average of 11.03 sessions (SD = 3.3). MEASURES DPM coping processes were measured by the Inventory of Daily Widowed Life (IDWL; Caserta & Lund, 2007), consisting of 22 Likert-format items that inquire into how much time during the past week the respondents spent on loss-oriented activities (e.g., “Thinking about how much I miss my spouse;” “Feeling a bond with my spouse”) and restoration-oriented activities (e.g., “Finding ways to keep busy or occupied;” “Took some time away from grieving for my spouse”). Eleven loss-orientation items and 11 restoration-orientation items were identified largely from Stroebe and Schut’s (1999) description of the types of instances that would fall into each category. Construct validity has been established for both subscales (Caserta & Lund, 2007). LO and RO subscale scores were calculated by summing the identified items (on scales from 1 “rarely or not at all” to 4 “almost always,” each subscale ranging from 11 to 44), with lower scores indicating less LO or RO coping and higher scores indicating more LO or RO coping. Both subscales range from 11 to 44 and have high internal consistency (LO subscale, alpha = .91; RO subscale, alpha = .73). Oscillation balance or the degree to which the participants engaged in equal amounts of both processes was calculated by subtracting their total LO score from their total RO score (RO minus LO). Hence, the balance score can range from –33 (exclusively loss-orientation focus) to +33 (exclusively focused on restoration-orientation). A score equal to 0 indicates perfect balance between the two processes. In addition to examining the extent to which the participants were engaging in loss- and restoration-oriented coping processes, we also were interested in 300 / LUND ET AL. knowing to what extent they were consciously aware of where they were focusing their attention. Consequently, the participants were requested to respond to two additional items that asked, “During the past week, to what extent have you focused your attention on issues related to grief, emotions, and feelings regarding your loss?” to measure if they were aware of their engagement in loss-orientation, and to what extent they focused on “new responsibilities, activities, and time away from grieving” as a way to measure awareness of their own restorationoriented coping. Both items were measured on a scale from 1-5 with 1 indicating very little and 5 a great deal. Participants’ Assessment of Group Sessions At the O2 measurement, the participants completed a series of items that inquired into how well they believe they learned or understood each of the topics covered in the group meetings (ranging from 1 [not at all well] to 5 [very well]) and to what extent they were applying what they learned in their daily lives (1 = not at all; 5 = almost always). Each of the items corresponded to the content depicted in Table 1. There were 11 items related to loss-oriented aspects of coping with grief and loss that were completed by participants in both study conditions. Those in the DPM treatment group completed 11 additional items that represented the content addressed in the seven RO sessions. The participants also completed a checklist to indicate if they sought additional information outside the group meetings on any of the topics that were covered in their weekly sessions as well as an open-ended item where they were given the opportunity to identify any topics they wish were addressed but were not. We also compared the participants in both groups on items related to their motivation to attend group meetings (ranging from 1 = very little to 5 = very much), the extent to which their needs were met by their participation (ranging from 1 = not at all to 5 = very well), and whether or not they were glad they had participated (yes or no). RESULTS Participants’ Assessment of Their Group Experiences and Content Exposure Our first interest was in identifying how well participants learned or understood and were applying in their daily lives those intervention topics related to grief (LO sessions). We also compared the responses of participants in both study conditions. There were no statistically significant differences between DPM and comparison participants on their assessments of grief-related topics. The top five grief topics (based on 1-5 ratings) for “how much was learned” for both the DPM and LO participants were in the same rank order and with mean scores ranging from 4.0 to 4.5. They were: EXPERIENCES IN INTERVENTION BASED ON THE DPM / 1. 2. 3. 4. 5. 301 Experience of grief is unique to each person; Understanding the need to grieve; How grief affects everyday functioning; Understanding what grief is; and Expectations about how and when to grieve. With respect to how much they were applying what they learned in their daily lives, participants in both study conditions had the same top five answers (based on 1-5 ratings) and nearly in the same order with the highest ones being “Experiences of grief is unique,” “Understanding the need to grieve,” “Using humor as a way to cope with loss,” “How grief affects everyday functioning,” and “Dealing with challenges assuming new responsibilities.” The mean scores on these items ranged from 3.8 (new responsibilities) to 4.2 (grief is unique). Furthermore, no statistically significant differences were found between those in the two study conditions regarding their motivation to attend meetings or the percentages reporting they were glad they participated. Most participants had very favorable ratings of the program. Regarding motivation for attending, both groups had mean scores of 4.2 (1-5 scales). Ninety-nine percent of those in the comparison condition and 95% in the DPM condition said they were glad they participated. In short, there were far more similarities between those in the DPM and comparison group condition than there were differences and most participants, regardless of group condition, were very favorable about what they had learned and were applying, their motivation levels to attend meetings, and being glad they had participated. For those in the DPM condition, the top five restoration-oriented skills that they learned and applied in their daily lives were the same. The highest rated RO topic was “Taking responsibility for health needs” followed by “Maintaining clean and safe home,” “Keeping home and auto in good repair,” “Avoiding scams and fraud,” and “Managing finances and budget.” Participants gave average ratings ranging from 4.1 to 4.2 on all five of these topics. Two other skill topics with high scores for “what was learned” included “Nutrition for one” and “Insurance and legal matters.” There were eight RO skill topics for which notable proportions of DPM participants indicated they sought additional information beyond what they learned in the group sessions. These were “Insurance and legal matters” (35%), “Taking responsibility for health needs” (29%), “Setting personal goals” (29%), “Managing finances and budget” (27%), “Trying new things” (27%), “Nutrition for one” (24%), “Leisure, social and volunteer activities” (24%), and “Keeping home and auto in good repair” (23%). One interpretation of these data is that the RO sessions stimulated the interest of many participants in these topics as well as many persons recognized the need for them to learn more than what could be covered during the sessions. The participants also were asked, “Are there any topics you wish were covered but were not?” Very few comparison group participants indicated whether there 302 / LUND ET AL. was anything about which they would have liked to learn more but those in DPM groups gave many suggestions, including learning about new relationships, sprinkling systems, hobbies, driving, computers, cooking, books, making television recordings, working cameras, art, and spiritual issues. These responses might suggest that having participated in RO types of group sessions during the intervention stimulated their thinking about other topics and skills of interest which would be a positive outcome according to the Dual Process Model. Participants’ Engagement in LO and RO Coping Process Our next interest was to examine the extent to which the DPM intervention was enhancing LO and RO coping processes by comparing the participants in the DPM condition with those in the comparison group. Recall that we expected those in both study groups to be similar to each other regarding LO coping from O1 (prior to the intervention) to O2 (assessed within 2-4 weeks after the intervention) because all participants received intervention content devoted to LO coping. However, we expected those in the DPM condition to show greater gains in RO coping because the comparison group focused on loss-oriented issues without any deliberate attempt to foster restoration-orientation. Figure 1 shows the mean scores for both study groups at O1 and O2 using the IDWL subscales described earlier. The mean scores for both groups show a statistically significant decline in LO coping from O1 to O2 (p < .001) which is consistent with the Dual Process Model in that LO coping gradually subsides while RO coping tends to increase over time. The decline in LO coping, however, was independent of study condition as indicated by the failure of the group by time interaction to attain statistical significance. This could be interpreted as a positive outcome of the DPM intervention in that similar decrements were noted regarding LO coping even though DPM participants received overall less attention in their intervention to LO coping because they had fewer sessions devoted to those issues. Figure 2 shows the mean scores for the two study conditions regarding O1 to O2 changes in RO coping. Those who were in the DPM condition were expected to show greater gains in RO coping than the comparison group condition because they had half of their intervention sessions devoted to restoration-oriented issues. The data show that while those in the DPM condition did show greater gains in RO coping, those in the comparison group showed some gains as well. Therefore, both groups changed over time (p < .05) but there was no statistically significant group by time effect. The overall decline in the use of LO coping and increase use of RO coping among the participants is further reflected in the change in oscillation balance scores from O1 to O2, illustrated in Figure 3. Participants in both groups were fairly balanced between both processes at baseline but moved to a greater focus on restoration-oriented coping by the time the weekly sessions ended (p < .001). EXPERIENCES IN INTERVENTION BASED ON THE DPM / 303 Figure 1. Pre- and post-intervention measures of loss-oriented coping by study group. Loss-oriented coping is measured with an 11-item scale (range 11 to 44). Lower values indicate less loss-oriented coping. Although mean balance scores at O2 were further away from 0 than they were at the baseline assessment, the score values were of a magnitude that indicates that loss-oriented coping was still occurring at O2 for participants in both study conditions. At the same time, a trend appeared to be emerging (although not statistically significant) in which those in the DPM condition were beginning to invest more effort into RO (versus LO) coping, than the comparison group. Figure 4 shows that those in both group conditions had similar LO coping awareness scores at O1, and consistent with the model were consciously aware that they were devoting less attention to loss-oriented issues by the O2 assessment. The significant group by time interaction (p < .05) however, indicates that this awareness was greater among those in the DPM treatment group. Alternatively and somewhat unexpectedly, we found that participants in both conditions were showing greater awareness of RO coping from O1 to O2. Although there is a statistically significant overall increase in RO awareness (p < .001), both groups changed in a parallel fashion (see Figure 5). It may be possible that even those in the comparison condition were learning new skills and engaging in RO coping without having intervention sessions specifically devoted to these issues. Indeed, 304 / LUND ET AL. Figure 2. Pre- and post-intervention measures of restoration-oriented coping by study group. Restoration-oriented coping is measured with an 11-item scale (range 11 to 44). Lower values indicate less restoration-oriented coping. there were informal suggestions from participants, and the research assistants who attended sessions reported that some participants encouraged each other to seek assistance with restoration-oriented activities. While we expected to find stronger and more positive effects for the DPM intervention, even with using only the O1 and O2 data, the open-ended questions in the self-administered questionnaires did offer some further explanations and insights. We wanted to obtain information regarding any criticisms that the DPM participants might have had so we asked them if they had any suggestions for how we could improve on the intervention. The following quotes provide some potential reasons for why we find only modest support in favor of the DPM condition. “Some sessions of little or no value—could just give us written material,” “Guest speakers not among my needs or interests,” “Did not enjoy guest speakers, maybe use only half of the meeting time,” “Guest speakers weren’t helpful or interesting,” “Some presentations/speakers did not have enough time,” “Need a doctor as a speaker,” “Some speakers communicated as though we were totally inexperienced—wrong assumptions,” “Least effective EXPERIENCES IN INTERVENTION BASED ON THE DPM / 305 Figure 3. Pre- and post-intervention measures of oscillation balance by study group. Oscillation balance is measured by subtracting LO from RO with responses ranging from –33 to +33, zero equals perfect balance. were guest speakers,” “Speakers did not have enough time—rushed and couldn’t ask questions,” “Instructors were poor use of time,” “Needed better tips on house cleaning,” “Needed more info on low cost medical insurance,” “More help with financial matters, but everyone’s situation is different,” “Needed more guest speakers,” “Less speakers,” “Ask group for other specific topics,” “More knowledgeable speakers on autos and home services.” In short, these comments suggest that providing RO coping in group situations may not be the ideal way to provide what is most needed. Some participants saw the RO sessions as disruptions to the attention to loss-oriented issues from the previous week, others were not satisfied because their individual RO needs were not met in terms of the topics selected, the levels of knowledge, and/or the limited time available. CONCLUSION AND DISCUSSION There are three primary conclusions of this investigation. First, regarding nearly all measures of motivation and satisfaction with the intervention processes, 306 / LUND ET AL. Figure 4. Pre- and post-intervention measures of awareness of loss-oriented coping by study group. Awareness is measured with a single-item question (In the past week, to what extent have you focused your attention on issues related to grief, emotions, and feelings regarding your loss?), with responses ranging from\ 1 very little to 5 a great deal. individuals in both conditions were highly favorable about their participation. These participants were quite pleased with having their needs met and with what they were learning and applying to their daily lives, and were feeling glad they had participated. Although this provides satisfaction for us in the design and implementation of the intervention, it may also signal the natural benefits of group interventions and the related opportunities to speak and hear from others about the loss of a spouse or life partner. Second, although participant satisfaction was high, we identified only modest positive support for the DPM intervention versus the traditional bereavement support format represented by the comparison group condition. Those in both study conditions were engaging in similar LO coping even though the DPM group had fewer sessions devoted to it. However, although engagement in LO coping was similar for both groups, those in the DPM condition more consciously perceived themselves as engaging in less LO coping by the time the weekly sessions ended compared to those in the comparison group. Also, it appears that EXPERIENCES IN INTERVENTION BASED ON THE DPM / 307 Figure 5. Pre- and post-intervention measures of awareness of restoration-oriented coping by study group. Awareness is measured with a single-item question (In the past week, to what extent have you focused your attention on issues related to new responsibilities, activities, and time away from grieving?), with responses ranging from 1 very little to 5 a great deal. the comparison group participants were engaging in some RO coping even though they were not receiving an intervention targeted toward those processes. It is possible they could have learned new skills, for instance, from other sources like friends, family members, or elsewhere in the community, perhaps out of necessity (Lund et al., 1989; Utz, 2006). In a few instances as well, our group leaders occasionally reported to us that restoration-oriented issues were unintentionally raised at times by comparison group participants in the course of the weekly sessions. This potentially initiated some group discussion related to any issue that was raised as well as attention to restoration-oriented activity on the part of some outside the context of the group. Consequently, people in both group conditions were improving with respect to LO and RO coping from O1 to O2. This could suggest that nearly all bereaved persons might eventually deal with restoration-oriented stressors and engage in some restoration-oriented coping. The above conclusion is particularly interesting in light of how the sessions were sequenced within the DPM treatment group. Focusing the early sessions on 308 / LUND ET AL. loss-orientation and the latter ones on restoration-orientation was intended to approximate the intent of the dual process model (Stroebe & Schut, 1999). We now recognize, however, that this a potentially artificial approximation as it appears that RO does tend to occur to some extent early on. Furthermore, our recent work with the IDWL suggested that in some instances an “unbalanced” oscillation favoring greater restoration-orientation, independent of how long one was bereaved, was associated with better outcomes. A greater emphasis on RO was not equivalent with avoidance in that grief, depression and loneliness levels tended to be lower when this occurred (Caserta & Lund, 2007). Consequently, we argue that how much emphasis on both LO and RO processes is optimal throughout the bereavement trajectory still needs to be investigated and developed further. While we hope to begin to address this as we examine the long-term impact of the DPM treatment on outcomes, we believe that future intervention designs, as discussed below, should consider tailoring the sequencing and content of bereavement interventions to be more responsive to the unique situations and needs of each participant. This brings us to our third primary conclusion. While additional data and analyses are certainly needed in order to provide a more comprehensive test of the DPM intervention in terms of bereavement adjustment outcomes over time, it is clear at this point that at least some of the participants might benefit more from an individually targeted and delivered RO coping intervention. This comment is particularly supported by the openended data revealing the range and type of topics DPM participants identified as wanting to be covered. There is sufficient evidence from the qualitative reports included in this article as well as from comments made from our group leaders during our project meetings when the intervention was being delivered that there were problems and limitations due to providing an RO intervention in group settings. We have learned that, for some participants, they felt that the RO sessions interrupted the continuity or flow from one week’s meeting to the next. For example, when one meeting ended with a discussion of someone’s specific problem, the group was unable to continue that discussion the next week because an RO expert was scheduled to talk about an already identified RO coping topic. Also, it is clear that for some participants, the RO group sessions did not always address their most pressing needs, or specific sessions were not targeted at their specific level of knowledge or skill, or the sessions were too brief. Even preferences for speaker styles were mentioned as problematic for some participants. Our decisions about which topics to include were based on previous studies (Caserta et al., 2004; Lund et al., 1989), but, our present intervention involves different participants with differences in specific needs. While we remain confident that the DPM as a conceptual framework holds considerable promise for future research, education, and practice, we need additional data to refine and modify the ways in which we make use of the model. EXPERIENCES IN INTERVENTION BASED ON THE DPM / 309 It may be likely that the optimal way to deliver the RO feature is to do so with a targeted, tailored, and individually delivered format so that each bereaved person identifies the RO skills that are most needed, selects the format in which they learn the new skills, and in a way that fits their schedule and desired outcome with the least interference. Other bereavement researchers, educators, and clinicians have suggested more individualized interventions (Breen & O’Connor, 2007; Hooyman & Kramer, 2006; Hughes, 1995; Richardson, 2007; Utz, 2006; Worden, 2002). We realize that grief support groups offer valuable opportunities to bereaved persons who like that type of format but it should be possible to provide RO interventions simultaneously to these groups and do so with more individualized formats and with more flexible sequencing. With the addition of O3 and O4 data we will not only gain an improved understanding of how the intervention may have enhanced LO and RO coping processes, we will be able to assess the extent to which the expected changes in bereavement adjustment outcomes were achieved. Also, oscillation between the two coping processes is obviously the least well developed feature of the DPM but has considerable promise in facilitating more positive adjustment outcomes. We suggest that those who are prepared to, more aware of, and able to control their oscillation will be more adaptable to meet the changing needs and demands of the complex and long-term nature of bereavement. Future interventions should be targeted toward enhancing oscillation as a coping strategy. We also remain excited about using theoretically-based interventions to help those who are in the greatest need and have the most desire to receive assistance, knowing that not everyone will require sophisticated and expert interventions. We have learned that many bereaved persons are quite resilient and find ways to manage many difficult life transitions (Arbuckle & de Vries, 1995; Caserta et al., 2009; Carr, 2004; Richardson, 2007; Schaefer & Moos, 2001). ACKNOWLEDGMENTS The authors wish to acknowledge the helpful contributions of three external consultants; Drs. Bert Hayslip, University of North Texas; Judith Hays, Duke University; and Marilyn Skaff, University of California, San Francisco. REFERENCES Anderson, K. L., & Dimond, M. F. (1995). The experience of bereavement in older adults. Journal of Advanced Nursing, 22, 308-315. Arbuckle, N. W., & de Vries, B. (1995). The long-term effects of later life spousal and parental bereavement on personal functioning. The Gerontologist, 35(5), 637-647. 310 / LUND ET AL. Bennett, K. M., Hughes, G. M., & Smith, P. T. (2005). Psychological response to later life widowhood: Coping and the effects of gender. Omega: Journal of Death & Dying, 51(1), 33-52. Bisconti, T. L., Bergeman, C. S., & Boker, S. M. (2004). Emotional well-being in recently bereaved widows: A dynamical systems approach. Journals of Gerontology Series B: Psychological Sciences & Social Sciences, 59B(4), P158-P167. Boerner, K., Wortman, C. B., & Bonanno, G. A. (2005). Resilient or at risk? A 4-year study of older adults who initially showed high or low distress following conjugal loss. Journals of Gerontology Series B: Psychological Sciences & Social Sciences, 60B(2), P67-73. Bonanno, G. A. (2004). Loss, trauma, and human resilience: Have we underestimated the human capacity to thrive after extremely aversive events? American Psychologist, 59(1), 20-28. Breen, L. J., & O’Connor, M. (2007). The fundamental paradox in the grief literature: A critical reflection. Omega: Journal of Death and Dying, 55(3), 199-218. Calhoun, L. G., & Tedeschi, R. G. (2006). The foundations of posttraumatic growth: An expanded framework. In L. G. Calhoun & R. G. Tedeschi (Eds.), Handbook of posttraumatic growth: Research and practice. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. Carr, D. (2004). Gender, preloss marital dependence, and older adults’ adjustment to widowhood. Journal of Marriage and the Family, 66, 220-235. Carr, D., House, J. S., Kessler, R. C., Nesse, R., Sonnega, J., & Wortman, C. B. (2000). Marital quality and psychological adjustment to widowhood among older adults: A longitudinal analysis. Journal of Gerontology: Social Sciences, 55B(4), S197-S207. Carr, D., Nesse, R. M., & Wortman, C. B. (Eds.). (2006). Spousal bereavement in late life. New York: Springer. Caserta, M. S. (2003). Widowers. In R. Kastenbaum (Ed.), Macmillan encyclopedia of death and dying (pp. 933-938). New York: Macmillan Reference USA. Caserta, M. S., & Lund, D. A. (2007). Toward the development of an Inventory of Daily Widowed Life (IDWL): Guided by the dual process model of coping with bereavement. Death Studies, 31(6), 505-534. Caserta, M. S., Lund, D. A., & Obray, S. J. (2004). Promoting self-care and daily living skills among older widows and widowers: Evidence from the Pathfinders demonstration project. Omega: Journal of Death & Dying, 49, 217-236. Caserta, M. S., Lund, D. A., & Rice, S. J. (1999). Pathfinders: A self-care and health education program for older widows and widowers. The Gerontologist, 39, 615-620. Caserta, M. S., Lund, D. A., Utz, R., & deVries, B. (2009). Stress-related growth among the recently bereaved, Aging & Mental Health, 13(3), 463-467. Chen, J. H., Gill, T. M., & Prigerson, H. G. (2005). Health behaviors associated with better quality of life for older bereaved persons. Journal of Palliative Medicine, 8(1), 96-106. Doka, K. A., & Martin, T. (2001). Take it like a man: Masculine response to loss. In Men coping with grief (pp. 37-48). Amityville, NY: Baywood. Dutton, Y. C., & Zisook, S. (2005). Adaptation to bereavement. Death Studies, 29(10), 877-903. EXPERIENCES IN INTERVENTION BASED ON THE DPM / 311 Hansson, R. O., & Stroebe, M. S. (2007). Old age and widowhood: Issues of personal control and independence. Washington, DC: American Psychological Association. Hooyman, N. R., & Kramer, B. J. (2006). Living through loss: Interventions across the lifespan. New York: Columbia University Press. Hughes, M. (1995). Bereavement and support: Healing in a group environment. Washington, DC: Taylor & Francis. Johnson, C. S. (2002). Nutritional considerations for bereavement and coping with grief. Journal of Nutrition, Health and Aging, 6, 171-176. Lazarus, R. S., & Folkman, S. (1984). Stress, appraisal, and coping. New York: Springer. Lee, M. A., & Carr, D. (2007). Does the context of spousal loss affect the physical functioning of older widowed persons? Research on Aging, 29(5), 457-487. Lund, D. A. (1989). Older bereaved spouses: Research with practical applications. Washington, DC: Taylor-Francis/Hemisphere Press. Lund, D. A. (1999). Giving and receiving help during later life spousal bereavement. In Living with grief at work, at school, at worship (pp. 203-212). Levittown, PA: Brunner/Mazel. Lund, D., Utz, R., Caserta, M., & de Vries, B. (2008). Humor, laughter & happiness in the daily lives of recently bereaved spouses. Omega: Journal of Death & Dying, 58(2), 87-105. Lund, D. A., Caserta, M., de Vries, B., & Wright, S. (2004). Restoration during bereavement. Generations Review, 14, 9-15. Lund, D. A., & Caserta, M. S. (1998). Future directions in adult bereavement research. Omega: Journal of Death and Dying, 36, 287-303. Lund, D. A., & Caserta, M. S. (2002). Facing life alone: The loss of a significant other in later life. In K. Doka (Ed.), Living with grief: Loss in later life. Levittown, PA: Brunner/Mazel. Lund, D. A., Caserta, M. S., Dimond, M. F., & Shaffer, S. K. (1989). Competencies, tasks of daily living and adjustments to spousal bereavement in later life. In Older bereaved spouses: Research with practical applications (pp. 135-156). Washington, DC: Taylor-Francis/Hemisphere Press. Montpetit, M. A., Bergeman, C. S., Bisconti, T. L., & Rausch, J. R. (2006). Adaptive change in self-concept and well-being during conjugal loss in later life. International Journal of Aging & Human Development, 63(3), 217-239. Moss, M. S., Moss, S. Z., & Hansson, R. O. (2001). Bereavement and old age. In Handbook of bereavement research: Consequences, coping, and care (pp. 241-260). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association. O’Rourke, N. (2004). Psychological resilience and the well-being of widowed women. Ageing International, 29(3), 267-280. Ong, A. D., & Bergeman, C. S. (2004). Resilience and adaptation to stress in later life: Empirical perspectives and conceptual implications. Ageing International, 29(3), 219-246. Ong, A. D., Bergeman, C. S., & Bisconti, T. L. (2004). The role of daily positive emotions during conjugal bereavement. Journal of Gerontology: Psychological Sciences, 59B, P168- P176. Pienta, A. M., & Franks, M. M. (2006). A closer look at health and widowhood: Do health behaviors change after the loss of a spouse? In D. Carr, R. M. Neese, & C. B. Wortman (Eds.), Spousal bereavement in late life (pp. 117-142). New York: Springer. 312 / LUND ET AL. Powers, L. E., & Wampold, B. E. (1994). Cognitive-behavioral factors in adjustment to adult bereavement. Death Studies, 18, 1-24. Quandt, S. A., McDonald, J., Arcury, T. A., Bell, R. A., & Vitolins, M. Z. (2000). Nutritional self-management of elderly widows in rural communities. Gerontologist, 40, 86-96. Richardson, V. E. (2007). A dual process model of grief counseling: Findings from the changing lives of older couples (CLOC) study. Journal of Gerontological Social Work, 48, 311-329. Richardson, V. E., & Balaswamy, S. (2001). Coping with bereavement among elderly widowers. Omega, 43, 129-144. Rosenbloom, C. A., & Whittington, F. J. (1993). The effects of bereavement on eating behaviors and nutrient intakes in elderly widowed persons. Journal of Gerontology: Social Sciences, 48, S223-S229. Schaefer, J. A., & Moos, R. H. (2001). Bereavement experiences and personal growth. In Handbook of bereavement research: Consequences, coping and care (pp. 145-167). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association. Schone, B. S., & Weinick, R. M. (1998). Health-related behaviors and the benefits of marriage for elderly persons. Gerontologist, 38, 618-627. Shahar, D. R., Schultz, R., Shahar, A., & Wing, R. (2001). The effect of widowhood on weight change, dietary intake, and eating behavior in the elderly population. Journal of Aging and Health, 13, 186-199. Stroebe, M. S., & Schut, H. (2010). Update on DPM. Omega, 61(4), 275-291. Stroebe, M. S., & Schut, H. (1999). The dual process model of coping with bereavement: Rationale and description. Death Studies, 23, 197-224. Stroebe, M. S., Stroebe, W., & Hansson, R. O. (1993). Handbook of bereavement: Theory, research, and intervention. New York: Cambridge University Press. Stroebe, M., Stroebe, W., & Schut, H. (2003). Bereavement research: Methodological issues and ethical concerns. Palliative Medicine, 17, 235-240. Utz, R. (2006). Economic and practical adjustments to late life spousal loss. In D. Carr, R. M. Nesse, & C. B. Wortman (Eds.), Spousal bereavement in late life (pp. 167-192). New York: Springer. Utz, R. L., Carr, D., Nesse, R., & Wortman, C. (2002). The effect of widowhood on older adults’ social participation: An evaluation of activity, disengagement, and continuity theories. The Gerontologist, 42(4), 522-533. Utz, R. L., Reidy, E., Carr, D., Nesse, R., & Wortman, C. B. (2004). The daily consequences of widowhood: The role of gender and intergenerational transfers on subsequent housework performance. Journal of Family Issues, 25, 683-712. Wells, Y. D., & Kendig, H. L. (1997). Health and well-being of spouse caregivers and the widowed. The Gerontologist, 37(5), 666-674. Wilcox, S., Evenson, K. R., Aragaki, A., Wassertheil-Smoller, S., Mouton, C. P., & Loevinger, B. L. (2003). The effects of widowhood on physical and mental health, health behaviors, and health outcomes: The women’s health initiative. Health Psychology, 22(5), 1-10. Williams, K. (2004). The transition to widowhood and the social regulation of health: Consequences for health and health risk behavior. Journals of Gerontology Series B: Psychological Sciences & Social Sciences, 59B(6), S343-S349. EXPERIENCES IN INTERVENTION BASED ON THE DPM / 313 Worden, J. W. (2002). Grief counseling and grief therapy: A handbook for the mental health practitioner (3rd ed.). New York: Springer. Znoj, H. (2006). Bereavement and posttraumatic growth. In L. G. Calhoun & R. G. Tedeschi (Eds.), Handbook of posttraumatic growth: Research and practice (pp. 176-196). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. Direct reprint requests to: Dr. Dale Lund Department of Sociology California State University San Bernardino 5500 University Parkway San Bernardino, CA 92407 e-mail: dlund@csusb.edu OMEGA, Vol. 61(4) 315-332, 2010 LOSS AND RESTORATION IN LATER LIFE: AN EXAMINATION OF DUAL PROCESS MODEL OF COPING WITH BEREAVEMENT* KATE M. BENNETT KERRY GIBBONS SUZANNA MACKENZIE-SMITH University of Liverpool, United Kingdom ABSTRACT The Dual Process Model (DPM) of Coping with Bereavement identified two oscillating coping processes, loss and restoration (Stroebe & Schut, 1999). The utility of the model is investigated in two studies. In the first, we carried out secondary analyses on a large-scale qualitative study that we had conducted previously. In the second, we conducted a small-scale study specifically examining the DPM. In the first study we re-examined the interviews for Loss- (LO) and Restoration-Oriented (RO) Coping and examined whether these were associated with psychological adjustment. The results showed that those adjusting well reported the stressors New Roles/Identities/ Relationships and Intrusion of Grief significantly more. Those adjusting less well reported the stressors Denial/Avoidance of Restoration Changes and Distraction/Avoidance of Grief significantly more. In the second study, we asked participants about four RO stressors of the DPM: Attending to Life *Study 1 was supported by the Economic and Social Research Council (Award No. L480254034) and is part of the Growing Older Programme of 24 projects studying the quality of life of older people. However, the findings reported here are entirely the responsibility of the researcher. 315 Ó 2010, Baywood Publishing Co., Inc. doi: 10.2190/OM.61.4.d http://baywood.com 316 / BENNETT, GIBBONS AND MACKENZIE-SMITH Changes; New Roles/Identities/Relationships; Distraction from Grief; and New Activities. These data showed that not all participants experienced all aspects of RO Coping. In particular, participants had diverse views about the utility of Distraction from Grief as a coping mechanism. The article concludes by discussing the challenges of testing the DPM empirically. Over recent years researchers have debated the efficacy of traditional models of bereavement (Bennett & Bennett, 2000-2001; Footman, 1998; Walter, 1996). At the same time those working in the field of stress have developed models to explain how people cope with life events in general (Carver, Scheier, & Weintraub, 1989; Lazarus & Folkman, 1984; Moos, 1995). Until 1999 these two areas of research proceeded independently even though bereavement is a subset of life events. However, in 1999, Stroebe and Schut published their DPM that aimed to address deficits in both areas. In this issue they describe the model in detail. Relatively few studies have examined the DPM empirically (Richardson & Balaswamy, 2001; Richardson, 2007). Richardson and Balaswamy (2001) examined the model in a sample of older widowed men during the second year of their bereavement. They found that both Loss- (LO) and Restoration-Oriented (RO) stressors were important. The former were more important nearer the time of bereavement and influenced negative affect. On the other hand, RO stressors were more relevant later and were often associated with positive affect. In addition, the circumstances of the death were also significant in adjustment, an aspect not accounted for by the DPM. More recently, Richardson (2007) studied older widows and widowers over a longer timeframe. She confirmed that both LO and RO coping were important throughout bereavement. She found a negative relationship between rumination and affect and argued that it was important to help widowed people distinguish between constructive and destructive grief work. Finally, she suggested that widowed people needed to balance out LO and RO activities; a focus on one over and above the other might be problematic for recovery. An important question which researchers have so far been unable to address is whether there is an optimal balance between LO and RO coping, and if so, does that balance change over time? A second question, that researchers do not yet know the answer to, is whether there are optimal degrees of LO or RO coping at different points during the course of bereavement. Lund, Caserta, de Vries, and Wright (2004) have also considered the DPM therapeutically. They developed a 14-week intervention tailored around LO and RO coping. Their proposed sessions included discussions about the physical, cognitive, and behavioral aspects of grief through to the management of household, motorcar, and nutritional responsibilities. The former discussions focus on LO, while the latter focus on RO. In 2004, they had been awarded funds to test this intervention but as yet there has been no published account of the LOSS AND RESTORATION IN LATER LIFE / 317 empirical results. These are eagerly anticipated and an update is presented in this issue. In addition, using the DPM as a theoretical basis Caserta and Lund (2007) developed the Inventory of Daily Widowed Life, which had both LO and RO subscales. They found that those more recently widowed experienced greater oscillation, while those bereaved for longer experienced more RO coping than LO coping. In 2005, Shear, Frank, Houck, and Reynolds also used the DMP as the basis for their randomized control trial of treatment for complicated grief. In their complicated grief treatment not only did they focus on grief but they also focused on personal life goals, therefore addressing both LO and RO coping. Their results showed that this approach was more effective than an interpersonal psychotherapy intervention (see also this issue). Recently, Stroebe, Folkman, Hansson, and Schut (2006) have proposed a framework which integrates both the DPM and Cognitive Stress Theory (Lazarus & Folkman, 1984). Their aim is to enhance the understanding of risk factors in adaptation to bereavement. They suggest that using this framework may enable clinicians to focus on those people who are at risk of serious reactions to bereavement and recognize that for most people bereavement does not require intervention. They also hope that this approach may be of benefit to theoreticians in enabling them to test their hypotheses more effectively. As yet this approach has not been tested empirically. In two earlier papers we established that it was possible to distinguish between those participants who demonstrated good psychological adjustment (the term coping well was used in the original paper) and those who did not (Bennett, Hughes, & Smith, 2005a, 2005b). In our 2005a paper we found that those who were less well adjusted had more depressive feelings than those who were more well adjusted. In the 2005b paper we showed that talking to one’s dead spouse was associated with good adjustment, while keeping oneself to oneself was associated with poor adjustment. In addition, we also found that there were three behaviors that showed an adjustment and gender interaction. Men were more likely to adjust better if they described themselves as “selfish” and as “upset,” women were more likely to adjust better if they described themselves as being comfortable “alone.” Here we present two studies. The first re-examines this earlier data in the light of the DPM. Codes that reflect the components of LO and RO coping are examined to see whether they are associated with psychological adjustment. We would predict, based on earlier work, that some stressors such as Doing New Things and Breaking Bonds/Ties/Relocation are common to most bereaved people. On the other hand, we would predict that those engaged in Distraction from Grief or in Denial/Avoidance of Restoration Changes would be less well adjusted, and those with New Roles/Identities/Relationships would be more likely to be well adjusted. In the second study, we focus only on RO stressors to provide an in-depth understanding of how these are incorporated into the daily lives of older widows. 318 / BENNETT, GIBBONS AND MACKENZIE-SMITH STUDY 1 Method Participants The participants were 46 widowed men (only 45 of whom were interviewed) and 46 women. In the analyses presented here, one woman was excluded since she had been widowed for 60 years. The remaining participants were aged between 55 and 95 years (mean = 74), living in Merseyside, United Kingdom. They had been widowed between 3 months to 32 years (mean 8.68 years). Although, some had been widowed for several years, we decided not to exclude them because our previous work has suggested that the effects of bereavement last for a longer time than has previously been found and even those widowed for a long time may still be making RO adjustments (Bennett, 1997; Davidson, 1999; Moore & Stratton, 2003). Demographic details are summarized in Table 1 and include information concerning the excluded woman for consistency with other papers published from the study. Recruitment The research team communicated the aims of the Older Widowed Men and Women Project to a diverse range of formal and informal groups of older people. We also made contact with other welfare organizations and agencies, social services, and sheltered housing schemes, through which links with widowed people were established. The local ethics committee approved the study and confidentiality and anonymity were assured. Names have been changed to preserve anonymity. Table 1. Demographic Data by Gender of Study 1 Participants Mean Standard deviation Range Women (n = 46) Age Years married Years bereaved 73.29 35.75 10.94 8.93 10.70 1.72 57-95 2-63 1-60 Men (n = 46) Age Years married Years bereaved 75.02 39.37 8.18 7.88 12.97 6.72 55-93 5-63 0.25-25 LOSS AND RESTORATION IN LATER LIFE / 319 The Interview We tape-recorded the semi-structured interviews that lasted from 45 minutes to 1½ hours, conducted in the respondents’ own homes. Respondents gave informed consent. The interview was not prescriptive; the aim was to learn from the widowed people what was important to them. The approach was “I am the novice and you have the experience.” The interview schedule consisted of seven parts. The first part contained factual questions concerning age, length of marriage, widowhood, and family relations, followed by four sections inquiring about the widowed person’s life at various times. The first of these addressed married life before the death of the spouse, asking questions about hobbies, division of labor, and marital quality. The second section asked about the time around the death of the spouse. They were asked to describe what a typical day had been like after the death, what support they had, and how they had felt. They were then asked what they did and how they felt 1 year on. They were asked how their lives had changed by then, what a typical day was like at that stage, whether they were now doing anything new, and in what ways (if any) their feelings had changed. The fifth section asked what their lives were like at the present time. Questions related to what they did with their time, how they felt about their widowhood, how their lives had changed, and what their emotions were. Comment on the Method There are two potential disadvantages with this type of retrospective interview. First, it assumes that widowed people accurately recall the events surrounding and following the deaths of their spouses. Second, recall may be subjective and dependent on the individual experiences of the bereaved, and may indeed differ from the recollections of bereaved children, for example. However, it is difficult to obtain contemporaneous qualitative accounts of these experiences, particularly in relationship to those that occur very close to the death, for both practical and ethical reasons. We have also found that respondents appear to have detailed recollections about some events, such as those leading up to the death, and poor or non-existent recollections of the events immediately after the death—participants report that these events are often a “blur” (Bennett & Vidal-Hall, 2000). It is also the case that it is the participant’s subjective experience that is of interest: there is a great deal of evidence, for example, that subjective rather than objective measures of health are better predictors of mortality (Benyamini, Blumstein, Lusky, & Modan, 2003). The lived experience of the bereaved is important in understanding the DPM, and in particular in the context of its relationship to wellbeing and adjustment. Another potential disadvantage of the method is the variety of length of times since bereavement that are used in the study. As mentioned earlier, other authors have also found that there is great variation in the impact that time since bereavement has on widowed people’s lives. If we were to confine our data collection to those bereaved within, for example 2 years 320 / BENNETT, GIBBONS AND MACKENZIE-SMITH (as many studies do, see Zisook, Paulus, Shuchter, & Judd, 1997), we might miss important bereavement experiences that occur much later than others have suggested (Bennett, 1997; Davidson, 1999; Moore & Stratton, 2003). Analysis Two members of the original research team coded the interviews using grounded theory and content analysis methods (Bennett & Vidal-Hall, 2000; Charmaz, 1995; Glaser & Strauss, 1969; Smith, 1995). Each transcript was first read through in its entirety to gain an impression of the interview. It was then re-read line-by-line and coded. This process was reflexive; as new topics emerged they were looked for in earlier parts of the interview. Examples of coded topics include: guilt, independence, presentation of husband, quality of marriage, and death narrative. The transcripts were further examined for broader themes. A number were common to all the interviews and these included: the domain of death, the social domain, the emotional domain and the domain of time. Brief memos were written for each interview (see also Bennett & Vidal-Hall, 2000). In addition reliability checks were undertaken. The reliability was found to be satisfactory. A total of 311 codes emerged from analysis of the 91 interviews. Reliability was assessed and agreement was found to be 80% between the coders. This article focuses on codes relevant to the DPM (Stroebe & Schut, 1999). An initial trawl of the codes suggested that 56 might be relevant. Careful reading of the texts suggested that 44 described aspects of DPM. These codes were then assigned to the nine individual aspects of LO stressors (Grief Work; Intrusion of Grief; Breaking Bonds/Ties/Relocation; and Denial/Avoidance of Restoration Changes) and RO stressors (Attending to Life Changes; Doing New Things; Distraction from Grief; Denial/Avoidance of Grief; and New Roles/Identities/ Relationships). Finally, the frequency with which each of the nine stressors occurred was calculated. We assessed psychological adjustment by expert reading of the interviews and assessment of non-verbal aspects of the interview. For example, coders looked for reports of medication, contact with primary care, not coping. In addition, the non-verbal content of the interviews was taken into account. If a participant mentioned specifically that they were not coping, they were classified as not adjusting well. They were classified either as psychologically well-adjusted or not (and in previous work as good coper or poor coper). Characteristically, people showing good adjustment had developed a life without their spouse, were not unduly distressed during the interview, were able to discuss the issues surrounding their bereavement and widowhood in positive as well as negative terms, and described the events surrounding their bereavement with a degree of distance (see also Bennett et al., 2005a, 2005b). Two members of the team made the assessment, again by reading the transcripts independently, and agreement was found to be 95%. The interviews where there was not agreement were sent to the third member of the team for final decision. LOSS AND RESTORATION IN LATER LIFE / 321 We used chi-square analysis to test whether particular stressors were more common among those with good or poor adjustment. Results Table 2 shows the frequency table and c2 for LO and RO coping. Only two LO stressors differentiated between good and poor adjustment. Significantly more people with good adjustment reported the stressor Intrusion of Grief than those with poor adjustment (c2 = 6.58, p £ .001) and the reverse was true for Denial/Avoidance of Restoration Changes (c2 = 16.68, p £ .001). In addition, it is worth noting that the majority of respondents reported Breaking Bonds/Ties/ Relocation. On the other hand, relatively few reported Grief Work. There were two RO stressors that differentiated between good and poor adjustment. More people with good adjustment reported New Roles/Identities/Relationships (c2 = 5.42, p £ .02) more often and those with poor adjustment reported the stressor Distraction from Grief more frequently (c2 = 4.05, p £ .04). Many participants reported the stressors Doing New Things and Avoidance/Denial of Grief. We were also interested in seeing whether we could use this data to begin to assess oscillation. As a staring point we began by calculating how many participants experienced both LO and RO coping. We suggest that those people who reported only one type of coping were unlikely to be oscillating. Twelve participants were, therefore, identified as not oscillating. Eleven of those did not experience LO coping, and only one did not experience RO coping. The remaining 79 did experience both RO and LO coping, therefore, could have experienced oscillation (87%). We then calculated whether experiencing oscillation was significantly associated with good psychological adjustment but we did not find a significant association (Fisher’s exact test: p = .67). Our analysis is not as refined as that conducted by Caserta and Lund (2007), and as they point out it is likely that oscillation is complex, multi-dimensional, and dynamic. Nevertheless, these analyses suggest that oscillation, or something resembling it, is common among widowed people. STUDY 2 Method Participants Thirteen widowed women were interviewed in this small-scale study (one widow was excluded because she had been widowed for 25 years). These widows had been widowed from 1.5 to 16 years (mean 7 years) and were aged from 51 to 85 (mean 70 years) (see Table 3). The sample was an opportunity sample recruited both from the NW of England and the South of England. The University of Liverpool’s Research Governance Committee approved study and confidentiality and anonymity were assured. Names have been changed to preserve anonymity. 322 / BENNETT, GIBBONS AND MACKENZIE-SMITH Table 2. Chi-Square Analysis of Coping and Use of Dual Process Model Categories from Study 1 Use of Dual Process Model Categories Loss-Oriented Grief Work Yes No Intrusion Yes No Breaking Bonds/Ties Yes No Denial/Avoidance of Restoration Changes Yes No Restoration-Oriented Attending to Life Changes Yes No New Activities Yes No Distraction from Grief Yes No Denial/Avoidance of Grief Yes No New Roles Yes No c2 p Overall % of participants using strategy 0.65 .42 23% 6.58 .001 59% 0.08 .78 67% 16.68 0.001 27% 1.01 .32 71% .93 80% 4.05 0.04 24% 0.43 .84 63% 5.42 0.02 54% Coping Yes No 17 48 9 17 44 21 10 16 43 22 18 8 10 55 15 11 35 7 30 19 52 13 21 5 12 53 10 16 41 24 17 9 40 25 9 17 .007 The Interview The interviews were conducted by KG and SM-S, who were undergraduate students, for their final year research project. They were both young women aged from 20 to 22 at the time of the interviews. They were trained by KMB who has substantial experience in interviewing widowed women. There was no evidence that the quality or detail of the interviews was significantly different from that of Study 1. LOSS AND RESTORATION IN LATER LIFE / 323 Table 3. Study 2 Demographic Detailsa Mean Standard deviation Range Age 71.1 9.75 51-85 Years married 34.1 15.0 10-59 6.5 4.3 1.5-1.6 Years bereaved aExcludes Widow 6 who was widowed for 25 years. We tape-recorded the semi-structured interviews that lasted from 45 minutes to 1½ hours, conducted in the respondents’ own homes. Respondents gave informed consent. The interview focused on RO coping. Participants were asked about practical changes that they had made since their spouse died and how difficult it had been to make those changes (Attending to Life Changes). They were asked whether they had taken up any new activities and why they had done so (Doing New Things). They were asked about the kinds of things they did to keep their minds off the loss and whether these had helped (Distraction from Grief). Finally, they were asked whether they had any new roles, relationships, or a changed sense of identity (New Roles/Identities/Relationships) and what their impact had been. We did not ask participants about Denial/Avoidance of Grief because we wished to avoid highly sensitive topics, as undergraduate students undertook these interviews. In the event, participants did talk spontaneously about sensitive subjects and about LO coping. When this occurred, participants were not discouraged from discussing these issues, and the interviewers responded sensitively. No participants became unduly distressed during the interviews. Analysis As with Study 1, the interviews using grounded theory and content analysis methods (Bennett & Vidal- Hall, 2000; Charmaz, 1995; Glaser & Strauss, 1967; Smith, 1995). In traditional grounded theory methods of analysis there are no pre-conceived views about what the data will show. Rather, the data is read with a view to identifying new areas for theoretical development. However, we were primarily interested in exploring issues raised by the DPM and, therefore, there were a priori questions that we wanted to address which have been outlined above. Each transcript was first read through in its entirety to gain an impression of the interview. It was then re-read line-by-line and coded. This process was reflexive; as new topics emerged they were looked for in earlier parts of the interview. All interviews were read and coded independently by all three authors. Any discrepancies between coders were discussed and resolved. However, in 324 / BENNETT, GIBBONS AND MACKENZIE-SMITH general, there were high levels of agreement. The focus, initially, of the coding was on RO but, since LO themes emerged spontaneously, these were also coded where they were present. Results Attending to Life Changes The results indicate that one of the most challenging and indeed immediate changes that had to be made concerned finance. Several of the women had not managed their finances before and they had to learn how to do this. For example, Widow 4 closed down all the automatic payments. Her quote illustrates not only the practical challenges but also the psychological challenges of bereavement, that is, the need to establish control over her new life and to reduce unnecessary anxiety: He paid everything on direct debit, and I closed them all down, because it frightened me to think that this money could go out. For Widow 14, the attending to life’s changes had already begun prior to her bereavement with her husband’s illness, but she also illustrates the financial strains that accompanies widowhood for many women: That was quite hard because Ken was always in charge of the bills . . . but Ken was ill I had time to get used to sorting things out. My finances just hit the floor because when he died, everything died with him. In other work, with both men and women, pre-bereavement caring and anticipation has also been found to be useful in preparing people for the practical challenges that bereavement brings (Bennett, 2007). For other women, prior experience also assisted them in attending to life’s changes: There was nothing different in that because I always pay the bills. Thus, from a practical standpoint prior learning and pre-bereavement independence are valuable in attending to life’s changes and contribute to lessening the stresses of bereavement. The ability to carry out these tasks may also have an impact in lessening the psychological burden that acquiring new skills often entails, allowing more psychological resources to be directed where they are most needed. There is the potential, for soon-to-be-widowed people at least (for example, those with terminally ill spouses), to receive pre-widowhood training in the same way that Lund and colleagues proposed post-bereavement (2004). It is interesting that these widows focused particularly on the financial skills that they need to acquire. It is likely that this is a cohort effect. Women born later may have more experience of managing their own finances as they begin to spend more time living alone, rather than going straight from parental to marital home, and also spend more time in the labor force. Nevertheless, whenever a spouse dies LOSS AND RESTORATION IN LATER LIFE / 325 there are likely to be financial matters that need to be resolved and redistributed, along with other practical life changes. Doing New Things In the first study we found that the majority of participants engaged in new things. This was also the case in this study. Some women took up voluntary work or education as in the cases of Women 9 and 4. Widow 9 points out that it was something that she had taken up before her husband died, put on hold while he was dying and then took up again: I started this voluntary work when I retired, and I’d just started it funnily enough, just before he died. But I’d put my life on hold for a year because I knew he was probably going to die in that time. (Widow 9) I feel as if I could cope with the studying now. (Widow 4) For other women, family activities were important new things. In the case of Widow 8 it was the birth of a grandchild that provided the focus. In the course of the interview she describes how she transferred her love from her husband to her granddaughter: It’s having the granddaughter and stuff that has actually saved me . . . it seemed like a replacement thing to love. Finally, there were widows who took up multiple activities. Widow 15 provides a list of the clubs she has joined and Widow 11 recalls how it was a chosen strategy (and we shall return to her shortly): I joined the B* Support group, . . . I’ve joined Fairbrook, . . . a scrabble club. (Widow 15) In the initial period I made a point of never saying no . . . a consciously chosen strategy. (Widow 11) Widow 11 describes the taking up of new activities as a deliberate strategy and it is evidence that some behaviors fit into more than one type of DPM coping behavior, in this case Doing New Things and Distraction from Grief. Distraction from Grief When we asked about Distraction from Grief two clear opinions were held by the widows. Their strength of opinion is interesting for two reasons. First, it was the only one of the stressors where the women discussed it in strategic terms. Second, they reflect on a debate that occurs within the bereavement community, both academic and practice-based. Widows who practice distraction as a means of coping with bereavement hold the first view, exemplified by Widow 11 above. Widow 1 who avoided going to sleep at first and later to bed, because at first she could not face waking up and later not face waking up alone, also illustrates it: 326 / BENNETT, GIBBONS AND MACKENZIE-SMITH I didn’t go to bed. I couldn’t. . . . And at first I didn’t want to go to sleep. Other women talk about staying away from home or avoiding rooms or reminders of their spouses. On the other hand, there were women who firmly believed that distraction was unhelpful and indeed that found solace from the closeness with their dead spouse: I’ve never made any attempt to keep my mind off it cos I don’t think that’s a particularly good thing to do. (Widow 8) I used to go into his room and tidy all his things . . . because I felt really close to him. (Widow 4) New Roles/Identities/Relationships Finally, women discussed their new roles, identities, and relationships. For two of the women there was a clear association between their new role and roles held by their husbands, what might be seen as identification: He was chairman of a World War I association and I took that on. (Widow 9) I felt it was a more positive thing to do to have a kind of living memorial and pass on something that had been characteristic of them . . . so . . . I set up a Latin club. (Widow 13) Many of the women discussed how their characters and outlooks have changed. Widows 2 and 4 typify these changes, respectively: I’m like a bird out of a cage. (Widow 2) I’ve become stronger. (Widow 4) Finally, we asked about intimate relations. Often the widows do not want the burden of looking after a man: I wouldn’t want a relationship, to have to look after a man. (Widow 12) For others, they see both advantages and disadvantages: The independence on the one hand that you wish you had someone to help you with, but independence, on the other hand where you can sit and watch what you like. (Widow 11) Among the younger widows there was more interest in finding another partner: I’m interested but there aren’t any men about are there (laughs), none that fancy me. (Widow 13, age 51) Finally, there were two widows who had been widowed, repartnered, and then widowed again. Widow 10 had been married and widowed twice, and Widow 3 had been widowed once and then had been bereaved of her LAT (living alone together) partner—of whom she said: He was such a soul mate for me. LOSS AND RESTORATION IN LATER LIFE / 327 DISCUSSION We present data from two studies that looked at the DPM in detail. The first found that there was an association between some components of the DPM and psychological adjustment, the second provided qualitative data on the ways in which widows experienced RO coping. In the first study we found that four components of the Dual Process Model differentiated between good and poor adjustment. Two, Intrusion of Grief and New Roles/Identities/Relationships, were associated with good adjustment and two, Denial/Avoidance of Restoration Changes and Distraction from Grief, were associated with poor adjustment. The remainder did not significantly differentiate adjustment. Many experienced some components such as Doing New Things, whereas Grief Work was experienced by relatively few. Turning first to behaviors associated with LO coping, one might not expect Intrusion of Grief to be used more often by those who are adjusting well, especially given the negative valence of the word “intrusion.” However, when we were looking at our existing analysis and codes which fitted with Stroebe and Schut’s description of “intrusion” we identified some of the following: talking to the deceased; projecting and identifying with the deceased; and memorials. These codes appear to be more positive than the word “intrusion” implies, and are perhaps more associated with the continuation of a bond with the deceased. In addition, when reading the interviews, it is clear that these experiences give the bereaved comfort rather than causing distress. Given the association between the frequency of Intrusion of Grief and good adjustment, we would suggest that intrusion be relabeled Continuing Bonds. In contrast, Denial/Avoidance of Restoration Changes is associated with poor adjustment. The codes which reflect this type of coping behavior are: life at an end; no change; and kept-self-to-self. In previous work this last code differentiated, on its own, between good and poor adjustment (Bennett et al., 2005b). People who report these are clinging onto their previous marital state and are maintaining an isolation both from their new status and from the social world, at an emotional level. These two contrasting findings address a hot topic in bereavement research— “to continue or relinquish bonds” (Stroebe & Schut, 2005, p. 477; also Schut, Stroebe, Boelen, & Zijerveld, 2006). In their 2005 paper, Stroebe and Schut reviewed the evidence for and against the maintenance of the bond. It suggests, they argue, that certain types of continuing bond may be helpful while others might be harmful, and similarly it may be the case for relinquishing bonds. Further, some people may need help to relinquish bonds while others might not. The current results appear to differentiate between two types of bond. Those reporting Intrusion of Grief, or as we believe more appropriately, Continuing Bonds adjust well. These participants do not deny their changed situation nor are they avoiding making necessary changes. Rather they are using their bonds 328 / BENNETT, GIBBONS AND MACKENZIE-SMITH with the deceased to make those changes—to seek advice from, and talk things over with, the deceased and considering what the deceased might have done had the situation been reversed. Those who adjust less well report Denial/Avoidance of Restoration Changes. This avoidant situation might reflect what Stroebe and Schut (2005) describe as clinging to a past attachment, where there is no acceptance of the necessity for change. It is possible that the Denial/Avoidance of Restoration Changes and Intrusion of Grief are opposite ends of the same continuum. Further research is needed to explore these issues. We found that most people Do New Things but only those who have New Roles/Identities/Relationships are found to adjust better. In contrast, those who engage in Distraction from Grief are found to adjust less well. When analyzing interviews one of the challenges we face is in deciding how to classify a particular response, especially with respect to these three stressors. The original DPM paper does not specify the characteristics of these stressors. Following our studies we believe we can look for two responses in the data, or from the participant that can enable us to classify a behavior. First, does the response primarily concern feeling or doing? If the response concerns doing then we could classify that response as Doing New Things. If, in addition, it also concerns feeling, then we could classify it as New Roles/Identity/Relationships. This distinction might explain why although many people do new things, it is only those who take on a New Role/Identity/Relationship that have better adjustment. Thus, the psychological benefit comes not from doing new things, but from feeling something new about oneself. Second, what is the motivation for a particular behavior? For example, a new activity might be undertaken simply for the fun of it, in which case we might classify that behavior as Doing New Things, on the other hand a new activity might be undertaken to keep one’s mind of one’s loss, in which case it might be Distraction from Grief. Although we find that Doing New Things is not associated with any psychological benefit, Distraction from Grief is associated with poor adjustment. The value or benefit of a particular RO coping strategy may also change over time. For example, an activity that initially was undertaken as a distraction, may take on new meaning and significance. An excellent illustration comes from an earlier study of widows that recruited from a Widows Club (Bennett & Vidal-Hall, 2000). I found women had joined the club to distract themselves from the loneliness of Sunday afternoons (Distraction from Grief), who then went on holiday with the club (New Activities) and then became organizers of the club and developed “true” friendships (New Roles/Identities/Relationships). For these women, and for the women in the current studies, a single behavior may have multiple functions in relationship to the DPM, which can depend on the time course, on motivation, and on other situational factors. It is, therefore, important not to confine the analysis of the DPM to a narrow time period. This example also illustrates the value of qualitative research in capturing the multi-dimensional nature of LO and RO that might be less easily captured by quantitative methods. LOSS AND RESTORATION IN LATER LIFE / 329 Alongside Doing New Things there are also other behaviors that are undertaken by most people. Almost everyone begins to break bonds. Most people discuss the decision to dispose of their spouses’ possessions and this is a task that must be tackled to some degree. The majority of people discard most things at some point (sometimes early on, sometimes later) although many keep hold of one or two significant things. Thus, people are simultaneously continuing and breaking bonds. It is not a case of one or the other. In addition, the majority of people also discuss times when they experienced Denial/Avoidance of Grief. People often talk of not wanting to be in the house, or of the distress they feel at returning to an empty house, or sleeping in an empty bed. They talk about the numbness and the lack of memories concerning the days immediately after the death. These appear to be common, and are part of the normal pattern of grief. Thus, some periods of avoidance may be necessary in order to survive the pain of bereavement. Finally, although most people have to Attend to Life Changes, it is important to consider what the components of Attending to Life Changes might be. At first glance the focus is on practical tasks and illustrated by our participants. The ease to which some of these tasks were attended was influenced by at least two factors. Those widows who had prior experience, either as a consequence of an independent personality or lifestyle, or those women who as a consequence of prior caregiving, were more able to attend to these changes (and this has been found elsewhere; Bennett, Hughes, & Smith, 2003). In addition, there is some evidence that there is also a psychological component to Attending to Life Changes, and this illustrated by Widow 4 who changed the way her bills were managed to exercise control. It would be interesting to explore the psychological components of this type stressor, since this may have an impact on psychological adjustment. Thus, as with the other RO stressors, it may be important to consider the underlying meaning of a particular activity—does it represent simply a functional change or does it represent a psychological change? Richardson and Balaswamy (2001) found that LO coping was associated more often with negative affect and restoration with positive. Although positive and negative affect cannot be directly mapped onto good and poor adjustment there may be similarities. If that is the case the current results are different from theirs. However, these results do reflect another literature, that of the traditional stress’ literature (Carver et al., 1989; Folkman & Lazarus, 1980). Those behaviors associated with poor adjustment, Denial/Avoidance of Change and Distraction from Grief, may be seen as avoidant coping strategies. On the other hand, those associated with good adjustment, Intrusion of Grief and New Roles/Identities/ Relationships, may be seen as confrontative coping strategies. There were some interesting attitudes toward new relationships. Many men in the first study discussed the question of whether they would remarry and considered the idea positively, whereas the women in that study were unenthusiastic about remarriage or repartnering. However, relatively few men or women had embarked on new relationships, although this may have been because the 330 / BENNETT, GIBBONS AND MACKENZIE-SMITH study was explicitly for people who remained widowed. In that study the participants mean age was 74 and all were 55 years of age and older. In the second study the participants’ mean age was 70 and the youngest was 51. Here there were mixed views from the women with respect to new relationships. There is evidence that the attitude toward new relationships is age dependent: younger widows would like to re-partner; older widows would not. This effect may also be cohort-dependent. Attitudes toward re-partnering and sexual relationships changed greatly during the 20th century, and that may also explain the range of views the widows had about re-partnering. That both people with good and poor adjustment share some coping experiences suggests that the model is doing what it is supposed to be doing, describing and explaining the everyday experiences of bereaved people. At the same time some of those coping experiences/behaviors could be placed in the integrated risk-factor model proposed by Stroebe et al. (2005). The reporting of Denial/Avoidance of Change and Distraction from Grief might be useful in identifying those who are at risk of complicated grief, while those who report Intrusion of Grief or New Roles/Identities/Relationships might be selected out of interventions. ACKNOWLEDGMENTS Thanks are due to the men and women who participated in these studies and to Georgina Hughes and Philip Smith. REFERENCES Bennett, K. M. (1997). A longitudinal study of wellbeing in widowed women. International Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry, 12(1), 61-66. Bennett, K. M. (2007). “No Sissy Stuff”: Towards a theory of masculinity and emotional expression in older widowed men. Journal of Aging Studies, 21, 347-356. Bennett, K. M., & Bennett, G. (2000-2001). “And there’s always this great hole inside that hurts”: An empirical study of bereavement in later life. Omega, 42(3), 237-251. Bennett, K. M., & Vidal-Hall, S. (2000). Narratives of death: A qualitative study of widowhood in women in later life. Ageing and Society, 20(4), 413-428. Bennett, K. M., Hughes, G. M., & Smith, P. T. (2003). “I think a woman can take it”: Widowed men’s views and experiences of gender differences in bereavement. Ageing International, 28(4), 408-424. Bennett, K. M., Hughes, G. M., & Smith, P. T. (2005a). Coping, depressive feelings and gender differences in late life widowhood. Aging and Mental Health, 9(4), 348-353. Bennett, K. M. Hughes, G. M., & Smith, P. T. (2005b). The effects of strategy and gender on coping with widowhood in later life. Omega, 51(1), 33-52. LOSS AND RESTORATION IN LATER LIFE / 331 Benyamini, Y., Blumstein, T., Lusky, A., & Modan, B. (2003). Gender differences in the self-rated health-mortality association: Is it poor self-rated health that predicts mortality or excellent self-rated health that predicts survival? The Gerontologist, 43(3), 396-405. Carver, C. S., Scheier, M. F., & Weintraub, J. K. (1989). Assessing coping strategies: A theoretically based approach. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 56(2), 267-283. Caserta, M. S., & Lund, D. A. (2007). Toward the Development of an Inventory of Daily Widowed Life (IDWL); guided by the Dual Process Model of Coping with Bereavement. Death Studies, 31(6), 505-535. Charmaz, K. (1995). Grounded theory. In J. A. Smith, R. Harré, & L. Van Langenhove (Eds.), Rethinking methods in psychology (pp. 27-49). London: Sage. Davidson, K. (1999). Marital perceptions in retrospect: A study of older widows and widowers. In R. Miller & S. Browning (Eds.), With this ring: Divorce, intimacy and cohabitation from a multicultural perspective (pp. 127-145). Stamford, CT: JAI Press. Folkman, S., & Lazarus, R. S. (1980). An analysis of coping in a middle-aged community sample. Journal of Health and Social Behavior, 21, 219-239. Footman, E. B. (1998). The loss adjusters. Mortality, 3, 291-295. Glaser, B. G., & Strauss, A. L. (1967). The discovery of grounded theory: Strategies for qualitative research. Chicago: Aldine. Lazarus, R. S., & Folkman, S. (1984). Stress, appraisal, and coping. New York: SpringerVerlag. Lund, D., Caserta, M., de Vries, B., & Wright, S. (2004). Restoration after bereavement. Generations Review, 14(4), 9-15. Moore, A. J., & Stratton, D. C. (2003). Resilient widowers: Older men adjusting to a new life. New York: Prometheus Books. Moos, R. H. (1995). Development and applications of new measures of life stressors, social resources, and coping resourced. European Journal of Psychological Assessment, 11, 1-13. Richardson, V. E. (2007). A dual process model of grief counseling: Findings from the Changing Lives of Older Couples (CLOC) study. Journal of Gerontological Social Work, 48(3/4), 311-329. Richardson, V. E., & Balaswamy, S. (2001). Coping with bereavement among elderly widowers. Omega, 43(2), 129-144. Schut, H. A. W., Stroebe, M. S., Boelen, P. A., & Zijerveld, A. M. (2006). Continuing relationships with the deceased: Disentangling bonds and grief. Death Studies, 30, 757-766. Shear, K., Frank, E., Houck, P. R., & Reynolds, C. F. III. (2005). Treatment of complicated grief: A randomized controlled trial. Journal of the American Medical Association, 293(21), 2601-2608. Smith, J. A. (1995). Semi-structured interviewing and qualitative analysis. In J. A. Smith, R. Harré, & L. Van Langenhove (Eds.), Rethinking methods in psychology (pp. 9-26). London: Sage. Stroebe, M. S., & Schut, H. (2005). To continue or relinquish bonds: A review of the consequences for the bereaved. Death Studies, 29(6), 477-494. 332 / BENNETT, GIBBONS AND MACKENZIE-SMITH Stroebe, M., & Schut, H. (1999). The dual process model of coping with bereavement: Rationale and description. Death Studies, 23, 197-224. Stroebe, M. S., Folkman, S., Hansson, R. O., & Schut, H. (2006). The prediction of bereavement outcome: Development of an integrative risk factor framework. Social Science & Medicine, 63(9), 2440-2451. Walter, T. (1996). A new model of grief. Mortality, 1, 7-25. Zisook, S., Paulus, M., Shuchter, S. R., & Judd, L. L. (1997). The many faces of depression following spousal bereavement. Journal of Affective Disorders, 45, 85-95. Direct reprint requests to: Dr. Kate M. Bennett School of Psychology University of Liverpool Eleanor Rathbone Building Bedford Street South Liverpool L69 7ZA UK e-mail: kmb@liv.ac.uk OMEGA, Vol. 61(4) 333-356, 2010 LENGTH OF CAREGIVING AND WELL-BEING AMONG OLDER WIDOWERS: IMPLICATIONS FOR THE DUAL PROCESS MODEL OF BEREAVEMENT VIRGINIA E. RICHARDSON, PH.D. The Ohio State University, Columbus ABSTRACT The intent of this study was to examine if length of caregiving was associated with older widowers’ adjustment to bereavement and to identify factors, based on principles underlying the Dual Process Model of Bereavement, that might mitigate the potential adverse effects of time spent caring. Twohundred men over the age of 60 and in the second year of bereavement were identified from death records of older women who had died within a 12-month period. Interviews lasted about 2 hours and focused on widowers’ experiences surrounding their wives’ deaths along with questions about social support, health, retirement, and other demographic information. The Bradburn Affect Scale was used to measure positive and negative affect. Restoration-oriented coping, such as starting new relationships and activities were measured. These variables included extent of family contact, number of friends, having a confidante, involvement with neighbors, and participation in sports and clubs. Time since death and demographic variables were used as controls. Hierarchical linear regression was conducted on positive and negative affect after which potentially moderating effects were analyzed. Results indicated that the most important influences on negative affect were time since death, ethnicity, and participation in clubs while for positive affect the most significant factors included length of caregiving, number of friends, and having a confidante. Although no interaction effects were significant, patterns emerged. Implications for applying the DPM with older bereaved men are made. 333 Ó 2010, Baywood Publishing Co., Inc. doi: 10.2190/OM.61.4.e http://baywood.com 334 / RICHARDSON Widowhood is the most significant life event for most older persons. Spousal bereavement is associated with poor health, increased mortality rates, reductions in income, and problems in self-care that may require learning new skills, such as meal planning and preparation (Lund, Caserta, de Vries, & Wright, 2004; Lund, Caserta, & Dimond, 1993; Stroebe & Stroebe, 1993). Recent studies of bereavement have used prospective longitudinal research designs to examine bereavement (Carr, 2008). We now know that older widowed persons vary in their response to bereavement depending on the social context, e.g., place of death (at home or at the hospital), circumstances of death (sudden or protracted), and psychological influences, including interpersonal features, such as the quality of the marital relationship. Caregiving is another important factor that affects widowed persons’ post-bereavement adjustment. Most scholars who have studied caregiving have used either the stress-relief or chronic strain models (Schulz, Boerner, & Hebert, 2008). Those who have applied the stress-relief model expect that caregivers will feel relief after care recipients die, especially if the death was protracted and required extensive medical care. Proponents of the chronic strain model maintain that caregiving leads to short- and long-term stress reactions, which sometimes continue for years after bereavement (Kiecolt-Glaser, 1999; Selye, 1993). This is often referred to as the general adaptation syndrome (GAS). An alternative model used in this study (and discussed earlier in this issue), is The Dual Process Model of Bereavement (DPM), which has several advantages over these other models. One advantage of the DPM is that it is based on the premise that bereaved persons oscillate between positive and negative reactions, that is, a dialectic tension underlies grieving between focusing on the loss, i.e., loss-oriented coping, and restoration-oriented coping, such as meeting new challenges or starting new relationships. According to Stroebe and Schut (1999), restoration-oriented coping requires attending to the secondary stressors that result from bereavement, including participating in different roles, mastering new tasks, engaging in instrumental activities, or making decisions about living alone or relocating. Archer (2008, p. 57) describes restoration-coping as “. . . coping with the loss by turning attention away from it and engaging in new tasks and relationships.” Although people differ in how they cope with loss, most people go back and forth (i.e., they oscillate, between loss- and restoration-oriented coping). Instead of assuming that people grieve in orderly sequences as many stage theories of bereavement have suggested, Stroebe and Schut emphasize how variously bereaved persons respond to their loss. Another advantage of the DPM is its potential relevance for practitioners assisting bereaved adults. By assessing widowed persons’ coping styles, clinicians can help bereaved persons balance their coping approaches and identify those at-risk for complicated grief reactions. LENGTH OF CAREGIVING AND WELL-BEING / 335 In this study I apply principles from the DPM to determine whether bereaved caregivers’ engagement in restoration-oriented coping might moderate the potential adverse effects that some older adults experience during bereavement. In previous investigations, researchers have observed that older widowed persons who focused exclusively on their loss without attending to new, previously avoided challenges, struggled more during bereavement than those who laughed, visited friends, or took up hobbies (Keltner & Bonnano, 1997; Lund, Utz, Caserta & de Vries, 2008-2009; Richardson, 2007). Ong, Bergeman, and Bisconti (2004) found that older widowed persons who had fun and entertained themselves became less depressed or anxious. These studies underscore the need for restoration-oriented coping during bereavement and challenge assumptions that healthy grieving only occurs after concentrating intensely on the loss. RESEARCH ON THE EFFECTS OF CAREGIVING POST-BEREAVEMENT Several studies have shown that most caregivers felt at peace after their loved one died, especially if the cause was Alzheimer’s disease (AD). Schulz and colleagues (2001) found that 90% of dementia caregivers felt relieved following their loved ones’ death although about 25% of these caregivers were depressed a year later. Zhang, Mitchell, Bambauer, Jones, and Prigerson (2008) observed that one-half of the caregivers evidenced depression 1.5 years after their care recipients died from Alzheimer’s disease, and many evidenced long-term stress reactions, including elevated blood pressure (Grant, Adler, Patterson, Dimsdale, Ziegler, & Irwin, 2002). Similarly, Kiecolt-Glaser, Dura, Speicher, Trask, and Glaser (1991) found that caregivers’ immune systems remained compromised many years after bereavement and that some never returned to previous levels of functioning. The caregivers who struggle most during bereavement often had experienced high levels of caregiver burden, felt overwhelmed with caring, or were overly invested in the caregiver role (Boerner, Schulz, & Horowitz, 2004; Schulz, Boerner, Shear, Zhang, & Gitlin, 2006; Schulz et al., 2008). Those who care extensively for long periods without respite especially are at risk for complicated grief reactions (Aneshensel, Botticello, & Yamamoto-Mitani, 2004; Richardson, 2007; Richardson & Balaswamy, 2001; Robinson-Whelan, Tada, MacCallum, McGuire, & Kiecolt-Glaser, 2001; Zhang et al., 2008). Research findings vary depending on the outcome and control variables used. Most scholars have focused on negative bereavement outcomes, such as depression and other mental health symptoms. Few have examined positive responses, such as humor, positive affect or stress-related growth reactions, postbereavement (Lund et al., 2008-2009; Znoj, 2006). In this investigation I consider negative and positive outcomes. 336 / RICHARDSON STUDY OBJECTIVES I ask two questions in this study: 1. Is there a significant association between length of time spent caregiving and well-being, operationalized as positive affect (PA) and negative affect (NA), among older widowers during the second year of bereavement, and, if so, does the association hold when time since bereavement is considered? 2. Does engagement in restoration activities—specifically involvement in relationships and pleasurable activities—mitigate potentially adverse postbereavement reactions? METHOD Sample The sample was identified through county obituaries and death records that appeared in a Midwestern daily newspaper of women approximately 60 years of age or older. All obituaries for older women who had died within a 12-month period from the summer of 1992 until the spring of 1993 were considered. County death records were accessed to identify decedents for whom no obituary had been published, or to obtain the address, marital status, and ethnic background of potential respondents. A pool of 357 widowers was initially identified, and 200 of the older widowers were interviewed. Within this pool several men were ineligible for the study. Specifically, 18 (5%) were too ill to participate; 34 (9%) died soon after their wives within the first year of bereavement; 12 (3%) were under age 60; 31 (8.5%) had already relocated outside of the area; and 11 (3%) could not be located. Of the remaining respondents who were eligible to participate in the study 51 (20%) refused to be interviewed. The sample of 200 widowers ranged in age from 58 to 91 years old, and the average age was 75. A description of the sample is presented in Table 1. Almost three-quarters (73%) of these men lived alone in a place at which they had resided, on average, for about 20 years. Most widowers were in good health, averaged about 13 years of education, and brought in between $1400 and $1500 of income each month. Eighty-two percent were Caucasian and 18% were African-American. These bereaved men averaged 520 days since the death of their spouses. About 56% (113) reported that it had been between 13 and 18 months since bereavement and 43% (87) were between 19 and 24 months since bereavement. The interview was conducted at least 1 year after the death of the spouse (between 12-22 months) to allow respondents time to recover from the initial loss and grief (O’Bryant & Hansson, 1995). This also improves response rates (O’Bryant, 1983, 1987). Among studies that contacted widows or widowers before 12 months after the spouse’s death, refusal rates have been as high as 65% LENGTH OF CAREGIVING AND WELL-BEING / 337 Table 1. Description of Sample (N = 200) Characteristics Age Mean (SD) Mdn 75 74 Ethnicity % Caucasian % African-American 82% 18% Living alone % Alone % With others 74% 26% Education # Years Monthly income 13 12 $1400 $1400 Driving status % Yes % No 90% 10% Health 1 = Poor 2 = Fair 3 = Good 4 = Excellent 10% 27% 49% 14% (Lund, 1989). Widowed women, over the age of 60, conducted the interviews because they had participated in an earlier study and were familiar with the interview schedule. Widows from African-American backgrounds interviewed African-American widowers. We also used interviewers with similar characteristics (age, marital status, and ethnic background) to enhance empathy and rapport. In-depth training was administered until all interviewers correctly and consistently asked questions to reduce potential interviewer effects. Those who refused to participate were compared with those who agreed to be interviewed based on available data: days since wife’s death, wife’s age, respondent’s ethnic background, and the median income in their particular census tract. No significant differences emerged between the refusers and participants on these variables. The interviews lasted about 2 hours and focused on many issues including information on social support, self-esteem, health, occupational history, retirement, and other demographic factors. They also included questions about 338 / RICHARDSON experiences surrounding the death of the spouse, psychological well-being, socialization patterns, and relationships with family, friends, and neighbors. The 14-page interview schedule included close-ended questions and several wellestablished scales. Although open-ended contingency questions were included, I focused on the close-ended items in this analysis. Measures Bradburn Affect Scale Affect was measured using the Bradburn Affect Balance Scale (ABS) (Bradburn, 1969). The ABS asks participants to indicate whether or not they have experienced each of 10 feelings in the past week. Negative affect (NA) items included: 1. 2. 3. 4. 5. During the past week did you feel depressed or unhappy (yes or no). Did you feel lonely? That people disliked you? Bored? Restless? Positive affect (PA) items included: 1. During the past week did you feel particularly excited or interested in something? (yes or no). 2. Pleased about having accomplished something? 3. That things were going your way? 4. Proud because someone complimented you on something you had done? These questions originally were coded as 1 = yes and 2 = no, and recoded to 0 = no and 1 = yes. A factor analysis (requested eigenvalues > 1.0) using varimax rotation of these 10 items yielded two major factors, referred to as positive affect (PA) and negative affect (NA), that corresponded to those that previous researchers have identified. These two components, with 2.6 and 1.8 eigenvalues for positive and negative affect, respectively, contributed to 44.5% of the variance (PA explained 26% and NA explained 18%). The alpha coefficient for PA was .65, and for NA it was .67, which was consistent with what other investigators have reported for these scales (Kim & Mueller, 2001; Maitland, Dixon, Hultsch, & Hertzog, 2001; Sikorska, 1999). The Positive Affect (PA) index was comprised of items asking about positive sentiments while the Negative Affect (NA) scale included items asking about negative feelings. High scores reflected high levels of Positive Affect (PA) or Negative Affect (NA), respectively. LENGTH OF CAREGIVING AND WELL-BEING / 339 Independent Variable Length of caregiving was based on participants’ responses to the question: How long did you provide care for your wife? Answers were coded in weeks. While 49.2% of the men reported that they did not spend any time caring for their wives, the other half (49.8%) spent, on average, about 132 weeks or 33 months; the median was 52 weeks. They ranged from no time spent caregiving to 728 weeks or 182 months. The mean for the total sample was 67 weeks. Restoration Variables The restoration variables were organized into relationship and activity measures. The relationship variables included: amount of contact with family, number of friends, extent of interaction with neighbors, and whether or not the participant had a confidante. The activity variables focused on joining clubs, playing sports, and relaxing since bereavement. The family contact variable (family contact), was created based on questions regarding the frequency with which they saw or talked with their siblings in the past year (1 = never, 2 = a few times a year, 3 = every month, 4 = every other week, 5 = daily). Most participants (90%) had children who lived nearby; the average number of children they had was two. A variable reflecting the total frequency of contact with children and siblings was computed into a total family contact scale, which was normally distributed and evidenced minimal kurtosis or skewness. The size of friendship network (#friends) was based on participants’ responses to the question asking them how many individual friends they spent time with in the past week prior to the interview. The confidante now variable was coded as 0 = no and 1 = yes based on respondents’ answers to this question: Is there anyone in particular now that you confide in or talk to about your problems? A neighbor interaction (neighbor interaction) scale, previously developed by Lopata (1973), included 11 questions focusing on various activities, including: 1. 2. 3. 4. 5. 6. 7. 8. 9. 10. 11. casually talking outside with neighbors; borrowing tools from them; dropping by each other’s homes; visiting by invitation each other’s homes; going to club meetings together; going out together for meals or entertainment; providing each other with transportation; helping with the mail or newspaper if either was away; helping with household and yard maintenance; assisting if someone became ill; or helping out in emergencies. 340 / RICHARDSON Respondents used a Likert scale (1= never, 2 = seldom, 3 = occasionally, 4 = frequently) to respond to these questions. Although a factor analysis of these items indicated three underlying constructs, the 11 items were added together to create a total score for amount of interaction with neighbors because the total scale, which had an alpha coefficient .76, evidenced higher construct validity with other measures than the separate factors. In addition, the total item scale was normally distributed and had low scores on kurtosis and skewness. The following variables represented the activity restoration variables: Have you joined any clubs since your wife’s death? Do you take time to relax 15-20 minutes daily? Do you play sports, jog, or participate in other physical activities at least three times weekly? These variables were coded as 0 = no and 1 = yes. The mean and standard deviation scores for each variable are shown in Table 2. A histogram and analysis of skewness and kurtosis were conducted for each variable, and outliers were identified. The variables measuring weeks of Table 2. Descriptive Statistics of Study Variables (N = 200) Variables Weeks of caregiving Days since death Family contact M SD 67 135.8 520 97.9 33.0 16.4 Number of friends 1.82 1.25 Neighborhood interaction 8.61 2.69 Confidante now .69 .464 Join club since wife’s death .140 .256 Play sports often .275 .448 Relaxation .930 .692 Ethnicity .18 .381 Live alone .74 .442 Health 2.67 .839 Economic status 2.87 .802 Months retired 127 71.9 Ethnicity 1.18 .381 Health 2.67 .839 Negative affect 2.10 1.30 Positive affect 2.29 1.21 LENGTH OF CAREGIVING AND WELL-BEING / 341 caregiving and number of friends had two participants who reported numbers that were obvious outliers. In these two instances the outliers were recoded to the highest value of the variable to improve these variables’ normal distribution. No data were missing for these variables. Control Variables The following variables were included as control measures. Time since death was coded as the number of days since bereavement (Days since death). In addition, we controlled for demographic variables, including ethnicity (0 = White and 1 = Black), and whether they lived alone (0 = no and 1 = yes). The participants’ economic status was coded as: 1 = restricted, 2 = rather short, 3 = comfortable, 4 = fairly well-fixed. Health was measured using a self-rating of health with: 1 = poor, 2 = fair, 3 = good, 4 = excellent. Income and years of education, which were highly correlated with other variables, were omitted to avoid problems with multicollinearity. Analysis Plan Hierarchical ordinal least squares (OLS) regression analyses were conducted for negative affect (NA) and positive affect (PA). In step one, days or time since death was entered into the model, and in step two, length of time caregiving was added. The relationship restoration variables—family contact, number of friends, neighbor interaction, and confidante now—were added in step three while the activity restoration variables—join club, engage in sports, and relax since death— were included in step four. Finally, the control variables, specifically, ethnic status, live alone, current health, and economic status were included in the last step. Potentially significant interaction effects were analyzed in two ways. First, variables that were statistically significant and were theoretically important were analyzed. Computing the product of two significant predictor variables with a potential moderator variable created variables representing interaction effects. The interaction effects subsequently were included as an additional step in the multiple regression analyses. This strategy is supported by Aiken and West (1991) who emphasize the importance of analyzing associations between predictor and outcome variables and of determining how they work together, i.e., whether the association is weakened (moderated) or strengthened (amplified) under certain conditions. Second, significant interaction effects were probed to determine the condition(s), i.e., the value(s), in which a potentially moderating variable, was significantly associated with the outcome variable. MODPROB, a software program developed by Hayes and Matthes (2009) that can be downloaded from www.comm.ohio-state.edu/ahayes/macros.htm, was used to examine possible interaction effects. MODPROB is programmed to use the JohnsonNeyman technique, which determines “. . . at what values of q does t equal or 342 / RICHARDSON exceed the critical t so as to produce a p value for t no greater than a.” These values define limits of the regions of significance for a focal predictor and a moderator variable. According to Hayes and Matthes (2009), if no qs are statistically significant then the overall interaction effect is significant across all values of the whole observation. In addition to the J-N technique, the program produces a table that shows the estimates of the effects of a focal variable that is specified for all values on a previously identified moderator variable. The output identifies “regions of significance” and computes the data needed to visually depict the associations among variables. Graphs were created to show how the focal and moderator variables and effects were related. RESULTS Descriptive Characteristics Descriptive statistics of variables used in the subsequent analyses are shown in Table 2. Causes of death are presented in Table 3; most wives died from either cancer (39%) or heart disease (32%) although 7% died from a stroke. Table 4 indicates the number of weeks spent caregiving for each disease; the length of caregiving was highest for those who died from chronic obstructive pulmonary disease, such as emphysema, respiratory failure, and pulmonary embolism, and lowest for those who died from cancer. Association between Length of Caregiving and Well Being (PA and NA) Tables 5 and 6 present the results from the regression analyses for Positive and Negative Affect (PA and NA, respectively). After time since bereavement Table 3. Cause of Death Disease Percent Heart disease 32% Cancer 39% Stroke 7% COPD 2% Liver/Kidney 4% Pneumonia/Influenza/Infections 3% Diabetes 3% Alzheimer’s disease 4% Other 6% LENGTH OF CAREGIVING AND WELL-BEING / 343 Table 4. Length of Caregiving (Weeks) Disease Weeks Heart disease 57 Cancer 49 Stroke 51 COPD 202 Liver/Kidney 84 Pneumonia/Influenza/Infections 114 Diabetes 169 Alzheimer’s disease 143 Other 80 was included as a control in step one, length of caregiving, which was inversely related to PA (Beta = –.169, p < .01), was added in step two. These results indicated that the more time a widower spent caring for his wife, the lower the level of positive feelings post-bereavement. The Beta coefficient (b) decreased to .136 when restoration relationship variables, specifically, family contact, number of friends, neighbor interaction, and confidante now were added in step three. When activity variables—join club, engage in sports, and relax since death—were added in step four the regression coefficient for length of caregiving increased to –.150 and was –.148 when ethnicity, living alone, health, and economic status were included in the last step. Length of caregiving remained statistically significant with PA in all five models. Despite substantial time since death the widowers (during the second year of bereavement) who engaged in many months of caregiving maintained lower levels of positive affect than those who did no caregiving or engaged in caregiving for a shorter duration. In the final model, the friends and confidante variables remained statistically significant. Those who maintained these relationships evidenced higher levels of positive affect than those who were less involved with friends and confidantes. The amount of variance explained, however, was .089. The same steps and variables were repeated using negative affect (NA) instead of positive affect (PA) as the outcome measure, and the results, shown in Table 6, revealed no significant associations with length of caregiving. What was most significant in all models was time since bereavement, and the regression coefficient for this variable increased from –.164 to –213 in models one and five, respectively. The longer the time since bereavement the lower the negative affect. When the activities since bereavement variables, specifically join clubs, 344 / RICHARDSON Table 5. Multiple Regression of Caregiving and Positive Affect (Beta Coefficients) (N = 200) Models Model 1 Model 2 Model 3 Model 4 Model 5 Variables Days since death .055 Caregiving length .065 .055 .051 .068 –.169* –.136* –.150* –.148* .000 –.011 –.045 Family contact #Friends .180*** .190*** .175* Neighbor interaction .100 .120 .129 Confidante now .144* .139* .151* .111 .122 Sports –.059 –.073 Relax –.022 –.006 Join club since death .127 Ethnicity –.033 Live alone .053 Current health .048 Economic status R2 Adjusted R2 .003 .032 .117 .131 .149 –.002 .022 .089 .090 .089 *p £ .05; **p £ .01; ***p £ .001. participate in sports, and relax often, were added in model four the amount of variance explained increased by about 4%, and join clubs emerged as a significant variable. Contrary to expectations, joined clubs was inversely related to NA, that is, those who joined clubs had higher levels of NA than those who did not engage in this activity. Finally, significant ethnic differences emerged: Caucasian widowers evidenced higher negative affect than Black widowers. The adjusted R2 was .124. Interaction effects between time since bereavement and join club and between time and ethnicity were added in the sixth step, but these were not statistically significant. Potential Interaction Effects Positive Affect Analyses The MODPROB program was used to test possible interaction effects. In this program one identifies the outcome variable (PA), a focal variable (length of caregiving), and a potential moderator variable (friends or confidante). The LENGTH OF CAREGIVING AND WELL-BEING / 345 Table 6. Multiple Regression of Caregiving and Negative Affect (Beta Coefficients) (N = 200) Models Model 1 Model 2 Model 3 Model 4 Model 5 –.164* –.171* –.173** Variables Days since death –.183** –.213** –.094 .072 .069 Family contact –.066 –.094 –.041 #Friends –.099 .056 –.064 Neighbor interaction –.105 –.067 –.080 Confidante now –.050 –.055 –.072 Caregiving length .114 Join club since death .212** .194** Sports –.090 –.066 Relax –.079 –.105 Ethnicity –.199** Live alone .061 Current health –.099 Economic status –.078 R2 .027 .040 .079 .134 .182 Adjusted R2 .022 .030 .051 .093 .124 *p £ .05; **p £ .01; ***p £ .001. program yields a regression summary and conditional effects of the focal predictor for values of the moderator program, which automatically categorizes continuous variables into three categories: low (one SD below the mean), moderate (sample mean), and high (one SD above the mean). Unlike other approaches that analyze interaction effects, one is not required to dichotomize the predictors to probe the interactions. Values t and p for coefficients measuring conditional effects for each category of the potential moderator variable are used to test the null hypothesis that the conditional effects are equal to 0. The conditional effects revealed no significant interaction effects with number of friends although a trend was found for the high (b1 = –.0013, t = –1.79, p < .07) and moderate levels of number of friends (b2 = –.0011, t = –1.75, p < .08). These effects are shown graphically in Figure 1. Length of caregiving reduced positive affect among respondents regardless of the size of their friendship network. Both length of caregiving and friends had independent effects although those who had more friends demonstrated less of a reduction in positive feelings than those who had few friends. 346 / RICHARDSON Figure 1. A visual depiction of the interaction between length of caregiving and number of friends with positive affect. A visual depiction of the interaction between length of caregiving and having a confidante is presented in Figure 2. The results of these conditional effects revealed a significant association for those who reported that they did not have a confidante now (b1 = –.0017, t = –1.93, p < .05). Those without a confidante evidenced a steeper decline in PA than those who had a confidante, but regardless of the duration of caregiving, those with a confidante indicated higher levels of PA than those without this relationship. When widowers without a confidante were involved in prolonged caregiving their levels of positive affect declined; having a confidante partially moderated the potential adverse consequences related to lengthy caregiving. LENGTH OF CAREGIVING AND WELL-BEING / 347 Figure 2. A visual depiction of the interaction between length of caregiving and having a confidante now with positive affect. Negative Affect Analyses Although the overall interaction effects for predictors yielded nonsignificant results, patterns emerged. The focal predictor variable, days or time since bereavement, which was statistically significant in the analysis of NA, and the potential moderator variable, ethnicity in which 0 = Caucasian and 1 = Black, also significant, yielded significant conditional effects for the Caucasian widowers 348 / RICHARDSON (b1 = –.0023, t = –2.47, p < .01) and for those who did not join clubs (b1 = –.0021, t = –211, p < .04). These data, shown in Figures 3 and 4, raise questions about the applicability of the assumption that widowhood is the most significant crisis in late life for all older persons. The results indicating that those who joined a club had more negative affect also underscore the importance of considering various relationship types, and perhaps differentiating between the effects of formal and informal relationships on bereavement. Figure 3. A visual depiction of the interaction between time since bereavement and ethnic status with negative affect. LENGTH OF CAREGIVING AND WELL-BEING / 349 Figure 4. A visual depiction of the interaction between time since bereavement and joined club since bereavement with negative affect. DISCUSSION The primary objective of this study was to determine whether there was an association between length of caregiving, as reported by older widowers during their second year of bereavement, and their current well-being, specifically positive and negative affect (PA and NA, respectively). A second objective was to assess if older widowers’ engagement in selected restoration-oriented activities, 350 / RICHARDSON such as socializing, joining new clubs, or playing sports, moderated the potentially adverse effects related to prolonged caregiving on widowers’ well-being. The findings from this investigation corroborated results from previous research that caregiving prior to the death of a spouse adversely influences older persons’ well-being during bereavement (Schulz et al., 2006). In addition, involvement in relationships partially moderated the association between length of caregiving and reduction of positive affect although number of friends and having a confidante enhanced positive affect independent of the duration of caregiving. The results from these analyses also concurred with Boerner and colleagues (2004) observations that those who were most involved in the caregiver role struggled more during bereavement than those who were less engaged in this role. Extensive caring for a spouse frequently precludes social interactions with others, and informal social participation often declines as a spouse’s illness worsens (Utz, Carr, Nesse, & Wortman, 2002). Caregivers who withdraw from others and focus intensely on their caring role increase their risks for poor bereavement outcomes and complicated grief reactions, as mentioned earlier. At the same time, most bereaved caregivers eventually rebound. For example, Utz and colleagues (2002) observed that the bereaved persons who reduced their social participation during the worst part of their spouses’ illness typically resumed their interactions with friends and relatives within 6 months after their spouses died. Although caregivers might resume their social interactions, they still might demonstrate long-term stress-related reactions especially if they were responsible for spouses who died from Alzheimer’s disease and illnesses that require extensive caregiving. The lack of significant findings between length of caregiving and negative affect (NA) most likely was due to when these widowers were interviewed. Most bereaved persons’ negative feelings typically subside by the second year although, as shown in this research, the circumstances surrounding a spouse’s death substantially influence how quickly and successfully older widowed persons adjust to their losses. The outcome variable used in this investigation is another possible explanation for the lack of significant findings with NA. Although NA is comprised of multiple emotions including depression, loneliness, restlessness, and boredom, it excludes many other indices that reflect negative emotions that investigators typically have considered, such as in the Geriatric Depression Scale or the Center for Epidemiologic Studies Depression Scale. NA is a broader measure of negative emotions than depression (Watson, 2005). Previous investigators also have suggested that NA is primarily influenced by adverse events and circumstances surrounding a loss (Richardson & Balaswamy, 2001; Watson, 2005). In addition, Bennett, Smith, and Hughes (2005a) found that gender differences influenced responses. These researchers observed that widows and widowers’ responses differed depending on the instrument used to measure concepts. LENGTH OF CAREGIVING AND WELL-BEING / 351 The significant association between NA and ethnicity was consistent with Carr’s (2004a) findings that African-American bereaved persons reported lower levels of anger and despair compared to Caucasian widows and widowers and with McCallum and Yarry’s (2008) conclusions that African-American caregivers appraise caregiving as less stressful and less negatively than caregivers from other ethnic groups. The substantial differences in levels of negative affect between older Black widowers and older Caucasian widowers underscore the need for more studies of older bereaved persons from various cultures and backgrounds. Time since bereavement was the most salient predictor of NA in this study and corroborated previous findings using data from the Changing Lives of Older Couples (CLOC). In an analysis of these longitudinal data, again using the Bradburn Affect Scale, Richardson (2007) found that over time older widows and widowers’ negative affect decreased while their positive affect increased. The finding that joining a club post-bereavement increased older widowers’ negative affect was surprising. Based on assumptions from DPM, one would have expected the opposite result, but different restoration activities apparently satisfy different needs. Although I was unable to assess individual differences in restoration-oriented coping in this research, I concur with other investigators’ conclusions that practitioners should assess which activities work best for each person. Restoration-oriented coping, apparently, is a broad construct that is manifested in various ways. Several issues need to be explored in greater depth. Most importantly, we lack consensus on how best to operationalize loss- and restoration-oriented coping. Investigators also need to more closely examine how pre-bereavement circumstances, such as caregiving, affect post-bereavement responses. This is especially important given that increasing numbers of older persons are dying from protracted illnesses, many of which require extensive medical care. More older bereaved persons will have been involved in prolonged caring than in previous years. If we know which subgroups are at greatest risk for potentially complicated grief reactions, practitioners can intervene earlier. For example, increased respite support and other resources might counteract the social restrictions that many bereaved caregivers engage in that inevitably reduce their positive feelings. Such supports also might decrease the stress-related reactions that previous researchers have observed among some bereaved caregivers. Although investigators need to replicate these analyses using longitudinal data, we know that positive sentiments and outlooks during bereavement enhance older widowers’ well-being. For example, Lund and colleagues (2008-2009) observed that laughter and humor increased older bereaved persons’ resilience. Their findings concur with what Frederickson (2005) and Watson (2005) have emphasized in their broaden-and-build theory of positive emotions. Frederickson (2005) asserts that positive emotions augment an individual’s personal, physical, and social resources and thereby enhance coping and resilience 352 / RICHARDSON when a person is confronted with a threatening event, such as bereavement (Stein, Folkman, Trabasso, & Richards, 1997). Proponents of the broaden-andbuild theory also argue that positive affectivity—but not negative affectivity— increases social behavior, especially with respect to number of friends, frequency of contact with friends, making new friends, involvement in organizations, and overall socializing. Positive affectivity is both a cause and an effect of social behavior. While interacting with friends and confidantes improves bereaved persons’ well-being their enhanced positive sentiments presumably motivate them to socialize more. I have identified a subgroup of older widowers who are at risk for developing problems during post-bereavement that corroborate previous results that Schulz et al. (2008) have observed. The older bereaved men who were caregivers for prolonged periods demonstrated less positive affect than the older bereaved men who were not caregivers in this investigation. The deprivation of positive feelings as a result of prolonged caregiving presumably places older widowers at greater risk for developing complicated grief reactions than their peers who are not caregivers. Future research is needed to determine whether practitioners can intervene with older widowed persons to prevent or mitigate potentially adverse reactions that can arise from prolonged caregiving. However, the findings observed here and discussed in the other articles suggest that restoration-oriented coping, especially that which involves socializing with friends, might buffer the reduction in positive feelings that extensive caregiving causes even during the second year of bereavement. A significant limitation of this investigation was that the results applied only to widowers although, as previously mentioned, Richardson (2007) found similar findings among widows and widowers using longitudinal data from CLOC. Given the gender differences that Bennett, Hughes, and Smith (2005b) and Carr (2004b) have observed, however, we need more studies that examine within gender effects instead of between groups’ analyses. This research is one of the few that has examined within-group differences among older bereaved men. Another limitation was the cross-sectional research design used in this study that precluded any conclusions about causality although caretakers’ well-being obviously cannot influence length of caregiving. A longitudinal research design following the same bereaved persons over time would shed light on the dynamics between affect and coping style during bereavement and yield more information about which type of restoration or loss activity is most beneficial at various points during bereavement. Finally, my results underscore the importance of PA and restoration-oriented coping during bereavement, although we need more information regarding the function of loss-oriented coping and identifying moderators that have the potential not only to enhance positive affect but also might reduce older bereaved persons’ negative feelings during bereavement. LENGTH OF CAREGIVING AND WELL-BEING / 353 REFERENCES Aiken, L. S., & West, S. G. (1991). Multiple regression: Testing and interpreting interactions. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage. Aneshensel, C. S., Botticello, A. L., & Yamamoto-Mitani, N. (2004). When caregiving ends: The course of depressive symptoms after bereavement. Journal of Health and Social Behavior, 45, 422-440. Archer, J. (2008). Theories of grief: Past, present, and future perspectives. In M. S. Stroebe, R. O. Hansson, H. Schut, & W. Stroebe (Eds.), Handbook of bereavement research and practice: Advances in theory and intervention (pp. 45-67). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association. Bennett, K. M., Smith, P. T., & Hughes, G. M. (2005a). Coping, depressive feelings and gender differences in late life widowhood. Aging & Mental Health, 9, 348-353. Bennett, K. M., Hughes, G. M., & Smith, P. T. (2005b). Psychological response to later life widowhood: Coping and the effects of gender. Omega: Journal of Death and Dying, 51(1), 33-52. Boerner, K., Schulz, R., & Horowitz, A. (2004). Positive aspects of caregiving and adaptation to bereavement. Psychology and Aging, 19, 668-675. Bradburn, N. M. (1969). The structure of psychological well-being. Chicago, IL: Aldine. Bradley, E. H., Prigerson, H., Carlson, M. D. A., Cherlin, E., Johnson-Hurzeler, R., & Kasl, S. V., et al. (2004). Depression among surviving caregivers: Does length of hospice enrollment matter? American Journal of Psychiatry, 161, 2257-2262. Carr, D. (2004a). Black/White differences in psychological adjustment to spousal loss among older adults. Research on Aging, 26, 591-622. Carr, D. (2004b). Gender, preloss marital dependence, and older adults’ adjustment to widowhood. Journal of Marriage and the Family, 66, 220-235. Carr, D. (2008). Factors that influence late-life bereavement: Considering data from the Changing lives of older Couples Study. In M. S. Stroebe, R. O. Hansson, H. Schut, & W. Stroebe (Eds.), Handbook of bereavement research and practice: Advances in theory and intervention (pp. 417-440). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association. Frederickson, B. (2005). Positive emotions. In C. R. Snyder & S. J. Lopez (Eds.), Handbook of positive psychology (pp. 120-134). New York: Oxford University Press. Grant, I., Adler, K. A., Patterson, T. L., Dimsdale, J. E., Ziegler, M. G., & Irwin, M. R. (2002). Health consequences of Alzheimer’s caregiving transitions: Effects of placement and bereavement. Psychosomatic Medicine, 63, 477-486. Hayes, A. F., & Matthes, J. (2009). Computational procedures for probing interactions in OLS and logistic regression: SPSS and SAS implementations. Behavior Research Methods, 41, 924-936. Kiecolt-Glaser, J. K., Dura, J. R., Speicher, C. E., Trask, O. J., & Glaser, R. (1991). Spousal caregivers of dementia victims: Longitudinal changes in immunity and health. Psychosomatic Medicine, 53, 345-362. Kiecolt-Glaser, J. K. (1999). Stress, personal relationships, and immune function: Health implications. Brain, Behavior and Immunity, 13, 61-72. 354 / RICHARDSON Keltner, D., & Bonanno, G. A. (1997). A study of laughter and dissociation: Distinct correlates of laughter and smiling during bereavement. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 73, 687-702. Kim, A. K., & Mueller, D. J. (2001). To balance or not to balance: Confirmatory factor analysis of the Affect Balance Scale. Journal of Happiness Studies, 2, 289-306. Lopata, H. Z. (1973). Widowhood in an American city. Cambridge, MA: Schenkman. Lund, D. (1989). Conclusions about bereavement in later life and implications for interventions and future research. In D. Lund (Ed.), Older bereaved spouses (pp. 3-16). New York: Hemisphere. Lund, D. A., Caserta, M. S., & Dimond, M. F. (1993). The course of spousal bereavement in later life. In M. S. Stroebe, W. Stroebe, & R. O. Hansson (Eds.), Handbook of bereavement: Theory, research, and intervention (pp. 240-254). London: Cambridge University Press. Lund, D., Caserta, M., de Vries, B., & Wright, S. (2004). Restoration after bereavement. Generations Review, 14, 9-15. Lund, D. A., Utz, R., Caserta, M. S., & de Vries, B. (2008-2009). Humor, laughter, and happiness in the daily lives of recently bereaved spouses. Omega: Journal of Death and Dying, 58, 87-105. Maitland, S. B., Dixon, R. A, Hultsch, D. F., & Hertzog, C. (2001). Well-being as a moving target: Measurement and equivalence of the Bradburn Affect Balance Scale. Journal of Gerontology: Psychological Sciences, 56B, P69-P77. McCallum, T. J., & Yarry, B. A. (2008, Spring). Applying stress-related growth concepts to the caregiving process among African Americans. African American Research Perspectives, 12, 54-70. O’Bryant, S. (1983). The subjective value of “home” to older homeowners. Journal of Housing for the Elderly, 1, 29-44. O’Bryant, S. (1987). Precursors of physical, economic, and psychological well-being in widowhood. Final report to the AARP Andrus Foundation. Washington, DC. O’Bryant, S., & Hansson, R. (1995). Widowhood. In R. Bliesner & V. Kilkevitch Bedford (Eds.), Handbook of aging and the family (pp. 440-458). Westport, CT: Greenwood Press. Ong, A. D., Bergeman, C. S., & Bisconti, T. L. (2004). The role of daily positive emotions during conjugal bereavement. Journal of Gerontology: Psychological Sciences, 59B, 168-176. Richardson, V. E., & Balaswamy, S. (2001). Coping with bereavement among elderly widowers. Omega: Journal of Death and Dying, 43, 129-144. Richardson, V. E. (2007). A dual process model of grief counseling: Findings from the Changing Lives of Older Couples (CLOC) study. Journal of Gerontological Social Work, 48, 311-329. Robinson-Whelan, S., Tada, Y., MacCallum, R. C., McGuire, L., & Kiecolt-Glaser, J. K. (2001). Long-term caregiving: What happens when it ends? Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 110, 573-584. Rosenberg, M. (1979). Conceiving the self. New York: Basic Books. Ross, C. E., & Sastry, J. (1999). The sense of personal control: Social structural causes and emotional consequences. In C. S. Aneshenel & J. C. Phelan (Eds.), Handbook of the sociology of mental health (pp. 369-394). New York: Plenum Press. LENGTH OF CAREGIVING AND WELL-BEING / 355 Roth, D. L., Mittelman, M. S., Clay, O. J., Madan, A., & Haley, W. E. (2005). Changes in social support as mediators of the impact of a psychosocial intervention for spouse caregivers of persons with Alzheimer’s disease. Psychology and Aging, 20, 634-644. Schulz, R., Boerner, K., & Hebert, R. S. (2008). Caregiving and bereavement. In M. S. Stroebe, R. O. Hansson, H. Schut, & W. Stroebe (Eds.), Handbook of bereavement research and practice: Advances in theory and intervention (pp. 265-285). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association. Schulz, R., Boerner, K., Shear, K., Zhang, S., & Gitlin, L. N. (2006). Predictors of complicated grief among dementia caregiver: A prospective study of bereavement. American Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry, 14, 650-658. Schulz, R., Mendelsohn, A. B., Haley, W. E., Mahoney, D., Allen, R. S., Zhang, S., et al. (2004). End of life care and the effects of bereavement among family caregivers of persons with dementia. New England Journal of Medicine, 349, 1936-1942. Schulz, R., Beach, S. R., Lind, V., Martire, L., Zdaniuk, B., Hirsch, C., et al. (2001). Involvement in caregiving and adjustment to death of a spouse: Findings from the Caregiver Health Effects Study. Journal of the American Medical Association, 285, 3123-3129. Schulz, R., Newsom, J. T., Fleissner, K., Decamp, A. R., & Nieboer, A. P. (1997). The effects of bereavement after family caregiving. Aging & Mental Health, 1, 269-282. Seyle, H. (1993). History of the stress concept. In L. Goldberger & S. Breznitz (Eds.), Handbook of stress: Theoretical and clinical aspects (pp. 2-17). New York: The Free Press. Shaw, B. A. (2005). Anticipated support from neighbors and physical functioning during later life. Research on Aging, 27, 503-525. Sikorska, E. (1999). Organizational determinants of resident satisfaction with assisted living. The Gerontologist, 39, 450-456. Stein, N. L., Folkman, S., Trabasso, T., & Richards, T. A. (1997). Appraisal and goal processes as predictors of psychological well-being in bereaved caregivers. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 72, 872-884. Stroebe, M., & Schut, H. (1999). The dual process model of coping with bereavement: Rationale and description. Death Studies, 23, 197-224. Stroebe, M., & Stroebe, W. (1993). The mortality of bereavement: A review. In M. S. Stroebe, W. Stroebe, & R. O. Hansson (Eds.), Handbook of bereavement: Theory, research, and intervention (pp. 175-195). London: Cambridge University Press. Utz, R. L., Carr, D., Nesse, R., & Wortman, C. (2002). The effect of widowhood on older adults’ social participation: An evaluation of activity, disengagement, and continuity theories. The Gerontologist, 42, 522-533. Watson, D. (2005). Positive affectivity: The disposition to experience pleasurable emotional states. In C. R. Snyder & S. J. Lopez (Eds.), Handbook of positive psychology (pp. 106-119). New York: Oxford University Press. Zhang, B., Mitchell, S. L., Bambauer, K., Jones, R., & Prigerson, H. G. (2008). Depressive symptom trajectories and associated risks among bereaved Alzheimer disease caregivers. American Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry, 16, 145-155. 356 / RICHARDSON Znoj, H. (2006). Bereavement and posttraumatic growth. In L. G. Calhoun & R. G. Tedeschi (Eds.), Handbook of posttraumatic growth: Research and practice (pp. 176-196). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. Direct reprint requests to: Dr. Virginia Richardson College of Social Work The Ohio State University 1947 College Road Columbus, OH 43210 e-mail: Richardson.2@osu.edu OMEGA, Vol. 61(4) 357-369, 2010 EXPLORING THE ROLE OF EXPERIENTIAL AVOIDANCE FROM THE PERSPECTIVE OF ATTACHMENT THEORY AND THE DUAL PROCESS MODEL* M. KATHERINE SHEAR, M.D. Columbia University School of Social Work, New York ABSTRACT Avoidance can be adaptive and facilitate the healing process of acute grief or it can be maladaptive and hinder this same process. Maladaptive cognitive or behavioral avoidance comprises the central feature of the condition of complicated grief. This article explores the concept of experiential avoidance as it applies to bereavement, including when it is adaptive when it is problematic. Adaptive avoidance is framed using an attachment theory perspective and incorporates insights from the dual process model (DPM). An approach to clinical management of experiential avoidance in the syndrome of complicated grief is included. Avoidance is sometimes an adaptive strategy in coping with adversity and sometimes maladaptive. In the case of bereavement, experiential avoidance usually plays a role in facilitating the healing process. The emotional pain associated with new information that a loved one has died is so severe that people need time interspersed with periods of respite in order to be able to fully acknowledge the unwanted reality. Respite can be achieved using cognitive avoidance, and *This work was supported by a grant from the National Institute of Mental Health: R01 MH70741. 357 Ó 2010, Baywood Publishing Co., Inc. doi: 10.2190/OM.61.4.f http://baywood.com 358 / SHEAR sometimes by also avoiding contact with triggers of emotion. When avoidance is used adaptively, it facilitates processing of the painful information as well as restoration of the capacity for a satisfying ongoing life. As processing and restoration are achieved, the need for avoidance diminishes and the strategy must be relinquished. If it is not, or if avoidance is over-used in the wake of bereavement, the strategy can backfire. Processing difficult information is impeded rather than facilitated and acute grief is prolonged. This article explores the boundary of adaptive and maladaptive use of avoidance during acute grief, considering both behavioral and cognitive strategies. We define acute grief as the abrupt onset of a bereavement response that is usually severe and that has a relatively short course, measured in months rather than years. Acute grief, analogous to acute inflammation, is a painful healing process that usually resolves over time if there are no complications. Healing is associated with restoration of functioning and a permanent residue of integrated grief. The discussion that follows is framed using an attachment theory perspective and incorporates important insights from the dual process model (DPM). The hypothesis is that avoidance facilitates the healing process, providing a needed respite from severe emotional pain. On the other hand, over-use or persistence of avoidance strategies beyond a certain point can hamper mourning and complicate acute grief. Once established, complicated grief is often chronic and unremitting with experiential avoidance at its core. Avoidance is notoriously difficult to bring to light as it can be overlooked by clinicians. Focused assessment and intervention are usually needed to identify and ameliorate the problem. This article concludes with a summary of our approach to clinical management of bereavement-related avoidance. Avoidance is often used as a method of evading external situations that are appraised, accurately or not, as dangerous. Avoidance is adaptive when real danger is present. However, people with anxiety disorders over-estimate the probability that a situation is dangerous and misjudge the likely consequences. For example, people with post-traumatic stress disorder often appraise neutral situations as dangerous because they have become associated with a life threatening event. These people fear that the external danger will strike again. Avoidance is also used as a way of achieving distance from emotions and other internal experiences. This type of avoidance, called “experiential,” was defined by Haves, Wilson, Strosahl, Gifford, and Follette (1996) as “the phenomenon that occurs when a person is unwilling to remain in contact with particular private experiences (e.g., bodily sensations, emotions, thoughts, memories, behavioral predispositions) and takes steps to alter the form or frequency of these events and the contexts that occasion them” (Haves et al., 1996, p. 1154). Bereaved people utilize avoidance to manage perceived internal rather than external threats. Haves and colleagues outline a number of ways that experiential avoidance can be adaptive, yet they contend that experiential avoidance is not advisable during grief: “When an unchangeable loss occurs, the healthy thing to ATTACHMENT, LOSS, AND AVOIDANCE / 359 do is to feel what one feels when losses occur” (Haves et al., 1996, p. 1157). Their belief in the categorical need to experience painful grief, disputed by grief researchers, is a good example of the confusion engendered when the process and goals of healing after bereavement are not spelled out. Bereaved people regularly utilize experiential avoidance in the process of adapting to a painful loss. Bowlby (1980) is clear that experiential avoidance used judiciously is a natural feature of acute grief. In fact, Bowlby goes farther and posits that there is an adaptive form of defensive exclusion (i.e., cognitive avoidance) in the resolution of acute grief. In the discussion that follows we consider adaptive as well as maladaptive use of experiential avoidance during acute grief. We first discuss adaptive avoidance using an attachment theory perspective on bereavement to provide the context for the discussion (Bowlby, 1980). ATTACHMENT, LOSS, AND ADAPTIVE AVOIDANCE There is good evidence that humans are instinctively oriented to seek, form, and maintain close relationships and to respond to separation and loss of these relationships. The biobehavioral attachment system underlying these innate tendencies operates throughout the lifespan. Attachment security contributes to optimal psychological functioning and fosters a sense of wellbeing. Attachment relationships impact daily life in countless ways, including learning, mastery and performance success, overall effective functioning, emotion regulation, psychophysiological reactivity, sleep quality, self-esteem and self-concept, cognitive functioning, coping skills and problem solving, and general interpersonal functioning (e.g., Carmichael & Reis, 2005; Cassidy, 1994; Feeney & Collins, 2003; Gillath, Bunge, Shaver, Wendelken, & Mikulincer, 2005; Kim, Carver, Deci, & Kasser, 2008; Mikulincer, Dolev, & Shaver, 2004; Mikulincer, Florian, Cowan, & Papa, 2002; Mikulincer, Shaver, & Pereg, 2003; Pereg & Mikulincer, 2004). The attachment working model is an internal mental representation thought to entail episodic and semantic memory systems that specifically map each attachment relationship. Attachment working models are believed to be dynamic both in the sense of their day-to-day operations and in the sense of being altered as needed by important changes in the actual relationship to the attachment figure (Collins & Feeney, 2004; Mikulincer & Shaver, 2003). The working model is thought to be the mechanism by which the impact of attachment on psychological functioning operates (Bowlby, 1980; Bretherton, 1999; Collins, 1996; Collins & Feeney, 2004; Gillath, Makulincer, Fitzsimons, Shaver, Schachner, & Bargh, 2006; Grossman, 1999; Meins, 1999; Mikulincer & Shaver, 2003; Roisman, Collins, Sroufe, & Egeland, 2005; Shaver & Mikulincer, 2002; Simpson, Winterheld, Rholes, & Oriña, 2007; Waters & Waters, 2006; Zimmermann, 1999). The working model makes it possible to experience day-to-day physical separation from an attachment figure without distress. However, people resist 360 / SHEAR prolonged separation and react strongly to perceived uncertainty about a loved one’s safety. The permanent loss of an attachment relationship is highly impactful. Bowlby posited that when the death of a loved one occurs, the unwanted and emotionally painful reality of its finality is only gradually accepted and integrated into the attachment working model. He contended that the response to bereavement evolves in quality during the process of integration and that avoidance regularly plays a role in this process (Bowlby, 1980). In the early aftermath of a death most bereaved people naturally employ strategies to avoid the severe pain entailed in fully acknowledging its decisiveness and consequences, nourishing instead a hope, however vague and slender, that reunion is possible. The hope grows primarily from the fact that the attachment working model does not initially register the meaning of the death. In this situation a separation response is instinctively activated and generates an irrational feeling, often unsettling, that the person could reappear. This is associated with yearning and searching for the deceased and preoccupation with thoughts and memories of this person. Inevitably, though, a belief in reunion is on a collision course with reality. Bowlby suggests that this means a newly bereaved person faces a painful dilemma: So long as he does not believe that his loss is irretrievable, a mourner is given hope and feels impelled to action; yet that leads to all the anxiety and pain of frustrated effort. The alternative, that he believes his loss is permanent, may be more realistic; yet at first it is altogether too painful and perhaps terrifying to dwell on for long. (Bowlby, 1980, p. 139) The private experience of the recent death of an attachment figure is extremely painful and it is natural for a person to be unwilling to remain in contact with this experience. Bowlby calls attention to the instinctive use of cognitive and behavioral avoidance strategies that are observed in this situation. He then alerts us to the limitations of these strategies and the need for a permanent adaptive resolution of the bereavement dilemma: It may be merciful, therefore, that a human being is so constructed, that mental processes and ways of behaving that give respite are a part of his nature. Yet such respite can only be limited and the task of resolving the dilemma remains. On how he achieves this turns the outcome of his mourning—either progress toward a recognition of his changed circumstances, a revision of his representational models, and a redefinition of his goals in life, or a state of suspended growth in life in which he is held prisoner by a dilemma he cannot solve. (Bowlby, 1980, p. 139) He provides specific examples of avoidance behavior: [the mourner] may then oscillate between treasuring . . . reminders and throwing them out, between welcoming the opportunity to speak of the dead and dreading such occasions, between seeking out places where they have been together and avoiding them. (Bowlby, 1980, p. 92) ATTACHMENT, LOSS, AND AVOIDANCE / 361 He cites Parkes who listed types of processes of “defensive exclusion,” that are strategies for cognitive avoidance: (a) processes that result in a bereaved person feeling numb and unable to think about what has happened (b) processes that direct attention and activity away from painful thoughts and reminders and towards more neutral or pleasant ones (c) processes that maintain the belief that the loss is not permanent and reunion is still possible (d) processes that result in recognition that loss has in fact occurred combined with the feeling that links with the dead nevertheless persist, manifest often in a continuing sense of the continuing presence of the lost person. (Bowlby, 1980, p. 140; Parkes, 1970) Bowlby posits that the bereavement dilemma is best resolved through gradually integrating the emotionally painful information about the death with positive thoughts and memories of the deceased person. Although not mentioned by Haves, the existence of contradictory elements of the private experience is another example of a situation in which avoidance of one or the other of these elements might be adaptive until the conflict is resolved. In the case of acute grief there are contradictory experiences of belief in the possibility of union and appraisal of the reality of the death. To manage this conflict, people experiencing acute grief make use of cognitive avoidance strategies to alternately dismiss either the belief that reunion is possible (bouts) or painful awareness of the death (moratoria.) Bowlby explains the process as follows: When an affectional bond is broken there is usually a preliminary registering of the relevant information combined with the inability to evaluate it to more than the most cursory extent. . . . Thereafter further evaluation proceeds in bouts plentifully interspersed with moratoria. During a bout certain of the implications already received are considered or reconsidered though others are still avoided; whilst additional information may be sought. . . . During each moratoria by contrast, some or all of the information regarding change already received is likely to be excluded and the old models partially or wholly reinstated. Hence the oscillation of feeling already documented. (Bowlby, 1980, p. 239) According to this view the response to bereavement is an evolving process in which avoidance is a dynamic and adaptive element. To say that avoidance is maladaptive following loss is to oversimplify the process of adjustment. To decide if experiential avoidance is adaptive during bereavement, we must consider when and how it occurs. Ultimately, a bereaved person must relinquish experiential avoidance focused on evading painful thoughts and emotions associated with acknowledging the reality of the death. Bowlby suggests the best way to resolve the dilemma is by merging love and loss. The bereaved person links recognition of the loss with ongoing feelings of love in the form of a continued sense of connectedness. Bowlby (1980) and Parkes (1970) label this sense of 362 / SHEAR connectedness in the face of a death as defensive exclusion, a form of experiential avoidance, and this is considered adaptive. In summary, an attachment theory view of successful outcome of bereavement centers on the need to reconcile the dilemma of conflicting inner experiences of love and loss, and places experiential avoidance at the center of the adaptive process by which these conflicting realities are ultimately combined. There is both a sense of ongoing connection to the deceased and awareness of the painful reality that they are gone. Bowlby suggests that the two sides of the bereavement dilemma are gradually integrated during a process of oscillation between processing and excluding private experiences of each. Most people achieve this. People do not forget loved ones who die, nor do they stop caring about them. Instead they feel a permanent sense of connection and responsibility to the person who died. OTHER CONSIDERATIONS RELATED TO THE PROCESS OF ACUTE GRIEF Two other biobehavioral motivational systems are linked to the attachment system and are affected by the loss. Attachment relationships are dyadic and require a caregiver. In an adult attachment relationship giving and receiving care are shared functions, so that loss of an attachment figure is also loss of a caregiving recipient. Caregiving is also thought to have instinctive roots focused on sensitive and responsive attention to nurturing and protecting others (Feeney & Collins, 2003; Gillath et al., 2005; Kim & Carver, 2007; Mikulincer & Goodman, 2006). Caregiving loss instinctively triggers a sense of failure of the protector role. Anger is triggered if it seems that the death could have been prevented by others. From the standpoint of caregiving, fully acknowledging the death evokes a painful sense of failure, with self-blame or anger. Adaptive use of experiential avoidance pertains to these difficult feelings as well as those associated with attachment. These feelings may also be the reason for over-use of avoidance strategies. Exploration is another instinctive system that is related to attachment. The exploratory system provides instinctive motivation for learning, mastery, and performance (Elliott & Reis, 2003). Both acute stress and threatening separation experiences inhibit the exploratory system. This system could play an important role in effective restoration of a satisfying life, yet the initial effect of bereavement is to shut it down. Inhibition of exploration impedes restoration through reducing enthusiasm for, and confidence in doing new activities and taking on new roles. Reconciling the bereavement dilemma reactivates exploration and fosters restoration activities. In resolving the instinctive attachment dilemma as well as responses related to the caregiving and exploration systems, a bereaved person must find ways to adjust to changes relevant to her or his unique situation. How does this process occur? The DPM (Stroebe & Schut, 1999) provides a framework for thinking ATTACHMENT, LOSS, AND AVOIDANCE / 363 about how coping occurs during the process of adaptation and considers this a different problem from that of processing bereavement-related information. The model posits that there are countless stressors contained in any bereavement episode and that these can be generally grouped as loss-related or restorationrelated. Restoration-focused coping includes attending to life changes, doing new things, taking on new roles and identities, as well as distraction from, denying/or avoiding grief. By contrast, the loss focus includes grief work, intrusion of grief, breaking bonds/ties/ relocation, as well as denial/avoidance of restoration related changes. The DPM is consistent with Bowlby’s premise that acknowledgment of the finality of the loss and its consequences leads the mourner to appropriately revise the working model (loss) and redefine life plans and goals (restoration). However, the DPM places a focus on stresses related to these objectives rather than the processing of new information, and introduces the seminal insight that loss and restoration-related processes proceed in tandem. Bowlby believed that bereaved people must “resolve the loss” before they “move on.” The DPM suggests otherwise. This model posits that people begin to cope with restoration-related stress even as they work to cope with acceptance of the loss. This is an important idea because progress in restoration-related activities can facilitate processing of the loss needed to revise the working model. Coping with restoration-related stressors makes the finality of the loss less frightening. Addressing issues pertaining to ongoing life in the absence of the deceased loved one opens possibilities for satisfaction and pleasure in this new situation. A daunting problem of daily life not only stymies restoration-related coping but also complicates acute grief. The DPM concept of a partnership between loss and restoration is therefore very important. However its authors propose that mourners oscillate between loss and restoration-focused coping. Bowlby’s description of oscillation toward and away from emotional pain is closer to the bereaved person’s lived experience than is oscillation between loss and restoration coping. Avoidance of grief is not necessarily focused on restoration, and restoration-related coping strategies are not necessarily associated with respite from grief. Sometimes restorationrelated coping entails activities that directly foster coping with the loss. Loss and restoration might better be illustrated visually as overlapping Venn diagrams that progress in tandem than as separate activities that are undertaken in an oscillating sequence. What oscillates is the private experience of thoughts and emotions. Oscillation progresses through use of experiential avoidance. A useful strategy for experiential avoidance of emotional pain includes directing attention and activity away from painful thoughts and reminders and toward more neutral or pleasant ones. There is now extensive data on the importance of positive emotion to psychological as well as physical health (Fredrickson, 1998, 2001; Fredrickson & Joiner, 2002; Fredrickson & Levenson, 1998; Fredrickson, Tugade, Waugh, & Larkin, 2003; Tugade & Fredrickson, 2004). Successful mourning is likely to be facilitated when periods of emotional pain 364 / SHEAR oscillate with respite from that pain, and preferably with thoughts or activities associated with positive emotions. There is some empirical support that such oscillation in emotion might occur (Ong, Bergeman, & Bisconti, 2004). Positive emotion may be elicited by fond memories or amusing anecdotes that are lossrather than restoration-related. On the other hand, restoration-related coping processes may be associated with emotionally painful anxiety or self-doubt. The notion of a partnership between loss and restoration coping during acute grief makes sense. The proposed oscillating pattern of attention of the two foci may not be as helpful. AVOIDANCE THAT COMPLICATES ACUTE GRIEF There is good evidence that most bereaved people find a way to adjust to the loss. However, a subgroup of about 10% experiences complicated grief in which avoidance often plays a central role. From the discussion above, we might conclude that experiential avoidance can become a hindrance to adjustment if avoidance strategies are not used judiciously or effectively or if they are not adaptively altered as healing progresses. People with complicated grief (CG) are held prisoner by a dilemma they cannot solve, just as Bowlby (1980) suggests above. Maladaptive use of experiential avoidance is often an important hindrance to the resolution process. Avoidance of private experiences of grief, including thoughts and emotions, through cognitive or behavioral strategies is a core symptom of complicated grief. Adaptive avoidance is dynamic in the sense that it is active and changing and fluid in the sense that it is not stable and fixed but rather responsive to the need for respite in the service of resolution. Maladaptive avoidance is more stable and less sensitive and responsive. Resolution of the bereavement dilemma requires cognitive engagement, including consideration of different ways of appraising new information. Excessive and fixed use of defensive exclusion of this information blocks this process. Processing new unwanted information about the death also requires some respite. A natural avenue for respite is through irrational hope for reunion. Defensive exclusion of the belief in reunion can also be employed excessively and this can lead the bereaved person to be barraged by the reality to an extent that they cannot engage in processing activities. Coming to terms with the death is a process that works best if it is grappled with, set aside, and revisited. Each mourner must find an effective balance between bouts and moratoria. Behavioral strategies are less sensitive and less effective than cognitive strategies for experiential avoidance. This is because cognitive strategies are also private experiences and can be implemented quickly and extensively. Moreover, there is less ambivalence about their use. Behavioral avoidance frequently entails situations that are both aversive and desirable. Perhaps for this reason, ATTACHMENT, LOSS, AND AVOIDANCE / 365 behavioral avoidance is more variable in frequency and extensiveness among people with CG than is cognitive avoidance. In the presence of unremitting pain related to the reality of the death, some people resort to behavioral avoidance as a desperate measure to try to control experiential stimuli. Examples of this kind of avoidance includes visiting the final place of rest, going to the place where death occurred, reading the obituary or reading letters of condolence, looking at photographs, thinking about the person, talking about the person, dealing with personal belongings, spending time in certain rooms of home, eating certain foods, listening to favorite music or watching favorite movies, going to places they went with the person, or going out with others. Many of these activities were a source of pleasure or satisfaction and avoidance not only leads to ineffective processing of the death but also impedes restoration of the capacity for joy and satisfaction in life. Moreover, we found that grief-related avoidance correlates with intrusive thoughts (r = 0.37) suggesting that extreme efforts to avoid reminders of the loss may oscillate with intrusive thoughts and each may fuel the other. Both avoidance and intrusions can interfere with information processing. There is evidence that avoidance behavior is related to poor outcomes of bereavement. For example, Bonanno, Papa, Lalande, Nanping, and Noll (2005) found that avoidance 4 months post loss is related to avoidance at 18 months and to worse physical health at 18 months. In a clinical population of individuals with CG, more avoidance scores correlate with CG severity (r = 0.40) and with impairment from grief (r = .33) (Shear, Monk, Houck, Melhen, Frank, Reynolds, et al., 2007). In another study, experiential avoidance correlated with CG (r = 0.63) (Boelen & Reijntjes, 2008). However, avoidance behavior can be difficult to recognize because people evade thinking and talking about situations that they are avoiding. SUGGESTIONS FOR CLINICAL WORK WITH AVOIDANCE BEHAVIOR IN COMPLICATED GRIEF Complicated grief appears to be a unique syndrome that occurs when a person bereaved of an attachment figure is unsuccessful in resolving the bereavement dilemma and is held prisoner by the continued pain and disruption of a prolonged period of acute grief. Acute grief can become complicated by a range of different pathways. However, all result in an internal response that is painful and a focus of experiential avoidance. We designed a targeted complicated grief treatment (CGT) that addresses experiential avoidance in several ways. Additionally, the treatment accepts the DPM premise that grief progresses most successfully when loss and restoration are addressed contemporaneously. Therefore, each of the 16 sessions of CGT addresses both loss- and restoration-oriented issues. Our objective is to help individuals caught in a seemingly endless cycle of grief to re-imagine their ongoing relationship to their deceased loved one and to re-envision their own lives in the present and future. 366 / SHEAR We address cognitive avoidance using a technique called imaginal revisiting similar to prolonged exposure developed for treatment of posttraumatic stress disorder. Imaginal revisiting is modified in order to foster the processing of the bereaved person’s private grief experience. We followed Bowlby in designing the treatment to encourage a pattern of oscillating confrontation with, and respite from, the loss. The technique entails visualizing the experience of having learned of the loved ones death with eyes closed, telling the story of that period of time out loud and in the present tense, while visualizing the scenes, and responding to the therapist’s periodic queries about emotional intensity (subjective units of distress.) At the end of about 15 minutes, the person opens her eyes and debriefs the visualization/story telling experience. During this period the therapist facilitates identification and processing of emotionally painful complicating thoughts or beliefs. At the end of the debriefing period, the therapist does another visualization exercise in which the person imagines rewinding a tape with the story on it, and putting it away in a safe place. The therapist then asks the bereaved person to think about how she might reward herself for this hard work and makes a concrete plan to do so. The remainder of the session is focused on activities expected to generate positive feelings. Goals work is the primary method for generating positive emotions and for working on restoration. CGT includes a segment of each session that focuses on considering what the bereaved person would want to do with her life if her grief were at a manageable level. The person is encouraged to think of long-term goals that she believes will provide significant gratification. The therapist talks with the person about how she will know that she is working toward her goal, how committed she is to her goal, and what she might expect could stand in her way. The therapist asks if there is anyone else who can help the person achieve her goal and together they make a specific plan for the upcoming week. Situational revisiting exercises are used to address experiential avoidance using behavioral changes. The approach is to identify activities that the patient is not doing because they do not want to be reminded that their loved one is gone and to rate these according to their desirability. Ideally the situational revisiting exercise will entail activities that have been pleasurable or satisfying in the past, have the potential to be so again, and that the person would like to be doing. This procedure helps with processing information about the finality of the death and it also helps restoration of ongoing life by reducing restrictions. There are other elements of the treatment approach that help to engage the person with complicated grief and provide a space in which she can feel safe. The person is provided with a handout that describes a model of complicated grief similar to the one presented in this article. Other work is done to help with integrating loss and love in order to facilitate resolution of the bereavement dilemma. We evaluated this treatment in a pilot study (Shear, Frank, Foa, Cherry, Reynolds, Vander Bilt et al., 2001) and tested it in a prospective randomized controlled trial (Shear, Frank, Houck, & Reynolds, 2005). ATTACHMENT, LOSS, AND AVOIDANCE / 367 CONCLUSION Avoidance is an important strategy used by bereaved people and is focused on private experiences rather than external danger. Experiential avoidance is identified by the DPM as a component of the normal process of coping with loss and restoration and is also described as a natural aspect of the loss of an attachment figure. Experiential avoidance can function as an adaptive strategy in the difficult process of resolving the bereavement dilemma. However, this strategy must be used judiciously and in a fluid dynamic way in order to facilitate healing. When over-used or when used in a rigid unchanging way throughout the acute grief period, avoidance can become an encumbrance to mourning and lead to the development of complicated grief. When this occurs, the bereaved person often needs assistance in decreasing cognitive and behavioral avoidance and facilitating a dual process model of coping. REFERENCES Boelen, P. A., & Reijntjes, A. (2008). Measuring experiential avoidance: Reliability and validity of the Dutch 9-item acceptance and action questionnaire (AAQ). Journal of Psychopathology and Behavioral Assessment, 30, 241-251. Bonanno, G. A., Papa, A., Lalande, K., Nanping, Z., & Noll, J. G. (2005). Grief processing and deliberate grief avoidance: A prospective comparison of bereaved spouses and parents in the United States and the People’s Republic of China. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 73, 86-98. Bowlby, J. (1980). Loss, sadness and depression, New York: Basic Books. Bretherton, I. (1999). Updating the “internal working model” construct: Some reflections. Attachment & Human Development, 1, 343-357. Carmichael, C. L., & Reis, H. T. (2005). Attachment, sleep quality, and depressed affect. Health Psychology, 24, 526-531. Cassidy, J. (1994). Emotion regulation: Influences of attachment relationships. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 59, 228-249. Collins, N. L., & Feeney, B. C. (2004). Working models of attachment shape perceptions of social support: Evidence from experimental and observational studies. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 87, 363-383. Collins, N. L. (1996). Working models of attachment: Implications for explanation, emotion and behavior. Journal of Personality Social Psychology, 71, 810-832. Elliot, A. J., & Reis, H. T. (2003). Attachment and exploration in adulthood. Journal of Personality & Social Psychology, 85, 317-331. Feeney, B. C., & Collins, N. L. (2003). Motivations for caregiving in adult intimate relationships: Influences on caregiving behavior and relationship functioning. Personality & Social Psychology Bulletin, 29, 950-968. Feeney, B. C., & Collins, N. L. (2001). Predictors of caregiving in adult intimate relationships: An attachment theoretical perspective. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 80, 972-994. Fredrickson, B. L. (2001). The role of positive emotions in positive psychology. The broaden-and-build theory of positive emotions. American Psychology, 56, 218-226. 368 / SHEAR Fredrickson, B. L. (1998). What good are positive emotions? Review of General Psychology, 2, 300-319. Fredrickson, B. L., & Joiner, T. 2002. Positive emotions trigger upward spirals toward emotional well-being. Psychological Science, 13, 172-175. Fredrickson, B. L., & Levenson, R. W. (1998). Positive emotions speed recovery from the cardiovascular sequelae of negative emotions. Cognition and Emotion, 12, 191-220. Fredrickson, B. L., Tugade, M. M., Waugh, C. E., & Larkin, G. R. (2003). What good are positive emotions in crises? A prospective study of resilience and emotions following the terrorist attacks on the United States on September 11th, 2001. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 84, 365-376. Gillath, O., Bunge, S. A., Shaver, P. R., Wendelken, C., & Mikulincer, M. (2005). Attachment-style differences in the ability to suppress negative thoughts: Exploring the neural correlates. Neuroimage, 28, 835-847. Gillath, O., Mikulincer, M., Fitzsimons, G, M., Shaver, P. R., Schachner, D. A., & Bargh, J. A. (2006). Automatic activation of attachment-related goals. Personality & Social Psychology Bulletin, 32, 1375-1388. Grossmann, K. E. (1999). Old and new internal working models of attachment: The organization of feelings and language. Attachment & Human Development, 1, 253-269. Haves, S. C., Wilson, K. G., Strosahl, E. V., Gifford, E. V., & Follette, M. (1996). Experiential avoidance and behavioral disorders: A functional dimensional approach to diagnosis and treatment. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 64, 1152-1168. Kim, Y., & Carver, C. S. ( 2007). Frequency and difficulty in caregiving among spouses of individuals with cancer: Effects of adult attachment and gender. Psychoncology, 16, 714-723. Kim, Y., Carver, C. S., Deci, E. L., & Kasser, T. (2008). Adult attachment and psychological well-being in cancer caregivers: The mediational role of spouses’ motives for caregiving. Health Psychology, 27, 144-154. Meins, E. (1999). Sensitivity, security and internal working models: Bridging the transmission gap. Attachment & Human Development, 1, 325-342. Mikulincer, M., Florian, V., Cowan, P. A., & Papa, C. (2002). Attachment security in couple relationships: A systemic model and its implications for family dynamics. Family Process, 41, 405-434. Mikulincer, M., & Goodman, G. (Eds.). (2006). Dynamics of romantic love: Attachment, caregiving, and sex. New York: Guilford Press. Mikulincer, M., Dolev, T., & Shaver, P. R. (2004). Attachment-related strategies during thought suppression: Ironic rebounds and vulnerable self-representations. Journal of Personality & Social Psychology, 87, 940-956. Mikulincer, M., & Shaver, P. (Eds.). (2003). The attachment behavioral system in adulthood: Activation, psychodynamics, and interpersonal processes. San Diego, CA: Elsevier Academic Press. Mikulincer, M., Shaver, P. R., & Pereg, D. (2003). Attachment theory and affect regulation: The dynamics, development, and cognitive consequences of attachment-related strategies. Motivation and Emotion, 27, 77-102. Ong, A. D., Bergeman, C. S., & Bisconti, T. L. (2004). The role of daily positive emotions during conjugal bereavement. Journal of Gerontology: Psychological Sciences, 59B, 158-167. Parkes, C. M. (1970). “Seeking” and “finding” a lost object: evidence from recent studies of the reaction to bereavement. Social Science Medicine, 4, 187-201. ATTACHMENT, LOSS, AND AVOIDANCE / 369 Pereg, D., & Mikulincer, M. (2004). Attachment style and the regulation of negative affect: Exploring individual differences in mood congruency effects on memory and judgment. Personality & Social Psychology Bulletin, 30, 67-80. Roisman, G. I., Collins, W. A., Sroufe, L. A., & Egeland, B. (2005). Predictors of young adults’ representations of and behavior in their current romantic relationship: Prospective tests of the prototype hypothesis. Attachment and Human Development, 7, 105-121. Shaver, P. R., & Mikulincer, M.( 2002). Attachment-related psychodynamics. Attachment & Human Development, 4, 133-161. Shear, M. K., Frank, E., Foa, E., Cherry, C., Reynolds, C. F. III, Vander Bilt, J., et al. (2001). Traumatic grief treatment: A pilot study. American Journal of Psychiatry, 158, 1506-1508. Shear, K., Frank, E., Houck, P. R., & Reynolds, C. F. III. (2005). Treatment of complicated grief: A randomized controlled trial. The Journal of the American Medical Association, 293, 2601-2608. Shear, K., Monk, T., Houck, P., Melhem, N., Frank, E., Reynolds, C., et al. (2007). An attachment-based model of complicated grief including the role of avoidance. European Archives of Psychiatry and Clinical Neuroscience, 257, 453-461. Simpson, J. A., Winterheld, H. A., Rholes, W. S., & Oriña, M. (2007). Working models of attachment and reactions to different forms of caregiving from romantic partners. Journal of Personality & Social Psychology, 9, 466-477. Stroebe, M., & Schut, H. (1999). The dual process model of coping with bereavement: Rationale and description. Death Studies, 23, 197-224. Tugade, M. M., & Fredrickson, B. L. (2004). Resilient individuals use positive emotions to bounce back from negative emotional experiences. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 86, 320-333. Waters, H. S., & Waters, E. (2006). The attachment working models concept: Among other things, we build script-like representations of secure base experiences. Attachment & Human Development, 8, 185-197 Zimmermann, P. (1999). Structure and functions of internal working models of attachment and their role for emotion regulation. Attachment & Human Development, 1, 291-306. Direct reprint requests to: Katherine Shear, M.D. Marion E. Kenworthy Professor of Psychiatry Columbia University School of Social Work 1255 Amsterdam Avenue New York, NY 10027 e-mail: ks2394@columbia.edu OMEGA, Vol. 61(4) 371-380, 2010 NEW PERSPECTIVES ON THE DUAL PROCESS MODEL (DPM): WHAT HAVE WE LEARNED? WHAT QUESTIONS REMAIN? DEBORAH CARR Rutgers University, New Brunswick, New Jersey In 1999, Stroebe and Schut published their seminal article on the Dual Process Model (DPM), a conceptual model which changed the direction of bereavement research. While earlier models of grief focused primarily on psychological adjustment in the wake of a severed emotional attachment, the DPM model places equal emphasis on practical—even mundane—daily life strains that follow from bereavement, such as learning new household management skills and establishing new relationships. In order to cope effectively, bereaved persons must “oscillate” between loss-oriented (LO) coping and restoration-oriented (RO) coping. The former refers to coping processes that focus directly on the stress of the loss itself, including symptoms of grief, loss, and sadness; the latter includes the processes one uses to cope with the secondary stressors that accompany one’s new status as a bereaved spouse. Oscillation is essential for optimal psychological adjustment; bereaved persons must attend to practical as well as emotional matters, and many may turn to RO activities as respite from negative emotions associated with the lost attachment. In the 10 years since Stroebe and Schut’s (1999) publication, many bereavement researchers have conducted empirical evaluations of specific components of the model. At the 2008 Gerontological Society of America’s annual meeting, a multidisciplinary panel of researchers spanning the fields of psychology, psychiatry, social work, and sociology came together to test, refine, and debate the model. The four papers presented in the symposium are published in this special issue. These papers represent a range of research methods, including 371 Ó 2010, Baywood Publishing Co., Inc. doi: 10.2190/OM.61.4.g http://baywood.com 372 / CARR in-depth qualitative interviews (Bennett, Gibbons, & Mackenzie-Smith, 2010), quantitative analysis of survey data (Richardson, 2010), and program evaluation (Lund, Caserta, Utz, & de Vries, 2010), as well as a theoretical essay (Shear, 2010). In this article, I summarize and critique the contributions of each of these works and highlight important yet unresolved questions about the DPM. Bennett and colleagues (2010) use data from two qualitative studies to explore whether the coping tactics adopted by widows and widowers are associated with their psychological adjustment to loss. Their first analysis uses data from in-depth interviews with 90 older bereaved spouses. Study participants had been bereaved anywhere from 3 months to 32 years, with an average of 9 years since loss. The interview obtained rich information about life prior to loss, psychological and social adjustment post-loss, and a retrospective assessment of “what a typical day had been like after the death, what support they had, and how they felt.” Their second analysis involved in-depth interviews with 13 widowed women, and focused on RO coping. Of particular interest was obtaining detailed reports about “practical changes” that were made since the death, and one’s appraisal of how difficult those changes had been. The analysis shows that particular clusters of stressors are associated with positive versus negative adjustment. Specifically, persons who adjust well to loss had experienced stressors related to New Roles, Identities, and Relationships (RO) and Intrusion of Grief (LO), while persons who adjust less well experienced stressors related to Denial/Avoidance of Restoration Changes and Distraction/ Avoidance of Grief. The article also elucidates the specific changes that widowed women made following loss; the key challenge they reported was managing personal finances, yet women also reported positive aspects of their newlyacquired independence, including the pursuit of new activities and relationships, and becoming self-sufficient emotionally and instrumentally. Bennett and colleague’s (2010) work makes important strides in pinpointing specific aspects of RO that facilitate adjustment to loss. However, the analysis is based on retrospective data only, and is thus susceptible to the critiques typically levied against such designs. Retrospective recall bias is a serious concern given that some of the study participants were bereaved more than 2 decades earlier. Moreover, retrospective data collection techniques—especially when focused on a stressful life event such as loss—often elicit overly rosy reports of adjustment. Emerging research on post-traumatic growth suggests that individuals tend to construct narratives that emphasize themes such as personal growth and improvement. It is psychologically and emotionally protective to believe that one has “grown” or learned from a traumatic experience; by contrast, it is distressing to admit that a bad event has no positive consequences (e.g., Aldwin & Levenson 2004). Like Bennett and colleagues (2010), Lund and colleagues (2010) focus primarily on the nature and utility of RO coping. Their research team developed the Living After Loss (LAL) project which evaluates the effectiveness of an NEW PERSPECTIVES ON THE DPM / 373 innovative DPM-based intervention (compared to a “traditional” support group). The DPM-based intervention provided bereaved spouses with lessons on how to cope with both LO and RO challenges, whereas the traditional intervention focused on LO issues only. Each of the concrete lessons taught mapped directly onto a component of the DPM model, such as “grief work” or “attending to life changes.” Further, in an effort to emulate the process of “oscillation,” participants in the DPM-based group were exposed to LO and RO lessons on alternating weeks. The authors expected that persons in the DPM-based intervention would evidence better coping outcomes at the end of the 14-week program. Study participants were recently bereaved spouses ages 50 and older, residing in the San Francisco and Salt Lake City regions. The analyses yielded several findings that have important implications both for gerontological practice and for model refinement. First, they (Lund et al., 2010) found that the DPM group (i.e., both LO and RO treatments) and the LO-only group did not differ significantly in terms of how much they learned about grief-related topics. Likewise, those in the DPM group did not show greater gains in RO coping than those in the comparison group. The authors speculate that discussion of RO issues might have arisen during the “traditional” LO group, or that bereaved persons may naturally consult their friends and family outside of the support group for assistance with RO issues. Perhaps the most interesting finding was that the needs of study participants varied tremendously in the RO group. When subjects were asked to name additional topics they would like to learn more about, multiple non-overlapping suggestions were offered—ranging from cooking to computers. The authors (Lund et al., 2010) conclude that “at least some of the participants might benefit more from an individually targeted and delivered RO coping option.” These findings suggest that while LO coping tactics and symptoms pertaining to emotional aspects to loss may be universalistic, RO coping tactics and the consequences thereof for bereaved people’s adjustment may be individualistic. The latter may depend heavily on one’s education, skill set, availability of social support, degree of preparation for the death, and both gender and cohort-specific socialization. This finding has important implications for policy and practice; while bereavement support groups may be appropriate for treating the nearly universal, short-term emotional aspects of loss, the practical challenges are much more idiosyncratic, and may require social workers or bereavement counselors to have a fairly in-depth knowledge of the bereaved person’s skills, needs, and other sources of support. While the LAL project focused heavily on practical and instrumental aspects of RO, Richardson’s analysis investigated a different aspect of RO coping: social relations and integration. Richardson (2010) explores the extent to which social relations and activities post-loss protect against the strains of pre-loss caregiving. She proposes that individuals who engage in long spells of spousal caregiving prior to loss may disengage from meaningful social roles and relations and thus 374 / CARR evidence poorer adjustment post-loss. Specifically, Richardson uses crosssectional survey data from a sample of 200 older widowers to investigate whether the length of caregiving affects widowers’ levels of positive and negative affect 2 years post-loss, and whether the caregivers’ engagement in RO activities, operationalized as social contact with friends, families, and neighbors and participation in new activities, moderates the adverse psychological consequences of prolonged caregiving. Richardson’s analysis, like the work of Lund and colleagues and Bennett and colleagues, finds some support for the effectiveness of RO coping. Lengthy durations of pre-loss of caregiving are associated with poorer psychological adjustment post-loss, yet this effect is moderated by social support. Having many friends and having a confidante with whom one can share their private thoughts each mitigate against the psychological strains associated with prolonged caregiving. These results are encouraging, yet provide only a partial test of the DPM. Several of the measures of social support, including number of friends and having a confidante, do not distinguish between relationships established prior to the loss versus post-loss. Although both measures focus on social contact occurring in the very recent past, it is not clear whether these relationships have persisted in the long term, or whether the intensity of the relationships has changed since bereavement. A core component of the DPM, by contrast, is a focus on new roles and relations developed post-bereavement. Despite this limitation, Richardson’s (2010) work makes an important contribution by considering a broad range of social relationships—including those with neighbors, friends, family members, and activities including clubs and sports. Whereas Bennett and colleagues, Lund and colleagues, and Richardson focus primarily on RO aspects of coping, Shear (2010) focuses on the complex interplay between RO and LO coping. Her essay sets forth a provocative hypothesis: avoidance, typically conceptualized as a maladaptive coping tactic, may be adaptive—at least in the short term following loss. Her argument draws heavily on attachment theory and DPM theory. Shear notes that the pain of losing a loved one is distressing, and individuals may need to temporarily and sporadically distance themselves from the harmful emotions and cognitions that accompany the loss. Unlike Stroebe and Schut (1999), however, Shear (2010) believes that bereaved persons do not necessarily oscillate between LO and RO coping, rather the two processes are overlapping and one may give rise to the other. For instance, one who needs to “achieve distance from emotions and other internal experiences” may rely on experiential avoidance and turn to new activities and relationships for a reprieve from their grief symptoms. Shear (2010) cautions that this strategy of experiential avoidance can “backfire”; however, if avoidance persists “beyond a certain point [it] can hamper mourning and complicate grief.” Bereaved persons engaging in unhealthy avoidance may stay away from activities or emotions that could provide them solace; for instance, bereaved persons may stop listening to the favorite music of NEW PERSPECTIVES ON THE DPM / 375 their deceased spouse, or may withdraw from married couple friends with whom they used to socialize. Shear recommends that strategies for treating grief recognize the important role of experiential avoidance. She proposes a cognitive strategy called “imaginal revisiting” which requires the bereaved persons to recall the death, talk about the experience, and set goals for the future. This process allows the bereaved person to identify the potentially pleasant or rewarding activities that they are avoiding because they fear reminders of the loss. Taken together, these four articles make important contributions to testing and refining DPM. They highlight the importance of RO coping for positive adjustment to loss; underscore the difficulty in conceptualizing and operationalizing oscillation; and show that avoidance may be adaptive, at least in small doses. They also reveal the importance of heterogeneity: whether, how, and to what end one copes with loss is conditioned by characteristics of the death, the late marriage, and the psychological and social resources of the bereaved spouse. However, these studies also highlight four important issues that require further theoretical development and empirical analysis: identifying the psychosocial factors that affect both one’s coping tactics and the implications of such tactics for psychological adjustment to loss (i.e., social selection); the role of agency and intent in coping with loss; the time course and relative balance of LO and RO coping; and the consideration of multiple outcomes when studying the effectiveness of LO and RO coping. I briefly summarize each of these limitations and suggest strategies for future study. SOCIAL SELECTION PROCESSES An overarching question posed by both the DPM and the articles presented in this issue is: to what extent do LO and RO coping facilitate psychological adjustment to loss? For instance, Bennett et al. (2010) find that persons who report stressors related to Denial/Avoidance of Restoration Chances and Distraction/ Avoidance of Grief report poor psychological adjustment to loss, while Richardson (2010) shows that persons with more friends and a confidante fare better post-loss, even in the aftermath of long caregiving spells. However, the analyses do not consider the social, economic, and psychological resources that may give rise to both stress/coping factors and post-loss distress, and thus may account for an observed correlation between coping and psychological adjustment. The issue of social selection is a critically important concern for researchers studying adjustment to loss; individuals with the fewest economic resources, poorest physical and mental health, and weakest social ties prior to loss may be most likely to face difficulties in managing practical tasks of loss and of adjusting emotionally to the loss. For instance, a poorly educated, economically distressed widow may not have the economic resources to engage in new activities (i.e., RO coping) yet also may be at particular risk of secondary stressors such as 376 / CARR financial strain, that further compromise her psychological well-being. Likewise, a widow with relatively high levels of trait neuroticism may select ineffective LO strategies, such as rumination or denial, which in turn compromise her psychological adjustment to loss. Prospective multiwave studies which obtain information on the psychosocial traits and resources of an individual prior to loss are necessary if researchers hope to distinguish the effects of coping tactics on adjustment. Longitudinal data sets such as the Changing Lives of Older Couples (CLOC), Health and Retirement Study (HRS), and Wisconsin Longitudinal Study (WLS) may be useful resources for bereavement researchers hoping to document both the precursors to and psychological consequences of particular coping strategies and secondary stressors related to loss. AGENCY AND INTENT IN COPING WITH LOSS One of the greatest strengths of the DPM model is that it allows for individuallevel agency and innovation. Unlike classic “stage theories” of grief, which assume that bereaved persons proceed through a series of stages in lock-step fashion, and where deviation from this progression is viewed as problematic for adjustment, the DPM model allows that there are multiple paths to adjustment, and that individuals will oscillate between LO and RO coping based on one’s own needs and demands. However, the model does not adequately address whether individuals are actively and purposively choosing these strategies, or whether they are passively defaulting to strategies due to lack of options. Understanding a bereaved person’s intent and motivation is particularly important when assessing the implications of “avoidance” for adjustment to loss. Shear (2010) proposes a plausible argument: that avoidance can be adaptive, provided it does not persist too long. However, Bennett finds that avoidance is associated with poorer adjustment to loss. These conflicting findings raise questions about the role of intent and agency. Is a bereaved person actively avoiding a thought or activity because it is too painful? Or are they passively avoidant because they lack the resources to engage in a particular activity? Or, are they avoiding a task or emotion because they simply have other more desirable or satisfying options? Importantly, both “push” and “pull” factors may cause a bereaved older adult to engage in new activities, roles, and relationships. For example, a widow may seek out new activities because she cannot bear to be in her home alone (i.e., push factor), or may seek out new activities because she happily embraces the prospects of developing new skills and friendships (i.e., pull factor). The extent to which one plays an active versus passive role in selecting a new activity or relationship, and one’s rationale for why one has chosen a particular path may condition the psychological consequences of that coping practice. Future qualitative studies could probe bereaved spouses for the reasons behind their choices to engage in particular practices; the simple question of “why?” may elicit valuable insights into the role of agency in the bereavement process. NEW PERSPECTIVES ON THE DPM / 377 TIME COURSE AND RELATIVE BALANCE OF LO AND RO AND COPING PROCESSES Each of the four studies presented here points out an important gap in bereavement research: neither the DPM model nor empirical tests thereof have yet established when, how much, and to what end one engages in LO versus RO coping. Of particular interest would be an investigation of whether such processes can begin prior to loss. Among the 2 million deaths in the United States each year, nearly three-quarters are to older adults—most of whom suffered from long-term chronic illnesses that required intensive caregiving (Federal Interagency Forum on Aging-Related Statistics, 2008). As such, soon-to-be bereaved spouses may begin to oscillate between RO and LO coping even prior to the loss. The concept of “anticipatory grief” suggests that individuals often begin to disengage emotionally from and mourn the loss of their loved one even prior to the death, as terminal illness and severe cognitive impairment irrevocably alter the nature of one’s relationship (Rando, 2000). Likewise, Bennett and colleague’s (2010) interviews reveal that some women began to cut back their activities, such as volunteering, when their husbands became ill. Yet others may ramp up their social engagement in preparation for the impending death. For example, research on divorce reveals that unhappily married women who anticipate a future divorce, may return to the labor market even prior to the divorce, so that they will be financially prepared for the transition (Johnson & Skinner, 1986). One might suspect that married persons may begin to learn new skills, such as cooking, financial management, or earning a driver’s license prior to the loss of their spouses in anticipation of the RO stressors they may ultimately encounter. Married caregivers may re-establish old friendships or re-invest in relationships with siblings in anticipation of the social and emotional support they will require upon bereavement. This attention to RO coping prior to loss may also provide respite from the emotional strain associated with watching a loved one die; thus it is plausible that oscillation is adaptive both pre- and post-loss. A further goal of DPM researchers is to ascertain the time points post-loss when RO versus LO coping is particularly valuable, and whether there are optimal time points for invoking one set of strategies more frequently than the other, or for oscillating more or less frequently. Each of the articles presented in this issue implicitly recognizes the importance of time, although none made this concern the focus of their study. For instance, while Bennett and colleague’s (2010) sample included persons who had been bereaved for as many as 32 years, she did not stratify her analysis by duration since loss. Both Richardson (2010) and Lund and colleagues (2010) focused exclusively on newly bereaved person (i.e., 1 to 2 years, and 2 to 6 months post-loss, respectively); this limited focus is well justified, given that psychological symptoms and practical challenges are most acute during the early stages of loss. Still, future studies could explore the extent to which and the effectiveness with which one oscillates between LO and 378 / CARR RO in the near term (i.e., less than 6 months) versus longer term (6 to 12 months) post-loss. This approach would be particularly useful in evaluating Shear’s (2010) claim that avoidance is adaptive in the short term only. Studies that focus explicitly on the time course of symptoms could help to identify the specific time period during bereavement when avoidance becomes maladaptive rather than adaptive. The issue of balance, or how much one focuses on LO versus RO coping, also remains an unresolved question. Bennett and colleagues (2010) found that a full 87% of bereaved persons in their sample reported both LO and RO coping, but they captured the presence of rather than oscillation between the two. Lund and colleagues (2010) attempted to emulate an oscillation process in their DPM intervention, but acknowledge that alternating weeks of LO and RO sessions may not be a realistic approximation of the “real” oscillation process. They note that “oscillation . . . is the least well-developed feature of the DPM but has considerable promise in facilitating more positive adjustment outcomes.” Of particular interest is whether the “optimal” balance varies based on one’s psychosocial characteristics including one’s gender, physical health, economic resources, social support systems, and other stressors in one’s life. Researchers now have the methodological tools to assess both the optimal balance of LO and RO coping, and to document the point in the bereavement process when one is invoked more heavily than the others. As Stroebe and Schut (1999) note in their introductory article, new data collection techniques including daily diary studies or “beeper” studies that ask bereaved persons to indicate what they are doing at every moment of the day may provide rich descriptive information on the coping tactics used, and the extent to which oscillation occurs. Large-scale time-diary studies such as the American Time Use Survey (ATUS) and National Survey of Daily Experiences (NSDE) provide methodological templates for developing diary studies of bereavement. This type of data could be used to predict psychological outcomes measured at a subsequent time point. Moreover, as existing studies of bereavement obtain multiple waves of data across time, data analysts will be able to use sophisticated analytic tools such as latent growth curves. This analytic approach allows researchers to track both the initial levels of RO and LO coping post-loss, changes in these levels over the course of bereavement, and the psychosocial correlates of such trajectories. IMPORTANCE OF MULTIPLE OUTCOMES A further strength of the DPM model is that it was developed to explore a range of behavioral and psychosocial outcomes. Future studies should continue to consider a diverse range of outcomes including physical, emotional, and social well-being; different aspects of adjustment may respond to different aspects of LO and RO coping. For example, Richardson (2010) found that the duration of NEW PERSPECTIVES ON THE DPM / 379 caregiving pre-loss predicts positive affect—but not negative affect—in her sample of 200 recently bereaved widowers. Similarly, positive affect only is associated with having a confidante and number of friends. It is important to study multiple outcomes because the time course of specific symptoms may vary and important consequences may go undetected. Studies that focus on the consequences of LO and RO coping at only one time point, such as 6 months post-loss, may fail to detect individual-level differences in adjustment that occur immediately after loss, as well as those effects that are lagged and manifest only in the longer-term post-loss. For instance, depressive symptoms may be most acute during the first 6 months post-loss, whereas indicators of social adjustment, such as interest in dating or forming new relationships, may emerge only in the longer-term, given cultural norms prohibiting relationship formation “too soon” after loss. Further, researchers should continue to consider the level of one’s psychological symptoms rather than solely one’s diagnostic category, such as a diagnosis of complicated grief. Studies that focus solely on discrete categories cannot provide information on the coping processes of those who barely fail to meet diagnostic criteria, or who experience severe symptoms at one point in time post-loss but whose symptoms have subsided by the time of data collection. For example, Lund and colleagues (2010) focus on a broad range of participants in the LAL, rather than only those who are coping poorly. This strategy enables them to focus on a diverse range of coping strategies and outcomes. In sum, influential theoretical and empirical advances have been made in the past decade as researchers have tested and refined the Dual Process Model. This model has influenced the development of innovative interventions to treat the bereaved (Lund et al., 2010; Shear, 2010) and has promise to inform practice and theory even more powerfully in the next decade, facilitated in part by the promising research presented in this issue. REFERENCES Aldwin, C., & Levenson, R. (2004). Post-traumatic growth: A developmental perspective. Psychological Inquiry, 15, 19-21. Bennett, K. M., Gibbons, K., & Mackenzie-Smith, S. (2010). Loss and restoration in late life: An examination of Dual Process Model of Coping with Bereavement. Omega, 61(4), 317-334. Federal Interagency Forum on Aging-Related Statistics. (2008). Older Americans 2008: Key indicators of well-being. Federal Interagency Forum on Aging-Related Statistics, Washington, DC: U.S. Government Printing Office. Johnson, W. H., & Skinner, J. (1986). Labor supply and marital separation. American Economic Review, 76, 455-469. Lund, D., Caserta, M., Utz, R., & de Vries, B. (2010). Experiences and early coping of bereaved spouses/partners in an intervention based on the Dual Process Model (DPM). Omega, 61(4), 293-315. 380 / CARR Rando, T. A. (Ed.). (2000). Clinical dimensions of anticipatory mourning: Theory and practice in working with the dying, their loved ones, and their caregivers. Champaign, IL: Research Press. Richardson, V. E. (2010). Length of caregiving and well-being among older widowers: Implications for the Dual Process Model of Bereavement. Omega, 61(4), 335-358. Shear, M. K. (2010). Exploring the role of experiential avoidance from the Perspective of Attachment Theory and the Dual Process Model. Omega, 61(4), 359-371. Stroebe, M. S., & Schut, H. (1999). The Dual Process Model of Coping with Bereavement: Rationale and description. Death Studies, 23, 197-224. Direct reprint requests to: Deborah Carr Department of Sociology and Institute for Health, Health Care Policy and Aging Research Rutgers University 54 Joyce Kilmer Avenue Piscataway, NJ 08854 e-mail: carrds@rci.rutgers.edu OMEGA, Vol. 61(4) 381-382, 2010 OMEGA: JOURNAL OF DEATH AND DYING Index—Contents of Volume 61, 2010 ABAKOUMKIN, GEORGIOS: See Stroebe, Wolfgang, jt. author BELL, JO: The Role of Perfectionism in Student Suicide: Three Case Studies from the UK, No. 3, p. 251 BENNETT, KATE M.: Loss and Restoration in Later Life: An Examination of Dual Process Model of Coping with Bereavement, No. 4, p. 315 BURKE, LAURIE A.: African American Homicide Bereavement: Aspects of Social Support that Predict Complicated Grief, PTSD, and Depression, No. 1, p. 1 CARR, DEBORAH: New Perspectives on the Dual Process Model (DPM): What Have We Learned? What Questions Remain? No. 4, p. 371 CASERTA, MICHAEL: Sampling, Recruitment, and Retention in a Bereavement Intervention Study: Experiences from the Living After Loss Project, No. 3, p. 181 CASERTA, MICHAEL: See Lund, Dale, jt. author CLUTE, MARY ANN: Bereavement Interventions for Adults with Intellectual Disabilities: What Works? No. 2, p. 163 COOLEY, ERIC: Reactions to Loss Scale: Assessing Grief in College Students, No. 1, p. 25 DE VRIES, BRIAN: See Caserta, Michael, jt. author DE VRIES, BRIAN: See Lund, Dale, jt. author GIBBONS, KERRY: See Bennett, Kate M., jt. author HAMID, TENGKU AIZAN: See Momtaz, Yadollah Abolfathi, jt. author HIJMANS, ELLEN: See Yang, William, jt. author HOLLAND, JASON M.: An Examination of Stage Theory of Grief among Individuals Bereaved by Natural and Violent Causes: A Meaning-Oriented Contribution, No. 2, p. 103 HOUSEMAN, CLARE A.: See Lundgren, Burden, S., jt. author IBRAHIM, RAHIMAH: See Momtaz, Yadollah Abolfathi, jt. author JOHNSON, CELESTE M.: African-American Teen Girls Grieve the Loss of Friends to Homicide: Meaning-Making and Resilience, No. 2, p. 121 381 Ó 2010, Baywood Publishing Co., Inc. doi: 10.2190/OM.61.4.h http://baywood.com 382 / INDEX—CONTENTS OF VOLUME 61, 2010 LIVINGSTON, KATHY: Opportunities for Mourning When Grief is Disenfranchised: Descendants of Nazi Perpetrators in Dialogue with Holocaust Survivors, No. 3, p. 205 LUND, DALE: Experiences and Early Coping of Bereaved Spouses/Partners in an Intervention Based on the Dual Process Model (DPM), No. 4, p. 291 LUND, DALE: See Caserta, Michael, jt. author LUNDGREN, BURDEN S.: Banishing Death: The Disappearance of the Appreciation of Mortality, No. 3, p. 223 MACKENZIE-SMITH, SUZANNA: See Bennett, Kate M., jt. author MALLON, SHARON: See Bell, Jo, jt. author MANTHORPE, JILL: See Bell, Jo, jt. author MCDEVITT-MURPHY, MEGHAN E.: See Burke, Laurie A., jt. author MOMTAZ, YADOLLAH ABOLFATHI: Mediating Effects of Social and Personal Religiosity on the Psychological Well Being of Widowed Elderly People, No. 2, p. 145 NEIMEYER, ROBERT A.: See Burke, Laurie A., jt. author NEIMEYER, ROBERT A.: See Holland, Jason M., jt. author RICHARDSON, VIRGINIA E.: Length of Caregiving and Well-Being among Older Widowers: Implications for the Dual Process Model of Bereavement, No. 4, p. 333 RICHARDSON, VIRGINIA E.: The Dual Process Model of Coping with Bereavement: A Decade Later, No. 4, p. 269 ROSCOE, LAUREN: See Cooley, Eric, jt. author SCHUT, HENK: See Stroebe, Margaret, jt. author SHEAR, M. KATHERINE: Exploring the Role of Experiential Avoidance from the Perspective of Attachment Theory and the Dual Process Model, No. 4, p. 357 SILVERMAN, SAM: The Death of Socrates: A Holistic Re-examination, No. 1, p. 71 STANLEY, NICKY: See Bell, Jo, jt. author STAPS, TON: See Yang, William, jt. author STROEBE, MARGARET: See Stroebe, Wolfgang, jt. author STROEBE, MARGARET: The Dual Process Model of Coping with Bereavement: A Decade On, No. 4, p. 273 STROEBE, WOLFGANG: Beyond Depression: Yearning for the Loss of a Loved One, No. 2, p. 85 TORAY, TAMINA: See Cooley, Eric, jt. author UTZ, REBECCA: See Caserta, Michael, jt. author UTZ, REBECCA: See Lund, Dale, jt. author YAHAYA, NURIZAN: See Momtaz, Yadollah Abolfathi, jt. author YANG, WILLIAM: Existential Crisis and the Awareness of Dying: The Role of Meaning and Spirituality, No. 1, p. 53 The goal of Suicide and Homicide-Suicide Among Police is to fully explore what the author refers to as “the near epidemic levels of suicide and homicide-suicide” among law enforcement officers, and ultimately to offer some recommendations and best practices with which to better address the problem. The book begins by discussing suicide in some depth, for one has to know suicide, unequivocally, to understand a suicidal or homicidal-suicidal officer. Briefly defined, suicide is the human act of self-inflicted, selfintentioned annihilation, precipitated by a multidimensional malaise—a general feeling of being unwell. Suicide is not a disease. Suicide is not a sin. Suicide is not a crime. Suicide and homicide-suicide are complex, multidetermined events—the result of an interplay of individual, relational, social, cultural, and environmental factors. This complexity of causation necessitates a parallel complexity of knowledge. There are at least two avenues to understanding: the nomothetic (general) approach, which deals with generalizations using empirical, statistical, and demographic methods or techniques; and the idiographic (specific) approach, which typically involves the intense study of individuals. This book explores both. Attempting to be mindful of the needs of the officer on the street, the mental health provider, the administrator, the forensic specialist, and the survivors of these needless tragedies, the belief is that by amalgamating the concerns of a diverse audience, we can meet the challenge of identifying at-risk individuals and situations and saving lives among police, their families, their fellow officers, and the community. Rather than a cognitively constricted approach, there needs to be continuing development of a multidimensional (or ecological) approach to understanding suicide and homicidesuicide, and suicide-prevention policies and procedures need to be in place to facilitate effective prediction and control. Format Information 6" × 9", Cloth, 222 Pages ISBN 978-0-89503-390-1 $48.95 + postage & handling* *please inquire for current rates Baywood Publishing Company, Inc. 26 Austin Ave., PO Box 337, Amityville, NY 11701 phone 631.691.1270 • fax 631.691.1770 • toll-free orderline 800.638.7819 email baywood@baywood.com • website http://baywood.com Neil Thompson, Ph.D. Death, Value and Meaning Series Series Editor: Dale A. Lund T he workplace is not immune to the problems, pressures, and challenges presented by experiences of loss and trauma and the grief reactions they produce. This clearly written, well-crafted book offers important insights and understanding to help us appreciate the difficulties involved and prepare ourselves for dealing with such demanding situations when they arise. People’s experiences of loss and trauma are, of course, not left at the factory gate or the office door. Nor are loss and traumatic events absent from the workplace itself. Loss, grief, and trauma are very much a part of life—and that includes working life. Executives, managers, human resource professionals, and employee assistance staff need to have at least a basic understanding of how loss, grief, and trauma affect people in the workplace. This book provides that foundation of understanding and offers guidance on how to find out more about these vitally important workplace issues. The text provides a valuable blend of theory and practice that will be of interest to both students and professionals involved in management, human resources, and organizational studies as well as those interested in the social scientific study of loss, grief, and trauma—and, of course, to those involved in the helping professions, i.e., social work, counseling and psychotherapy, health care, ministry, chaplaincy, and pastoral studies. It is essential reading for anyone concerned with making the workplace a more humane and effective environment, or anyone wishing to develop an understanding of the complexities of loss, grief, and trauma in our lives. Format Information 6" × 9", 138 Pages, Cloth, ISBN 978-0-89503-342-0 $43.95, plus $7.00 p&h in U.S. (please inquire for postage outside of U.S.) Baywood Publishing Company, Inc. 26 Austin Ave., PO Box 337, Amityville, NY 11701 phone 631.691.1270 • fax 631.691.1770 • toll-free orderline 800.638.7819 e-mail baywood@baywood.com • website http://baywood.com Complicated Grieving and Bereavement DEATH, VALUE AND MEANING SERIES • SERIES EDITOR: JOHN D. MORGAN UNDERSTANDING AND TREATING PEOPLE EXPERIENCING LOSS Editors: Gerry R. Cox, Robert A. Bendiksen and Robert G. Stevenson In praise . . . While one derives significance through the experience of loss, another may encounter bereavement with less consequence. Complicated Grieving and Bereavement: Understanding and Treating People Experiencing Loss examines complicated grief in special populations, including the mentally ill, POW-MIA survivors, the differentiallyabled, suicide survivors, bereaved children, death at birth, death in schools, and palliative-care death. Through humor, music, puppeteering, drama, family systems, spiritual care and support groups, the book presents practical suggestions to those managing grief in the face of traumatic death. “I received this book to review soon after the events of September 11. It was an awkward experience to review a book about loss when the number of lives lost in the terrorist attacks that day was so high. I wondered how any book could help individuals and communities grieve and cope with such loss. However, I was impressed by the content of this book and its emphasis on what are probably the very ideas that are essential in helping people cope. The book is well organized and highly readable. The authors of Complicated Grieving and Bereavement identify important issues involved in bereavement, but, more importantly, they emphasize the use of the many strengths our patients can draw on. They address a variety of of practical issues, many of which seem especially applicable in the aftermath of the recent terrorist attacks. This book is a valuable addition to the resources of any clinician who will deal with loss and bereavement.” —Jeffrey L . Geller, M.D., M.P.H. Editor, Psychiatric Services, May 2002, Volume 53, Number 5 “While the book covers a wide scope, it’s strengths lie in an exploration of grief in specific populations, rather than in a systematic review of complicated grief. Of particular interest is the inclusion of chapters on spiritual abuse and dementia, which alerts practitioners and researchers to issues not commonly dealt with.” —Jane Powell Department of Forensic Medicine Counselling Unit, Sydney, Australia Grief Matters 9(2), Winter 2006 Format Information: 6" × 9", 334 pages, Cloth, ISBN 0-89503-213-9 $70.95 + 7.00 p/h in U.S. (please inquire for rates outside of U.S.) BAYWOOD PUBLISHING COMPANY, INC. 26 Austin Ave., PO Box 337 • Amityville, NY 11701 phone (631) 691-1270 • fax (631) 691-1770 • toll-free orderline (800) 638-7819 e-mail baywood@baywood.com • website http://baywood.com Death, Value and Meaning Series John D. Morgan, Series Editor ETERNITY AND ME The Everlasting Things in Life and Death Allan Kellehear The 40 short reflections in this book address the ways in which we face the prospect of death and loss. The first 20 reflections are designed to be read by (or to) anyone living with a life-threatening illness; the other 20 are reflections on living with grief, especially bereavement. Each reflection is based on a single story drawn from one of three sources: Dr. Kellehear’s professional experience with individuals living with dying or loss; his own experiences and stories from childhood; and the retelling of some of the great myths and legends about life, love, and death, selected from around the world—from Ireland to Japan, from Melanesia to China. The book is written to be accessible to a wide general audience. It can be read from beginning to end like a conventional book; each self-contained piece is also suitable for reading on a bus, train, or plane journey, or before bed at night. Each piece can be selected as a stand-alone meditation for use as a discussion topic in pastoral care, counseling, or sermons. These reflections are stories about how we can make the most of life in the shadow of death and loss. They are designed to instill hope and meaning in the difficult times that can accompany human mortality. 6" × 9", 194 Pages, Paper, ISBN 0-89503-298-8 $37.95 + 7.00 p/h in U.S. (please inquire for rates outside of U.S.) BAYWOOD PUBLISHING COMPANY, INC. 26 Austin Ave., PO Box 337, Amityville, NY 11701 phone 631-691-1270 • fax 631-691-1770 • toll-free orderline 800-638-7819 email baywood@baywood.com • website http://baywood.com
© Copyright 2024